Laila Ketvel, Eero Vuoksimaa, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Ulla Vedenkannas, Pekka Rapeli, Anu Raevuori, Antti Latvala
{"title":"Dimensional attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and executive functioning in adolescence: A multi-informant, population-based twin study.","authors":"Laila Ketvel, Eero Vuoksimaa, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Ulla Vedenkannas, Pekka Rapeli, Anu Raevuori, Antti Latvala","doi":"10.1037/neu0000983","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000983","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate associations of executive function (EF) performance in adolescence with dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity assessed by multiple informants as well as ADHD (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) symptoms based on the <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> criteria, and whether familial factors and co-occurring symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder explain these associations in a population-based sample.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In 14-year-old twins from the population-based FinnTwin12 study (<i>N</i> = 638-1,227), we assessed EF with commonly used neuropsychological tests. <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> symptoms of ADHD and psychiatric disorders were assessed with a semistructured interview, and dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with behavioral ratings made by the twins, their co-twins, and teachers at age 14, and by parents and teachers at age 12, the latter being different from those at age 14.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Teacher-rated inattention had the strongest association with poorer EF performance across two measurement points with different teachers; these associations were not affected by adding symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder as covariates. Within-pair analyses suggested that the associations of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with EF were partly explained by familial factors.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Even at a subclinical level in a sample of adolescents representing general population, ADHD symptoms are associated with EF performance. Teachers' evaluations appear especially valuable when assessing adolescents' ADHD symptoms. Our findings support the notion of dimensional ADHD symptoms in the population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142648499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Capturing cognitive capacity in the everyday environment across a continuum of cognitive decline using a smartwatch n-back task and ecological momentary assessment.","authors":"Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe, Catherine Luna, Brooke Beech, Shenghai Dai, Diane J Cook","doi":"10.1037/neu0000984","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000984","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cognitive testing using mobile technologies can assist with early detection of cognitive decline. We use ecological momentary assessment to investigate the feasibility, reliability, and validity of a smartwatch n-back task (1-back) delivered to adults across the cognitive continuum.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>One hundred seventy-four community-dwelling individuals (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 70.51) representing healthy older adults, individuals with subjective cognitive decline, and mild cognitive impairment completed a neurocognitive assessment battery and wore a smartwatch for 7+ days. Participants were prompted 4 times per day to complete an n-back task on the smartwatch.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across all groups, findings indicated an acceptable task adherence rate (> 78%; <i>n</i> = 174) and response rate (> 89%; <i>n</i> = 158 <i>n</i>-back analysis sample). Supporting external validity, participants with mild cognitive impairment were less accurate, had fewer total correct responses, and performed at lower initial levels than both healthy older adults and subjective cognitive decline, ω²s > .09. Intraindividual variability was greater for the mild cognitive impairment group compared to healthy older adults, but subjective cognitive decline did not differ significantly from either group, ω² = .12. For discriminant and convergent validity, <i>n</i>-back total correct correlated with performance on standardized assessments of executive attention, whereas intraindividual variability correlated with real-world factors (i.e., context, everyday function). Reliability assessment revealed stability for <i>n</i>-back measures after four to six posttraining trials and excellent test-retest reliability for total correct after 5 months. Finally, combining <i>n</i>-back and clinical measures improved classification accuracy.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings suggest the smartwatch <i>n</i>-back task is feasible for collecting cognitive data across the cognitive continuum with demonstrated reliability and validity in the everyday environment using ecological momentary assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142648497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Affliction class moderates the dementing impact of adipokines.","authors":"Donald R Royall, Raymond F Palmer","doi":"10.1037/neu0000968","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000968","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Biomarker-specific interventions (e.g., for dementia) will necessitate an individualized approach to treatment. We have constructed a psychometric classifier to identify persons adversely impacted by plasma adipokines.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The subjects (<i>N</i> = 1,737) of the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative were assigned to groups \"afflicted\" by versus \"resilient\" against the unique effect of plasma adipokines using a classifier derived by confirmatory factor analysis in a structural equation model framework. The impact of affliction class above and beyond observed biomarker levels and covariates was tested by multivariate regression using CDR \"Sum of Boxes\" as the dependent variable. The affliction class' moderation of adipokines' effect was tested by chi-square difference. The effect of affliction class on prospective conversion risk was tested by Cox's proportional hazards models.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Seven hundred four out of the 1,737 subjects (40.53%) were assigned to the afflicted class. The afflicted subjects had greater dementia severity, lower (adverse) <i>Adipokines</i> factor composite scores (by analysis of variance, <i>F</i>(1, 1,735) 2619.68, <i>p</i> < .001) and higher observed levels of plasma adipokines (by Tukey's honestly significant difference test, all <i>p</i> < .001). <i>Adipokines</i>' association with dementia severity was moderated by affliction class. The effect persisted at 48 months. Afflicted cases were more likely to convert to Alzheimer's disease in that timeframe, by Cox's <i>F</i>: <i>F</i>(234, 286) = 3.89, <i>p</i> < .001.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our approach could guide precision interventions against specific biomarkers. This classifier could be administered by telephone, making class assignment feasible without direct patient contact or biomarker assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142624954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Timothy Fellin, Jessica Paxton, Adrian Thomas, Susan Torres-Harding
{"title":"Metabolic syndrome and mobility dysfunction in older adults with and without histories of traumatic brain injury: The mediating role of cognition.","authors":"Timothy Fellin, Jessica Paxton, Adrian Thomas, Susan Torres-Harding","doi":"10.1037/neu0000980","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000980","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Older adults are more susceptible than the general population to developing metabolic syndrome (i.e., three or more cardiovascular risk factors [MetS]), physical limitations, and traumatic brain injury (TBI). Recent research has demonstrated that cognition may moderate the negative association between MetS and mobility dysfunction in older adults. This study sought to determine if cognition mediates the relationship between MetS and mobility dysfunction and if this relationship differs in older adults with a history of TBI.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants (<i>N</i> = 20,156) were from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center's Uniform Data Set. Mediation models were tested to assess if processing speed, executive functioning, immediate memory, and delayed memory would independently mediate the association between MetS and mobility dysfunction.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Investigation of mediation models revealed that MetS had a significant indirect effect on mobility dysfunction through processing speed (<i>b</i> = .0674, 95% CI [.0412, .0953]) and executive functioning (<i>b</i> = .0354, 95% CI [.0228, .0493]). When TBI was included in the model as a moderator, MetS was not found to moderate the mediating effects of any of the cognitive variables. There were no significant indirect effects for immediate or delayed memory in either model.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings elucidate potential pathways by which MetS contributes to mobility dysfunction in older adults through specific reductions in processing speed and executive functioning capabilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142605772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Association between social networks and cognition among middle-aged and older adults in rural India.","authors":"Pooja Rai, Jonas S Sundarakumar","doi":"10.1037/neu0000971","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000971","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Prior studies using global cognitive measures have shown that social connectedness is linked with cognitive performance. We investigate the role of different social network dimensions on performance across distinct cognitive domains among dementia-free middle-aged and older rural Indians.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We utilized baseline assessment data of 2,525 participants (≥45 years) of an ongoing, prospective, aging cohort in rural India (Srinivaspura Aging, Neuro Senescence, and COGnition) for this study. We evaluated social networks using Cohen's Social Network Index on three dimensions: network diversity, network size, and network embeddedness. We measured cognitive performance on memory (narrative recall), visuospatial ability (geometric figure spatial recognition, visuospatial span), language (verbal fluency, semantic association, word comprehension, reading comprehension), and attention domains of a culturally adapted, computerized, neurocognitive test battery. Linear regression models, adjusted for age and sex, were used for statistical analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In the linear regression model adjusted for age and sex, we found that greater network diversity was significantly associated with better performance in narrative recall, geometric figure spatial recognition, reading comprehension, semantic association, and attention tests. Participants with a larger network size had significantly better performance in verbal fluency and semantic association tests. Further, participants with greater network embeddedness had better visuospatial span and verbal fluency.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Individuals having a greater network diversity, larger network size, and greater network embeddedness had better cognitive performance in multiple distinct cognitive domains. Following up these participants with serial cognitive monitoring can help understand if social networks play a role in delaying cognitive decline and protecting against dementia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"740-748"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Simonne E Collins, Alice C Burnett, Philippa Pyman, Rheanna M Mainzer, Leona Pascoe, Kristina M Haebich, Jeanie L Y Cheong, Lex W Doyle, Deanne K Thompson, Peter J Anderson
{"title":"Executive function is associated with the development of math performance in children born at <30 weeks' gestation or <1250 g birthweight.","authors":"Simonne E Collins, Alice C Burnett, Philippa Pyman, Rheanna M Mainzer, Leona Pascoe, Kristina M Haebich, Jeanie L Y Cheong, Lex W Doyle, Deanne K Thompson, Peter J Anderson","doi":"10.1037/neu0000970","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000970","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To describe associations between executive function (EF) domains (attentional control, information processing, cognitive flexibility, and goal setting) and concurrent math computation performance at age 7 and 13 years in children born <30 weeks' gestation or weighing <1,250 g, and second, to examine the impact of 7-year EF on math performance at 13 years.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a prospective, longitudinal cohort of children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g, assessment of EF and math performance was undertaken at 7 (n = 187) and 13 years (n = 174). Linear regression models were used to describe associations between EF domains with math performance at both time points, as well as to examine the impact of EF at 7 years on math performance at 13 years.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>At 7 and 13 years, all EF domains were positively and strongly associated with concurrent math performance (β = 11.35, 95% CI [9.28, 13.41] to β = 13.79, 95% CI [11.59, 15.98]). All EF domains at age 7 years were positively associated with math performance at 13 years, with the strongest associations observed for cognitive flexibility (β = 10.79 [8.64, 12.94]) and goal setting (β = 10.37 [8.08, 12.67]).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study provides evidence that EF is strongly associated with math performance in children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g and highlights the importance of early cognitive flexibility and goal setting performance for future math performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"714-726"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Is hyperactivity in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) a functional response to demands on specific executive functions or cognitive demands in general?","authors":"Elia F Soto, Katie Black, Michael J Kofler","doi":"10.1037/neu0000975","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000975","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Hyperactivity is a core and impairing deficit in the clinical model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, the extent to which hyperactivity in ADHD is evoked by cognitively challenging tasks in general or by demands on specific executive functions remains unclear.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A clinically evaluated and carefully phenotyped community-referred sample of 184 children ages 8-13 (M = 10.40, SD = 1.50; 61 girls) with ADHD (n = 119) and without ADHD (neurotypical children and children with psychiatric disorders other than ADHD) were administered multiple, counterbalanced executive (working memory, inhibitory control, set shifting) and nonexecutive tests. Objective measures of gross motor movement (hyperactivity) were obtained using actigraphy.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Using bifactor s-1 modeling, results indicate that children with ADHD demonstrate moderately elevated levels of motor movement relative to non-ADHD children. Additionally, findings indicated that hyperactivity in ADHD reflects the outcome of at least two similarly important factors: (a) a baseline level of elevated motor movement that is independent of environmental demands on their executive and nonexecutive cognitive abilities (d = 0.72); and (b) additional elevations attributable to demands placed on specific executive functions, with working memory and inhibition demands evoking similarly large, differential increases in movement for children with ADHD above and beyond their elevated baselines (Δd = 0.80).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings suggests that executive function demands exacerbate, but do not fully explain, hyperactivity in ADHD, and/or there are at least two pathways to hyperactivity in ADHD-hyperactivity caused by environmental demands that challenge their underdeveloped executive functions, and hyperactivity caused by one or more other factors that need future research to identify. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"699-713"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
NeuropsychologyPub Date : 2024-11-01Epub Date: 2024-09-26DOI: 10.1037/neu0000973
Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy
{"title":"Getting oriented: Redefining attention deficits in Parkinson's disease.","authors":"Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy","doi":"10.1037/neu0000973","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000973","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Parkinson's disease (PD) may affect not only motor functions, but also cognitive processes such as attention. While past research has examined PD impact on spatial attention, it has not addressed how the key functions of attentional orienting and alerting in PD are mediated by cueing format, an ecologically relevant parameter. We assessed how exogenous and endogenous orienting cue modes affect PD patients' visuospatial attention expressed as dorsal attention network orienting benefits, ventral attention network reorienting costs, and alerting abilities.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Ninety PD patients and 72 healthy comparison participants performed a spatial attention task in an engaging game format which required selection of a target location without prior cueing, or with temporal, valid spatial, or invalid spatial exogenous or endogenous cueing.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>PD patients differed from healthy participants only in response time benefits in orienting under endogenous probabilistically predictive cue processing. They did not exhibit greater reorienting costs, differences in inhibition of return, or alerting deficits, irrespective of modes of cueing.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results suggest that fundamental orienting and alerting functions might be intact in PD, with challenges emerging only if additional cognitive processes, including those related to motor preparation, are required to utilize cue information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"749-762"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142350923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg
{"title":"Sociodemographic predictors and cross-cultural comparisons in tests performance from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) among children aged 6-8 years from Montevideo, Uruguay.","authors":"Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg","doi":"10.1037/neu0000966","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000966","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cross-culturally comparative data on measures of executive function (EF) are essential, but the 6-8-year group remains insufficiently described. This study examined the sociodemographic predictors of EF test performance employing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). It also compared developmental trends in EF among children from Uruguay, the United States, and Mexico.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>EFs were assessed with the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift, Spatial Span (SSP), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) tests from the CANTAB. The study sample consisted of 6-8-year-old children from the Salud Ambiental Montevideo (SAM) cohort in Uruguay. Differences between cohorts were examined, and we performed generalized linear regressions to assess the association between sociodemographic factors, and each EF domain.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The final sample consisted of 525 participants (mean age in months 82.5 ± 6.0). Across all ages, SAM children had significantly lower performance in the SSP and SOC tasks compared to U.S. and Mexican children. On the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift task, SAM children had similar scores to U.S. and Mexican children. Mother's intelligence quotient (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02]), child's IQ (0.02 [0.02, 0.03]), the HOME total score (0.02 [0.01, 0.03]), as well as HOME subscales of accompaniment (0.13 [0.07, 0.20]), enrichment (0.11 [0.06,0.16]), and physical environment (0.07 [0.03, 0.10]) were positively associated with the span length (SSP task). Child's IQ (0.02 [0.01,0.03]) was positively associated with the number of problems solved on the SOC test.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Uruguayan children perform lower in working memory and planning tests than U.S. children but similarly to Mexican children, while cognitive flexibility is consistent across all groups. Further, mother and child IQ, as well as the home environment, are important predictors of EF. These differences should be examined in the context of diverse cultural values and sociodemographic factors affecting CANTAB construct validity in this population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"727-739"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546541","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher
{"title":"Corpus callosum structure and auditory interhemispheric transfer in spina bifida myelomeningocele.","authors":"Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher","doi":"10.1037/neu0000915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000915","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Maldevelopment of the posterior corpus callosum is common in spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) due to hydrocephalus-related hypoplasia and congenital partial hypogenesis. This study examined the relations of macro- and microstructural integrity of the interhemispheric temporal tract in SBM and auditory interhemispheric transfer using consonant-vowel dichotic listening.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected <i>T</i>₁-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging data from 46 individuals with SBM and 15 typically developing individuals. Probabilistic tractography was used to isolate the interhemispheric white matter connecting auditory processing regions in both hemispheres. Interhemispheric transfer was assessed with a dichotic listening task.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Although the typically developing group and the group with SBM showed the normative right-ear advantage, fewer participants showed a right-ear advantage in the group with SBM. The absence of the right-ear advantage was largely in the subgroup with hypogenesis of the splenium or severe posterior hypoplasia. Sex, anterior commissure cross-sectional area, and number of shunt pathways visible on magnetic resonance imaging predicted right-ear superiority.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Interhemispheric transfer is disrupted in individuals with SBM and hypogenesis or severe hypoplasia of the posterior corpus callosum. Preservation of interhemispheric transfer is related to expected connections through the posterior corpus callosum and possibly compensatory pathways in the anterior commissure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"687-698"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}