心理学最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Benefits of the "worst-case scenario": a multi-level examination of the effects of confronting the feared outcome during imagery-based exposure. “最坏情况”的好处:在基于图像的暴露中,对面对恐惧结果的影响进行多层次的检查。
IF 4.3 2区 心理学
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-30 DOI: 10.1080/16506073.2025.2456479
Sarah C Jessup, Thomas Armstrong, Catherine E Rast, Sarah E Woronko, Mitchell Jackson, Alexander L Anwyl-Irvine, Edwin S Dalmaijer, Bunmi O Olatunji
{"title":"Benefits of the \"worst-case scenario\": a multi-level examination of the effects of confronting the feared outcome during imagery-based exposure.","authors":"Sarah C Jessup, Thomas Armstrong, Catherine E Rast, Sarah E Woronko, Mitchell Jackson, Alexander L Anwyl-Irvine, Edwin S Dalmaijer, Bunmi O Olatunji","doi":"10.1080/16506073.2025.2456479","DOIUrl":"10.1080/16506073.2025.2456479","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Exposure therapy is an efficacious treatment for anxiety-related disorders. Yet, fear often returns after treatment. Occasional reinforcement, in which the feared stimulus is intermittently presented during extinction, increases safety learning and slows fear renewal in conditioning paradigms and analogue samples, but no studies to date have examined this strategy in clinical samples. The present study examined the effects of vicarious occasional reinforcement on fear renewal in a snake-phobic sample across multiple levels of analysis. Fear was intermittently reinforced by providing reminders of the feared outcome (a snake bite) throughout a two-session analogue video exposure manipulation. Snake-phobic adults were randomized to one of three conditions: a single-cue [S], multiple-cue [M], or multiple-cue+fear-outcome [M+FO] exposure group. Results showed the three groups did not significantly differ in threat expectancy or attentional bias for threat at follow-up. Despite sustained anxiety, however, the M+FO condition completed significantly more steps on a visual avoidance task at follow-up than the M and S conditions and heightened mean distress during exposure mediated this effect. The M and S groups did not significantly differ in visual avoidance at follow-up. These findings suggest incorporating reminders of the feared outcome into exposure may be an effective strategy for increasing inhibitory retrieval.</p>","PeriodicalId":10535,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Behaviour Therapy","volume":" ","pages":"644-663"},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143064325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Validation of the Reactive Attachment Disorder and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder Assessment (RADA): A DSM-5 Semistructured Interview. 反应性依恋障碍和抑制性社会参与障碍评估(RADA)的验证:DSM-5 半结构式访谈。
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Assessment Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1177/10731911241276625
Maude Archambault, Chantal Cyr, Helen Minnis, Stine Lehmann, Sebastien Monette
{"title":"Validation of the Reactive Attachment Disorder and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder Assessment (RADA): A <i>DSM</i>-5 Semistructured Interview.","authors":"Maude Archambault, Chantal Cyr, Helen Minnis, Stine Lehmann, Sebastien Monette","doi":"10.1177/10731911241276625","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10731911241276625","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Very few empirically validated tools exist for assessing reactive attachment disorder (RAD) and disinhibited social engagement disorder (DSED). The RAD and DSED assessment interview (RADA), a semistructured diagnostic interview, was updated in 2018 from the CAPA-RAD interview to reflect the diagnostic criteria changes in the <i>Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders</i> (5th ed.; <i>DSM-5</i>). The aim of this study was to validate the RADA on school-age children in Canada. Caregivers of 5 to 12-year-old children from the community (<i>n</i> = 98), in foster care (<i>n</i> = 147), and in residential care (<i>n</i> = 123) completed the RADA interview and a series of questionnaires. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of the RADA interview supported a four-factor structure similar to the <i>DSM-5</i> symptom clusters. A short \"strictly <i>DSM-5</i>\" version of the RADA showed a two-factor structure-RAD and DSED-and an excellent fit to the data. Scales of both structures showed good-to-excellent internal consistency, interrater reliability, convergent validity, and known-group validity. Classifying the children yielded RAD and DSED rates of <1% and 18%, respectively, for children in foster care and 7% and 10%, respectively, for children in residential care. This study supports the validity of the RADA interview for school-age children and is the first to provide RAD and DSED rates for children in residential care.</p>","PeriodicalId":8577,"journal":{"name":"Assessment","volume":" ","pages":"839-858"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142340179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining interrelations among trajectories of mindful awareness, acceptance, and values-consistent actions in acceptance-based behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. 研究以接受为基础的行为疗法治疗广泛性焦虑症过程中的正念意识、接受和价值观一致行动之间的相互关系。
IF 4.3 2区 心理学
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1080/16506073.2024.2423654
Stephanie Marando-Blanck, Sarah A Hayes-Skelton, Lizabeth Roemer, Susan M Orsillo
{"title":"Examining interrelations among trajectories of mindful awareness, acceptance, and values-consistent actions in acceptance-based behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder.","authors":"Stephanie Marando-Blanck, Sarah A Hayes-Skelton, Lizabeth Roemer, Susan M Orsillo","doi":"10.1080/16506073.2024.2423654","DOIUrl":"10.1080/16506073.2024.2423654","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goal of this study was to understand how mindful awareness, acceptance, and values-consistent action change across acceptance-based behavioral therapy (ABBT) for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and determine their effect on symptoms. We examined weekly data from 31 individuals who received ABBT as part of a randomized control trial for individuals with GAD (Hayes-Skelton, Roemer, & Orsillo, 2013). Participants answered questions weekly about three components of ABBT, including the percentages of time they spent over their past week: 1) aware of the present moment, 2) accepting of their internal experiences, and 3) engaging in values. GAD symptoms were examined using two self-report measures (DASS-21 Stress subscale and PSWQ) and two clinician-rated interview measures (CSR and SIGH-A) at pre- and post-treatment.<sup>1</sup> Mindful awareness, acceptance, and values-consistent action increased linearly across ABBT. All three change trajectories were positively correlated (<i>Z's</i> 2.99 to 8.74, <i>p'</i>s < .001). Mindful awareness, acceptance, and values-consistent action across treatment predicted decreases in GAD symptoms above and beyond baseline for most outcome measures (<i>Z's</i> -1.95 to -3.03, <i>p'</i>s < .05), with the exception that mindfulness did not predict DASS-stress (<i>Z = -1.39, p</i> = .17). These findings are consistent with the structure and model of ABBT.</p>","PeriodicalId":10535,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Behaviour Therapy","volume":" ","pages":"577-595"},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC12056163/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142602935","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looking on the (B)right Side of Life: Cognitive Ability and Miscalibrated Financial Expectations. 看生活的正确一面:认知能力和错误校准的财务预期。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231209400
Chris Dawson
{"title":"Looking on the (B)right Side of Life: Cognitive Ability and Miscalibrated Financial Expectations.","authors":"Chris Dawson","doi":"10.1177/01461672231209400","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231209400","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is a puzzle why humans tend toward unrealistic optimism, as it can lead to excessively risky behavior and a failure to take precautionary action. Using data from a large nationally representative U.K. sample <math><mrow><mo>(</mo><mi>N</mi><mo>=</mo><mn>36</mn><mo>,</mo><mn>312</mn><mo>)</mo><mo>,</mo></mrow></math> our claim is that optimism bias is partly a consequence of low cognition-as measured by a broad range of cognitive skills, including memory, verbal fluency, fluid reasoning and numerical reasoning. We operationalize unrealistic optimism as the difference between a person's financial expectation and the financial realization that follows, measured annually over a decade. All else being equal, those highest on cognitive ability experience a 22% (53.2%) increase in the probability of realism (pessimism) and a 34.8% reduction in optimism compared with those lowest on cognitive ability. This suggests that the negative consequences of an excessively optimistic mindset may, in part, be a side product of the true driver, low cognitive ability.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1703-1715"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC12276405/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring Asymmetries in Self-Concept Change After Discrepant Feedback. 探索不同反馈后自我概念变化的不对称性。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1177/01461672241232738
Franziska Brotzeller, Mario Gollwitzer
{"title":"Exploring Asymmetries in Self-Concept Change After Discrepant Feedback.","authors":"Franziska Brotzeller, Mario Gollwitzer","doi":"10.1177/01461672241232738","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672241232738","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Receiving self-relevant feedback that is discrepant from one's self-concept can lead to self-concept change. However, it is currently unclear whether positive or negative feedback has a larger effect on self-concept change. Across four studies (total <i>N</i> = 1,438), we demonstrate that intentions for self-concept change (Study 1) as well as actual self-concept change (Studies 2, 3, and 4) are larger (a) for larger discrepancies between self-concept and feedback and (b) for negative compared to positive discrepancies. Exploring these effects further in Study 4, we find no evidence that the opportunity for improvement influences whether self-concept change is positively or negatively biased. In sum, the present research provides consistent evidence for a negativity bias in self-concept change, investigates a theoretical explanation, and discusses alternative explanatory approaches.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1731-1744"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC12276396/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140028634","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal Changes in Chinese Prosociality. 中国人亲社会性的纵向变化。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-30 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231225367
Sijing Chen, Shasha Yang
{"title":"Longitudinal Changes in Chinese Prosociality.","authors":"Sijing Chen, Shasha Yang","doi":"10.1177/01461672231225367","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231225367","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article presents three studies using data from the <i>World Values Survey</i>, 128 published studies, and <i>China Family Panel Studies</i> to comprehensively examine the longitudinal dynamics of Chinese prosociality, encompassing prosocial attitudes, tendencies, and behaviors, with the overarching goal of shedding light on the evolving nature of prosociality in the Chinese context. These studies reveal a consistent pattern, illustrating a decline followed by a resurgence in all three aspects, with a nadir around 2014. In addition, the study investigates the intricate relationship between economic inequality, prosocial behavior, and prosocial attitudes. The findings suggest that while economic inequality significantly relates to prosocial behavior, it does not entirely explain its fluctuations. Prosocial attitudes partially mediate the connection between economic inequality and prosocial behavior. These insights suggest that addressing inequality could contribute to a more conducive social environment for societal-level prosociality. However, further research is imperative to explore additional determinants of prosociality shifts.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1574-1586"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139642665","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disrupted orientation after path integration by absence of anticipated prevalent spatial views. 由于缺乏预期的普遍空间视图,路径整合后方向中断。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2025-02-13 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001439
Yue Chen, Weimin Mou
{"title":"Disrupted orientation after path integration by absence of anticipated prevalent spatial views.","authors":"Yue Chen, Weimin Mou","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001439","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001439","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two previous studies indicated that removing expected landmarks disrupts homing, a new phenomenon concerning the interplay between path integration and landmark-based navigation. This study systematically investigated when this disruption occurs, which spatial representations are disrupted, and whether lessening landmark prevalence mitigates this disruption. In immersive virtual environments, against three landmarks, participants learned the location of a home object (Experiments 2 and 3), plus two additional objects (Experiment 1), or plus four additional objects (Experiments 4 and 5). They then navigated an outbound path originating from the home object. After participants' views were blocked, landmarks were revealed for nine standard paths/trials but removed in a subsequent catch trial, except in Experiment 3 where a curtain kept landmarks concealed. In Experiment 5, landmarks were rotated instead of being removed in the second catch trial. Participants replaced the home object in standard trials but all objects in catch trials. Baseline trials, which were identical to the catch trials except for no landmarks throughout the trials, followed catch trials. The results showed larger homing errors in first catch trials than baseline trials when landmarks were removed (Experiment 2) but not when the curtain concealed the landmarks (Experiment 3). For experiments with multiple objects, participants' represented position and heading were calculated based on the replaced and correct locations. Experiment 1 showed disrupted homing and heading estimates but intact position estimates, while Experiments 4 and 5 showed no disruption. Additionally, participants followed rotated landmarks in Experiment 5. These findings provide a comprehensive picture of the interplay between path integration and landmarks in familiar environments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1412-1429"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143416083","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using Freely Generated Labels Instead of Rating Scales to Assess Emotion in Everyday Life. 使用自由生成的标签而不是等级量表来评估日常生活中的情绪。
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Assessment Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1177/10731911241283623
Katie Hoemann, Evan Warfel, Caitlin Mills, Laura Allen, Peter Kuppens, Jolie B Wormwood
{"title":"Using Freely Generated Labels Instead of Rating Scales to Assess Emotion in Everyday Life.","authors":"Katie Hoemann, Evan Warfel, Caitlin Mills, Laura Allen, Peter Kuppens, Jolie B Wormwood","doi":"10.1177/10731911241283623","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10731911241283623","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To measure emotion in daily life, studies often prompt participants to repeatedly rate their feelings on a set of prespecified terms. This approach has yielded key findings in the psychological literature yet may not represent how people typically describe their experiences. We used an alternative approach, in which participants labeled their current emotion with at least one word of their choosing. In an initial study, estimates of label positivity recapitulated momentary valence ratings and were associated with self-reported mental health. The number of unique emotion words used over time was related to the balance and spread of emotions endorsed in an end-of-day rating task, but not to other measures of emotional functioning. A second study tested and replicated a subset of these findings. Considering the variety and richness of participant responses, a free-label approach appears to be a viable as well as compelling means of studying emotion in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":8577,"journal":{"name":"Assessment","volume":" ","pages":"859-877"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142340178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mabrouk's Measure of Adolescent Optimism: Development and Validation of a New Scale of Adolescent Optimism. Mabrouk's Measure of Adolescent Optimism:青少年乐观主义新量表的开发与验证》。
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Assessment Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1177/10731911241280769
Ezra Too, Esther Jebor Chongwo, Adam Mabrouk, Paul Mwangi, Millicent Makandi, Anita Kerubo, Rachel Odhiambo, Margaret Kabue, Joyce Marangu, Vibian Angwenyi, Amina Abubakar
{"title":"Mabrouk's Measure of Adolescent Optimism: Development and Validation of a New Scale of Adolescent Optimism.","authors":"Ezra Too, Esther Jebor Chongwo, Adam Mabrouk, Paul Mwangi, Millicent Makandi, Anita Kerubo, Rachel Odhiambo, Margaret Kabue, Joyce Marangu, Vibian Angwenyi, Amina Abubakar","doi":"10.1177/10731911241280769","DOIUrl":"10.1177/10731911241280769","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several measures of adolescent optimism exist. However, none of these measures have been developed in the African setting, and only a few of the available measures have been validated for use in this setting. We aimed to develop and validate a culturally appropriate measure for this context. We employed an exploratory sequential mixed methods design using a five-step approach to tool development, comprising literature reviews, construct clarification, item development, piloting, and scale evaluation. We constructed a six-item scale, the Mabrouk's Measure of Adolescent Optimism (MMAO). In a sample of 1616 adolescents from Kenya, the MMAO demonstrated adequate internal consistency (both Cronbach's alpha and McDonald's Omega > 0.80) and test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.46). Factor analysis supported a unidimensional scale with adequate criterion and divergent validity as well as measurement invariance across sex, age (younger vs. older adolescents), residence (rural vs. urban), schooling status (in-school vs. out-of-school adolescents), and language of administration (Swahili vs. English). We report on the development and validation of a new scale of adolescent optimism that can be used to assess optimism among adolescents in Kenya and similar contexts. Preliminary evidence shows support for this new measure's reliability and validity, although additional tests are needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":8577,"journal":{"name":"Assessment","volume":" ","pages":"932-945"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142340176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the use of interoceptive exposure exercises in people with and without a history of chronic physical health problems. 检查在有和没有慢性身体健康问题史的人群中使用内感受性暴露练习。
IF 4.3 2区 心理学
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Pub Date : 2025-09-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.1080/16506073.2024.2447722
Samantha G Farris, Michael J Zvolensky, Lorra Garey, Michael S Businelle
{"title":"Examining the use of interoceptive exposure exercises in people with and without a history of chronic physical health problems.","authors":"Samantha G Farris, Michael J Zvolensky, Lorra Garey, Michael S Businelle","doi":"10.1080/16506073.2024.2447722","DOIUrl":"10.1080/16506073.2024.2447722","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interoceptive exposure (IE) exercises are underused, especially in people with chronic physical health problems. Secondary data analyses were conducted to examine the use of and acute responses to IE exercises in individuals with and without a history of chronic physical health problem(s). Participants (<i>N</i> = 413; Mage = 38.6, 56.1% with chronic physical health problems) enrolled in a 6-month randomized controlled trial that aimed to reduce anxiety and depression. Participants had access to a smartphone-based intervention that included on-demand access to IE exercises: head rush, straw breathing, chair spinning, fast breathing, and running in place. Utilization of the IE exercises and acute responses to the exercises were evaluated in the context of chronic physical health problem history. Participants with versus without a chronic physical health problem completed statistically significantly more IE exercises. Acute effects of IE exercises on distress and physical sensations were similar for those with and without a history of chronic physical health problems, as well as those who were versus were not taking medication. There is no evidence that chronic disease history influences the acute response to IE exercises. Continued research is needed to further assess the safety of IE exercises in people with various medical conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":10535,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Behaviour Therapy","volume":" ","pages":"596-612"},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC12238293/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142945915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信