Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Goal Derailment and Goal Persistence in Response to Honor Threats 荣誉威胁下的目标脱轨与目标坚持
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-25 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221137749
Ceren Günsoy, S. Cross, Vanessa A. Castillo, Ayse K. Uskul, S. Wasti, P. Salter, Pelin Gul, A. Carter‐Sowell, Afşar Yegin, Betül Altunsu, Jaren D. Crist, M. J. Perez
{"title":"Goal Derailment and Goal Persistence in Response to Honor Threats","authors":"Ceren Günsoy, S. Cross, Vanessa A. Castillo, Ayse K. Uskul, S. Wasti, P. Salter, Pelin Gul, A. Carter‐Sowell, Afşar Yegin, Betül Altunsu, Jaren D. Crist, M. J. Perez","doi":"10.1177/00220221221137749","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221137749","url":null,"abstract":"In honor cultures, maintaining a positive moral reputation (e.g., being known as an honest person) is highly important, whereas in dignity cultures, self-respect (e.g., competence and success) is strongly emphasized. Depending on their cultural background, people respond differently to threats to these two dimensions of honor. In two studies, we examined the effects of morality-focused and competence-focused threats on people’s goal pursuit in two honor cultures (Turkey, Southern United States, and Latinx) and in a dignity culture (Northern United States). In Study 1, Turkish participants were more likely to reject a highly qualified person as a partner in a future task if that person threatened their morality (vs. no-threat), even though this meant letting go of the goal of winning an award. Participants from the U.S. honor and dignity groups, however, were equally likely to choose the people who gave them threatening and neutral feedback. In Study 2, Turkish and U.S. honor participants were more likely to persist in a subsequent goal after receiving a morality threat (vs. no-threat), whereas U.S. dignity participants were more likely to persist in a subsequent goal after receiving a competence threat (vs. no-threat). These results show that people’s responses to honor threats are influenced by the dominant values of their culture and by the tools that are available to them to potentially restore their reputation (e.g., punishing the offender vs. working hard on a different task). This research can have implications for multicultural contexts in which people can have conflicting goals such as diverse work environments.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"365 - 384"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47163779","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beliefs About Ideal Parents: A Discussion 关于理想父母的信念:一个讨论
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-24 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221138908
D. Best
{"title":"Beliefs About Ideal Parents: A Discussion","authors":"D. Best","doi":"10.1177/00220221221138908","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221138908","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"3 - 3"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49474634","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Must We Abandon Context and Meaning to Avoid Bias in Cultural Parenting Research? Commentary on “Parenting Culture(s): Ideal-Parent Beliefs Across 37 Countries” 在文化教养研究中,我们必须放弃语境和意义以避免偏见吗?对《教养文化:37个国家的理想父母信念》的评论
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-22 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221138907
Gisela Trommsdorff
{"title":"Must We Abandon Context and Meaning to Avoid Bias in Cultural Parenting Research? Commentary on “Parenting Culture(s): Ideal-Parent Beliefs Across 37 Countries”","authors":"Gisela Trommsdorff","doi":"10.1177/00220221221138907","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221138907","url":null,"abstract":"The authors of the current article advocate a culture-sensitive approach in research and interven-tions, including a culture-informed methodology. They examine how “ideal-parent” beliefs in different countries comprise “parenting cultures,” conceptualized as shared “ideal-parent” beliefs. The authors define “ideal-parent” beliefs as “a higher level construct—the meaning system and lens through which parents perceive, understand","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"36 - 42"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46682819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complexifying Individualism Versus Collectivism and West Versus East: Exploring Global Diversity in Perspectives on Self and Other in the Gallup World Poll 复杂的个人主义与集体主义,西方与东方:在盖洛普世界民意调查中探索自我和他者视角的全球多样性
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-18 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221130978
T. Lomas, Pablo Diego-Rosell, K. Shiba, Priscilla Standridge, Matthew T. Lee, B. Case, A. Lai, T. VanderWeele
{"title":"Complexifying Individualism Versus Collectivism and West Versus East: Exploring Global Diversity in Perspectives on Self and Other in the Gallup World Poll","authors":"T. Lomas, Pablo Diego-Rosell, K. Shiba, Priscilla Standridge, Matthew T. Lee, B. Case, A. Lai, T. VanderWeele","doi":"10.1177/00220221221130978","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221130978","url":null,"abstract":"A wealth of research has suggested the West tends toward individualism and the East toward collectivism. We explored this topic on an unprecedented scale through two new items in the 2020 Gallup World Poll, involving 121,207 participants in 116 countries. The first tapped into orientations toward self-care versus other-care (“Do you think people should focus more on taking care of themselves or on taking care of others?”). The second enquired into self-orientation versus other-orientation (“Which of the following is closest to your main purpose in life? Being good at what you do in your daily life, Caring for family and close friends, or Helping other people who need help?”). We anticipated that self-care and self-orientation would index individualism (hence be higher in the West), while other-care and other-orientation would index collectivism (hence be higher in the East). However, contrary to expectation, there was greater self-care in the East (45.82%) than in the West (41.58%). As predicted though, there was greater self-orientation in the West (30.20%) than in the East (23.08.%). Greater self-care in the East invites one of two interpretations. Either these items: (a) index individualism and collectivism as anticipated, so in some ways the East is more individualistic and the West less individualistic than assumed; or (b) do not index individualism and collectivism as anticipated, so the concepts are more complex than often realized (e.g., collectivism may involve prioritizing self-care over other-care). Either way, the findings help complexify these concepts, challenging common cross-cultural generalizations in this area.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"61 - 89"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46004471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Parenting Culture(s): Ideal-Parent Beliefs Across 37 Countries 育儿文化:37个国家的理想父母信仰
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-15 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221123043
Gao-Xian Lin, Moïra Mikolajczak, H. Keller, Ege Akgun, G. Arıkan, K. Aunola, E. Barham, E. Besson, M. A. Blanchard, E. Boujut, M. Brianda, A. Brytek-Matera, Filipa César, Bin-Bin Chen, G. Dorard, Luciana Carla dos Santos Elias, S. Dunsmuir, N. Egorova, M. Escobar, N. Favez, A. Fontaine, H. Foran, Kaichiro Furutani, M. Gannagé, M. Gaspar, L. Godbout, Amit Goldenberg, J. Gross, Maria Ancuta Gurza, Ogma Hatta, Alexandre Heeren, Mai Helmy, M. Huynh, Emérence Kaneza, T. Kawamoto, Nassima Kellou, Bassantéa Lodegaèna Kpassagou, L. Lazarević, S. Le Vigouroux, Astrid Lebert-Charron, V. Leme, C. MacCann, Denisse Manrique-Millones, Oussama Medjahdi, Rosa Bertha Millones Rivalles, María Isabel Miranda Orrego, M. Miscioscia, Seyyedeh Fatemeh Mousavi, Badra Moutassem-Mimouni, H. Murphy, Alexis Ndayizigiye, Tenkue Josué Ngnombouowo, S. Olderbak, Sophie Ornawka, Daniela Oyarce Cadiz, P. Pérez-Díaz, K. Petrides, Alena Prikhidko, Fernando Salinas-Quiroz, M. Santelices, Charlotte Schrooyen, Paola Silva, A. Simonelli, M. Sor
{"title":"Parenting Culture(s): Ideal-Parent Beliefs Across 37 Countries","authors":"Gao-Xian Lin, Moïra Mikolajczak, H. Keller, Ege Akgun, G. Arıkan, K. Aunola, E. Barham, E. Besson, M. A. Blanchard, E. Boujut, M. Brianda, A. Brytek-Matera, Filipa César, Bin-Bin Chen, G. Dorard, Luciana Carla dos Santos Elias, S. Dunsmuir, N. Egorova, M. Escobar, N. Favez, A. Fontaine, H. Foran, Kaichiro Furutani, M. Gannagé, M. Gaspar, L. Godbout, Amit Goldenberg, J. Gross, Maria Ancuta Gurza, Ogma Hatta, Alexandre Heeren, Mai Helmy, M. Huynh, Emérence Kaneza, T. Kawamoto, Nassima Kellou, Bassantéa Lodegaèna Kpassagou, L. Lazarević, S. Le Vigouroux, Astrid Lebert-Charron, V. Leme, C. MacCann, Denisse Manrique-Millones, Oussama Medjahdi, Rosa Bertha Millones Rivalles, María Isabel Miranda Orrego, M. Miscioscia, Seyyedeh Fatemeh Mousavi, Badra Moutassem-Mimouni, H. Murphy, Alexis Ndayizigiye, Tenkue Josué Ngnombouowo, S. Olderbak, Sophie Ornawka, Daniela Oyarce Cadiz, P. Pérez-Díaz, K. Petrides, Alena Prikhidko, Fernando Salinas-Quiroz, M. Santelices, Charlotte Schrooyen, Paola Silva, A. Simonelli, M. Sor","doi":"10.1177/00220221221123043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221123043","url":null,"abstract":"What is it to be “an ideal parent”? Does the answer differ across countries and social classes? To answer these questions in a way that minimizes bias and ethnocentrism, we used open-ended questions to explore ideal-parent beliefs among 8,357 mothers and 3,517 fathers from 37 countries. Leximancer Semantic Network Analysis was utilized to first determine parenting culture zones (i.e., countries with shared ideal-parent beliefs) and then extract the predominant themes and concepts in each culture zone. The results yielded specific types of ideal-parent beliefs in five parenting culture zones: being “responsible and children/family-focused” for Asian parents, being “responsible and proper demeanor-focused” for African parents, and being “loving and responsible” for Hispanic-Italian parents. Although the most important themes and concepts were the same in the final two zones—being “loving and patient,” there were subtle differences: English-speaking, European Union, and Russian parents emphasized “being caring,” while French-speaking parents valued “listening” or being “present.” Ideal-parent beliefs also differed by education levels within culture zones, but no general pattern was discerned across culture zones. These findings suggest that the country in which parents were born cannot fully explain their differences in ideal-parent beliefs and that differences arising from social class or education level cannot be dismissed. Future research should consider how these differences affect the validity of the measurements in question and how they can be incorporated into parenting intervention research within and across cultures.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"4 - 24"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48086125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Content Analysis of Preferred Recovery Pathways Among Urban American Indians and Alaska Natives Experiencing Alcohol Use Disorders 美国城市印第安人和阿拉斯加原住民经历酒精使用障碍的首选康复途径的内容分析
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-14 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221132778
Lonnie A. Nelson, S. Collins, Jasmine Birch, Raven Burns, Grace McPhail, Jemima Onih, Cameron Cupp, Tatiana Ubay, Victorio L. King, Emily M Taylor, Karissa Masciel, Trevor Slaney, Joseph Bunch, Roxanna King, Celina Mahinalani-Garza, Benjamin K. S. Piper, Annette Squetimkin-Anquoe
{"title":"Content Analysis of Preferred Recovery Pathways Among Urban American Indians and Alaska Natives Experiencing Alcohol Use Disorders","authors":"Lonnie A. Nelson, S. Collins, Jasmine Birch, Raven Burns, Grace McPhail, Jemima Onih, Cameron Cupp, Tatiana Ubay, Victorio L. King, Emily M Taylor, Karissa Masciel, Trevor Slaney, Joseph Bunch, Roxanna King, Celina Mahinalani-Garza, Benjamin K. S. Piper, Annette Squetimkin-Anquoe","doi":"10.1177/00220221221132778","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221132778","url":null,"abstract":"Approximately three fourths of the American Indian and Alaska Native (AI/AN) population lives in urban areas, and urban AI/ANs are disproportionately affected by alcohol-related morbidity and mortality. Although no studies have documented alcohol use disorder (AUD) treatment outcomes specific to urban AI/ANs, studies in other Native communities highlight concerns about the cultural acceptability of directive, abstinence-based approaches, such as cognitive behavioral therapy and 12-step programs. Understanding this population’s desired recovery pathways in their own words may help providers create more culturally appropriate, patient-centered, and effective approaches. Participants (N = 31) were urban AI/ANs who screened positive for AUD using the AUDIT-C. They participated in semi-structured interviews eliciting their experiences in AUD treatment to date and suggestions for redesigning AUD treatment in their own vision. Conventional content analysis was used to create a thematic description. Findings indicated that intrinsic motivation and not extrinsic pressure (e.g., mandated treatment) was associated with positive treatment engagement and outcomes. Participants appreciated feeling safe and supported in AUD treatment, but also felt AUD treatment could be institutional and oppressive. Participants preferred compassionate counselors with lived experience who could provide insights into recovery; they largely did not appreciate a “tough love” approach or power struggles with counselors. Native-led treatment centers providing access to cultural practices were preferred. Moving forward, participants suggested AUD treatment providers should help patients meet basic needs, prioritize patient-driven versus provider-driven goal-setting, support patients’ reconnection with meaningful activities, facilitate access to a supportive community network, and recognize cultural activities as important recovery pathways.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"142 - 160"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44447779","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How I Remember My Mother’s Story: A Cross-National Investigation of Vicarious Family Stories in Turkey and New Zealand 我如何记住我母亲的故事:土耳其和新西兰邪恶家庭故事的跨国调查
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-02 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221132833
T. Bakir-Demir, E. Reese, Basak Sahin-Acar, M. Taumoepeau
{"title":"How I Remember My Mother’s Story: A Cross-National Investigation of Vicarious Family Stories in Turkey and New Zealand","authors":"T. Bakir-Demir, E. Reese, Basak Sahin-Acar, M. Taumoepeau","doi":"10.1177/00220221221132833","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221132833","url":null,"abstract":"Stories that have not been personally experienced by children and are only told by their parents are called vicarious family stories. An emerging body of literature has shown that vicarious family stories are an important part of children’s narrative ecology. However, to date, only two studies from the same cross-cultural project have examined the role of culture in vicarious family stories. The aims of this study were to examine vicarious mother stories in Turkey and New Zealand (NZ) and to investigate individual variations in national groups with regard to the internalization of cultural orientations (i.e., self-construals). There were 108 Turkish and 79 NZ women in this study. We found that Turkish women’s stories were more thematically coherent and included more social interactions and other-related words than NZ women’s. When reporting reasons for why they thought family members told stories, didactic purposes and expressing emotions were more common reasons for Turkish women, whereas sharing family history and entertainment were more common reasons for NZ women. However, Turkish and NZ women’s vicarious stories were similar in terms of identity connections and affective tone. Unexpectedly, we did not find a significant role of individuals’ self-construals in the link between national groups and vicarious stories. This study contributes to the growing area of research on family narratives by showing the commonalities and differences in the construction of vicarious stories across national groups.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"340 - 364"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45610850","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Context-Sensitivity Influences German and Chinese Preschoolers’ Comprehension of Indirect Communication 语境敏感性对德汉语学前儿童间接交际理解的影响
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221104952
Cornelia Schulze, D. Buttelmann, Liqi Zhu, Henrik Saalbach
{"title":"Context-Sensitivity Influences German and Chinese Preschoolers’ Comprehension of Indirect Communication","authors":"Cornelia Schulze, D. Buttelmann, Liqi Zhu, Henrik Saalbach","doi":"10.1177/00220221221104952","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221104952","url":null,"abstract":"Making inferences in communication is a highly context-dependent endeavor. Previous research found cultural variations for context-sensitivity as well as for communication comprehension. However, the relative impact of culture and context-sensitivity on communication comprehension has not been investigated so far. The current study aimed at investigating this interplay and tested 4- and 6-year-old children from Germany (n = 132) and China (n = 129). Context-sensitivity was measured with an adapted version of the Ebbinghaus illusion. In this task, children have to discriminate the size of two target circles that only appear to be of similar size due to context circles surrounding the target circles. As expected, performance scores indicated higher degrees of context-sensitivity in Chinese compared to German children and that 6-year-olds were more context-sensitive than 4-year-olds. Further, in an object-choice communication-comprehension task, children watched videos with puppets performing every-day activities (e.g., pet care) and had to choose between two options (e.g., dog or rabbit). A puppet expressed what she wanted either directly (“I want the rabbit”) or indirectly (“I have a carrot”). The children had to choose one option to give to the puppet. In both cultures, 6-year-olds outperformed 4-year-olds and children understood direct communication better than indirect communication. Culture was found to affect children’s processing speed of direct communication. Moreover, culture influenced children’s context-sensitivity while context-sensitivity influenced children’s accuracy in the indirect (but not the direct) communication task. These findings demonstrate that taking context into account is especially important when we are confronted with indirect communication.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"53 1","pages":"1257 - 1276"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45481406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Relationship Between Cultural Engagement and Psychological Well-being Among Indigenous Adolescents: A Systematic Review 原住民青少年文化参与与心理健康之关系:系统回顾
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-10-31 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221128215
Elizabeth Doery, Lata Satyen, Y. Paradies, J. Toumbourou
{"title":"The Relationship Between Cultural Engagement and Psychological Well-being Among Indigenous Adolescents: A Systematic Review","authors":"Elizabeth Doery, Lata Satyen, Y. Paradies, J. Toumbourou","doi":"10.1177/00220221221128215","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221128215","url":null,"abstract":"The disproportionate burden of mental illness experienced by Indigenous adolescents is well established. Therefore, this review focused on how the well-being of Indigenous adolescents can be better promoted. The review identified studies that examined the relationship between cultural engagement and psychological well-being among Indigenous adolescents. To achieve this, a systematic search of published literature across seven online databases including Medline and EMBASE was conducted between October and November 2020. To meet the inclusion criteria, studies were required to include a sample of Indigenous adolescents and measure the relationship between psychological well-being and cultural engagement. Twenty-five studies met the inclusion criteria, yielding a total sample size of 19,231 participants. Eighteen studies (72%) reported a significant positive relationship between cultural engagement and psychological well-being, four studies (16%) reported a nonsignificant relationship, and three studies (12%) reported mixed findings. Despite measuring different domains of culture across the 25 studies, these findings demonstrate relatively strong evidence of a positive association between cultural engagement and psychological well-being. They highlight the importance of culture for young Indigenous Peoples in developing a positive well-being. In the future, researchers should focus on specifying how intervention factors contribute to cultural engagement effects and establish further contributors to well-being and positive development among Indigenous adolescents. The findings of this review advance our understanding of how Indigenous Peoples interpret culture and their engagement with this culture. This has implications for policy, programs, and interventions intended to enhance well-being outcomes for Indigenous communities.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"90 - 113"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46058829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural Variations in Perceived Partner Responsiveness: The Role of Self-Consistency 感知伴侣反应的文化差异:自我一致性的作用
IF 3 3区 心理学
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Pub Date : 2022-10-31 DOI: 10.1177/00220221221132786
Hyewon Choi, S. Oishi
{"title":"Cultural Variations in Perceived Partner Responsiveness: The Role of Self-Consistency","authors":"Hyewon Choi, S. Oishi","doi":"10.1177/00220221221132786","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221132786","url":null,"abstract":"Past research has shown that perceived partner responsiveness (PPR) is a key process contributing to individual and relational outcomes and identified dispositional, relational, and situational factors that can influence it. However, little is known about how cultural factors play a role in the process of PPR. In Studies 1 (n = 4,041) and 2 (n = 414), we examined whether the degree of PPR differs across cultures by comparing European Americans and East Asians. We found that East Asians are less likely to experience perceived responsiveness from others than European Americans (Cohen’s d = 1.11–1.25 for Study 1 and Cohen’s d = 0.23 for Study 2). Furthermore, we found that self-consistency explained the cultural difference in PPR, indicating that East Asians underperceived partner responsiveness compared with European Americans because they behave less consistently across social situations. We conclude by highlighting the importance of exploring the process of PPR from a cultural perspective.","PeriodicalId":48354,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology","volume":"54 1","pages":"303 - 318"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46432690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信