Elfriede R. Holstein, Maria Theobald, Leonie S. Weindorf, Garvin Brod
{"title":"Developing Conflict Monitoring Abilities Predict Children's Revision of an Intuitive Theory","authors":"Elfriede R. Holstein, Maria Theobald, Leonie S. Weindorf, Garvin Brod","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14241","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14241","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We investigated the role of children's conflict monitoring skills in revising an intuitive scientific theory. Children aged 5 to 9 (<i>N</i> = 177; 53% girls, data collected in Germany from 2019-2023) completed computer-based tasks on water displacement, a concept prone to misconceptions. Children predicted which of two objects would displace more water before receiving feedback. With increasing age, children showed slower response times for incorrect predictions (<i>β</i> = −0.04) and greater pupil dilation to unexpected outcomes (<i>β</i> = −0.04), indicating better conflict monitoring. Better conflict monitoring, in turn, predicted faster belief revision (<i>β</i> = 0.07). These findings suggest that conflict monitoring is crucial for learning in discovery-based activities.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1207-1219"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14241","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143745581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Karen L. Bierman, Brenda S. Heinrichs, Janet A. Welsh, Damon E. Jones, D. Max Crowley
{"title":"How a Preschool Intervention Affected High School Outcomes: Longitudinal Pathways in a Randomized-Controlled Trial","authors":"Karen L. Bierman, Brenda S. Heinrichs, Janet A. Welsh, Damon E. Jones, D. Max Crowley","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14235","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14235","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examined the impact of the Head Start Research-based, Developmentally Informed (REDI) preschool intervention on high school outcomes and explored longitudinal mediation. 356 children (58% White, 25% Black, 17% Latinx; 54% female, 46% male; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 4.49 years) were recruited from Head Start classrooms which were randomized to intervention (<i>N</i> = 192) or “usual practice” (<i>N</i> = 164). REDI effects emerged on high school emotional symptoms (teacher ratings, <i>d</i> = 0.41) and behavior problems (composite of teacher, parent, youth ratings, <i>d</i> = 0.27) with the latter benefits mediated by earlier intervention boosts to social–emotional learning, social adjustment, and parent involvement. REDI had no direct impact on GPA or on-time graduation but promoted these outcomes indirectly mediated by earlier intervention effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1236-1249"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14235","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143745580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Sophie Bridgers, Kiera Parece, Ibuki Iwasaki, Annalisa Broski, Laura Schulz, Tomer Ullman
{"title":"Learning Loopholes: The Development of Intentional Misunderstandings in Children","authors":"Sophie Bridgers, Kiera Parece, Ibuki Iwasaki, Annalisa Broski, Laura Schulz, Tomer Ullman","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14222","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14222","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>What do children do when they do not want to obey but cannot afford to disobey? Might they, like adults, feign misunderstanding and seek out loopholes? Across four studies (<i>N</i> = 723; 44% female; USA; majority White; data collected 2020–2023), we find that loophole behavior emerges around ages 5 to 6 (Study 1, 3–18 years), that children think loopholes will get them into less trouble than non-compliance (Study 2, 4–10 years), predict that other children will be more likely to exploit loopholes when goals conflict (Study 3, 5–10 years), and are increasingly able to generate loopholes themselves (Study 4, 5–10 years). This work provides new insights on how children navigate the gray area between compliance and defiance and the development of loophole behavior across early and middle childhood.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1066-1087"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143604111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Michaela C. DeBolt, Bess L. Caswell, Matthews George, Kenneth Maleta, Elizabeth L. Prado, Shannon Ross-Sheehy, Christine P. Stewart, Lisa M. Oakes
{"title":"A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Infants' Spatial Attention on the Infant Orienting With Attention (IOWA) Task","authors":"Michaela C. DeBolt, Bess L. Caswell, Matthews George, Kenneth Maleta, Elizabeth L. Prado, Shannon Ross-Sheehy, Christine P. Stewart, Lisa M. Oakes","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14228","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14228","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Research with Western samples has uncovered the rapid development of infants' visual attention. This study evaluated spatial attention in 6- to 9-month-old infants living in rural Malawi (<i>N</i> = 511; <span></span><math>\u0000 <semantics>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msub>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mtext>Boys</mtext>\u0000 </msub>\u0000 </mrow>\u0000 </semantics></math> = 255, <span></span><math>\u0000 <semantics>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msub>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mi>Yao</mi>\u0000 </msub>\u0000 </mrow>\u0000 </semantics></math> = 427) or suburban California, United States (<i>N</i> = 57, <span></span><math>\u0000 <semantics>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msub>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mtext>Boys</mtext>\u0000 </msub>\u0000 </mrow>\u0000 </semantics></math> = 29, <span></span><math>\u0000 <semantics>\u0000 <mrow>\u0000 <msub>\u0000 <mi>n</mi>\u0000 <mtext>White</mtext>\u0000 </msub>\u0000 </mrow>\u0000 </semantics></math> = 37) in 2018–2019. Using the Infant Orienting With Attention (IOWA) task, results showed that infants were faster and more accurate to fixate a target when a cue validly predicted the target location and were slower and less accurate when the cue was invalid. However, compared to US infants, Malawian infants took longer to fixate the target and were more accurate. These results both provide information about the development of spatial attention in an underrepresented population and demonstrate differences in spatial attention in infants with different lived experiences.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1050-1065"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143575265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
M. Perapoch Amadó, E. A. M. Phillips, G. Esposito, E. Greenwood, J. Ives, P. Labendzki, K. Lancaster, T. J. Northrop, N. K. Viswanathan, M. Gök, M. J. Peñaherrera, E. J. H. Jones, S. V. Wass
{"title":"Who Leads and Who Follows? The Pathways to Joint Attention During Free-Flowing Interactions Change Over Developmental Time","authors":"M. Perapoch Amadó, E. A. M. Phillips, G. Esposito, E. Greenwood, J. Ives, P. Labendzki, K. Lancaster, T. J. Northrop, N. K. Viswanathan, M. Gök, M. J. Peñaherrera, E. J. H. Jones, S. V. Wass","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14229","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14229","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Joint attention (JA) has been found to correlate with many developmental outcomes. However, little is known about how naturalistic JA is established and develops during early infancy. In this study, free-flowing tabletop toy play between infants at 5 and 15 months and their mothers (<i>N</i> = 48 dyads; 65% white) was observed to (1) examine changes in JA, (2) investigate whether infants become better leaders or followers of JA, and (3) explore the role of intentionally mediated forms of communication. JA episodes increased in frequency and duration, and initiations of JA became more evenly distributed between members of the dyad. Older infants became better at leading as well as following their mothers' attention behaviors and more frequently directed their attention towards their partner, though this had minimal impact on the organization of episodes of JA.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1112-1127"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14229","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143434988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Exploring the Out-Group Homogeneity Effect Among Arab Children in Israel: The Roles of Religion, Contact, and Group Identification","authors":"Francine Essa, Hannes Rakoczy, Gil Diesendruck","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14226","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14226","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The out-group homogeneity effect has been found to contribute to adults' inter-group biases. Three studies examined whether 5- and 8-year-old Arab (i.e., minority) children in Israel also manifest this effect (March 2017–January 2020). Arab children from different religious affiliations and social environments (<i>N</i> = 272, 54% females) were asked to choose either a homogeneous or a heterogeneous sample of group members to infer if a given property (biological or psychological) was true of a whole group: either the participant's in-group (Arabs) or out-group (Jews). Overall, differently from Jewish (i.e., majority) Israeli children, Arab children did not exhibit the out-group homogeneity effect. Nevertheless, there were indications that religious affiliation, social environment, and group identification affected children's responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1148-1164"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14226","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143427383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Sharon Wolf, Johanna Bernard, Chika R. Ezeugwu, Lubabatu Ahmad, Khadijah Bello Gurin
{"title":"Impacts of the Preschool Safe Program on Marginalized Girls in Northern Nigeria","authors":"Sharon Wolf, Johanna Bernard, Chika R. Ezeugwu, Lubabatu Ahmad, Khadijah Bello Gurin","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14225","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14225","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Quality early childhood education (ECE) can improve children's learning and development in low- and middle-income countries, but little evidence exists of programs targeting marginalized girls in vulnerable rural communities. This study used a matched-pair mixed-methods community-randomized trial to evaluate a preschool program for marginalized girls (<i>N</i> = 466, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 5-years; W<sub>1</sub> = 2022; W<sub>2</sub> = 2023) from 16 rural Hausa-ethnic communities in Nigeria. The program yielded considerably large impacts on every domain of school readiness assessed (<i>dt</i> = 0.89–1.52). In qualitative interviews (<i>N</i> = 30, data collected in 2023), parents discussed witnessing their daughters' learning and development as well as their future educational and life goals for their daughters. Results demonstrate that access to high-quality ECE in highly disadvantaged communities can dramatically improve girls' learning and potentially change parental perceptions about girl-child education.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1128-1147"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14225","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143413593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"The Role of Controllability and Foreseeability in Children's Counterfactual Emotions","authors":"Alicia K. Jones, Shalini Gautam, Jonathan Redshaw","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14224","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14224","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Counterfactual emotions such as regret may aid future decision-making by encouraging people to focus on controllable features of personal past events. However, it remains unclear when children begin to preferentially focus on controllable features of such events. Across two studies, Australian 4–9-year-olds (<i>N</i> = 336, 168 females; data collected during 2021–2022) completed tasks that led to positive or negative personal outcomes, and then reported their emotions toward different aspects of these tasks. In both studies, younger children unexpectedly reported stronger sadness toward uncontrollable or unforeseeable aspects of negative events, and only by 8–9 years did many children report stronger sadness toward controllable or foreseeable aspects. The tendency to focus on more functional counterfactuals may therefore emerge relatively late in development.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1098-1111"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14224","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143413545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Personal Identity Development in Chinese Early Adolescents: Predictions From Parenting Style and Peer Relationships","authors":"Weiqiao Fan, Mengting Li","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14227","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14227","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This four-wave longitudinal study among 698 Chinese early adolescents explored (1) how personal identity coherence and confusion develop; and (2) whether parenting style and peer relationships (i.e., close friend relationships and peer preference) were related to personal identity development. Participants (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 11.39 yrs., SD<sub>age</sub> = 0.53; 53.7% girls) reported perceived parenting style, close friend relationships, and personal identity in Grade 6. Peer preference was assessed using peer nominations. Personal identity was remeasured in Grades 7, 8, and 9. The multiple-indicator latent growth curve models revealed a decreased pattern in identity coherence and a curvilinear pattern in identity confusion. Higher authoritarian parenting and friend support were associated with sharper decreases in identity coherence. Implications for improving adolescent identity development were discussed.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"1165-1176"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143381711","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Luke McGuire, Tina Bagus, Alexander G. Carter, Emma Fry, Nadira S. Faber
{"title":"Reasoning to Justify Eating Animals Varies With Age","authors":"Luke McGuire, Tina Bagus, Alexander G. Carter, Emma Fry, Nadira S. Faber","doi":"10.1111/cdev.14217","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cdev.14217","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The present study examined the justifications used by children, adolescents, and adults to justify eating animals. Children (<i>n</i> = 100, <i>M</i><sup>age</sup> = 9.82, SD = 0.77, female <i>n</i> = 49) as compared to adolescents (<i>n</i> = 76, <i>M</i><sup>age</sup> = 14.0, SD = 1.62, female <i>n</i> = 36) and adults (<i>n</i> = 113, <i>M</i><sup>age</sup> = 44.1, SD = 14.4, female <i>n</i> = 54) were more ambivalent or opposed to eating animals, and they showed a distinct reasoning pattern. Children relied less on arguments about meat eating being natural or with to humane slaughter practices. These findings align with recent theoretical perspectives that reasoning may be used to counter cognitive dissonance arising from knowledge of food production systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":10109,"journal":{"name":"Child development","volume":"96 3","pages":"953-965"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.14217","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143071506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}