Orma Ravindranath , Maria I. Perica , Ashley C. Parr , Amar Ojha , Shane D. McKeon , Gerald Montano , Naomi Ullendorff , Beatriz Luna , E. Kale Edmiston
{"title":"Adolescent neurocognitive development and decision-making abilities regarding gender-affirming care","authors":"Orma Ravindranath , Maria I. Perica , Ashley C. Parr , Amar Ojha , Shane D. McKeon , Gerald Montano , Naomi Ullendorff , Beatriz Luna , E. Kale Edmiston","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101351","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101351","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recently, politicians and legislative bodies have cited neurodevelopmental literature to argue that brain immaturity undermines decision-making regarding gender-affirming care (GAC) in youth. Here, we review this literature as it applies to adolescents’ ability to make decisions regarding GAC. The research shows that while adolescence is a time of peak risk-taking behavior that may lead to impulsive decisions, neurocognitive systems supporting adult-level decisions are available given deliberative processes that minimize influence of short-term rewards and peers. Since GAC decisions occur over an extended period and with support from adult caregivers and clinicians, adolescents can engage adult-level decision-making in this context. We also weigh the benefits of providing GAC access during adolescence and consider the significant costs of blocking or delaying GAC. Transgender and non-binary (TNB) adolescents face significant mental health challenges, many of which are mitigated by GAC access. Further, initiating the GAC process during adolescence, which we define as beginning at pubertal onset, leads to better long-term mental health outcomes than waiting until adulthood. Taken together, existing research indicates that many adolescents can make informed decisions regarding gender-affirming care, and that this care is critical for the well-being of TNB youth. We highlight relevant considerations for policy makers, researchers, and clinicians.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101351"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000124/pdfft?md5=670a2a5407e04acfc8b1cf4149421e42&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000124-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139871839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Nwabisa Mlandu , Sarah A. McCormick , Lauren Davel , Michal R. Zieff , Layla Bradford , Donna Herr , Chloë A. Jacobs , Anele Khumalo , Candice Knipe , Zamazimba Madi , Thandeka Mazubane , Bokang Methola , Tembeka Mhlakwaphalwa , Marlie Miles , Zayaan Goolam Nabi , Rabelani Negota , Khanyisa Nkubungu , Tracy Pan , Reese Samuels , Sadeeka Williams , Laurel J. Gabard-Durnam
{"title":"Evaluating a novel high-density EEG sensor net structure for improving inclusivity in infants with curly or tightly coiled hair","authors":"Nwabisa Mlandu , Sarah A. McCormick , Lauren Davel , Michal R. Zieff , Layla Bradford , Donna Herr , Chloë A. Jacobs , Anele Khumalo , Candice Knipe , Zamazimba Madi , Thandeka Mazubane , Bokang Methola , Tembeka Mhlakwaphalwa , Marlie Miles , Zayaan Goolam Nabi , Rabelani Negota , Khanyisa Nkubungu , Tracy Pan , Reese Samuels , Sadeeka Williams , Laurel J. Gabard-Durnam","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101396","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101396","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Electroencephalography (EEG) is an important tool in the field of developmental cognitive neuroscience for indexing neural activity. However, racial biases persist in EEG research that limit the utility of this tool. One bias comes from the structure of EEG nets/caps that do not facilitate equitable data collection across hair textures and types. Recent efforts have improved EEG net/cap design, but these solutions can be time-intensive, reduce sensor density, and are more difficult to implement in younger populations. The present study focused on testing EEG sensor net designs over infancy. Specifically, we compared EEG data quality and retention between two high-density saline-based EEG sensor net designs from the same company (Magstim EGI, Whitland, UK) within the same infants during a baseline EEG paradigm. We found that within infants, the tall sensor nets resulted in lower impedances during collection, including lower impedances in the key online reference electrode for those with greater hair heights and resulted in a greater number of usable EEG channels and data segments retained during pre-processing. These results suggest that along with other best practices, the modified tall sensor net design is useful for improving data quality and retention in infant participants with curly or tightly-coiled hair.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101396"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000574/pdfft?md5=8e3a34e7b52c152b70ee46a378ff7da0&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000574-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141184230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
F. Abate , A. Adu-Amankwah , KA Ae-Ngibise , F Agbokey , VA Agyemang , CT Agyemang , C. Akgun , J. Ametepe , T. Arichi , KP Asante , S. Balaji , L. Baljer , PJ Basser , J. Beauchemin , C. Bennallick , Y. Berhane , Y. Boateng-Mensah , NJ Bourke , L. Bradford , MMK Bruchhage , SCR Williams
{"title":"UNITY: A low-field magnetic resonance neuroimaging initiative to characterize neurodevelopment in low and middle-income settings","authors":"F. Abate , A. Adu-Amankwah , KA Ae-Ngibise , F Agbokey , VA Agyemang , CT Agyemang , C. Akgun , J. Ametepe , T. Arichi , KP Asante , S. Balaji , L. Baljer , PJ Basser , J. Beauchemin , C. Bennallick , Y. Berhane , Y. Boateng-Mensah , NJ Bourke , L. Bradford , MMK Bruchhage , SCR Williams","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101397","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101397","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Measures of physical growth, such as weight and height have long been the predominant outcomes for monitoring child health and evaluating interventional outcomes in public health studies, including those that may impact neurodevelopment. While physical growth generally reflects overall health and nutritional status, it lacks sensitivity and specificity to brain growth and developing cognitive skills and abilities. Psychometric tools, e.g., the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, may afford more direct assessment of cognitive development but they require language translation, cultural adaptation, and population norming. Further, they are not always reliable predictors of future outcomes when assessed within the first 12–18 months of a child’s life. Neuroimaging may provide more objective, sensitive, and predictive measures of neurodevelopment but tools such as magnetic resonance (MR) imaging are not readily available in many low and middle-income countries (LMICs). MRI systems that operate at lower magnetic fields (< 100mT) may offer increased accessibility, but their use for global health studies remains nascent. The UNITY project is envisaged as a global partnership to advance neuroimaging in global health studies. Here we describe the UNITY project, its goals, methods, operating procedures, and expected outcomes in characterizing neurodevelopment in sub-Saharan Africa and South Asia.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101397"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000586/pdfft?md5=a2f5990dfb1b95ba95227afcc309a8ff&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000586-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141727933","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jonas G. Miller , Peter D. Gluckman , Marielle V. Fortier , Yap Seng Chong , Michael J. Meaney , Ai Peng Tan , Ian H. Gotlib
{"title":"Faster pace of hippocampal growth mediates the association between perinatal adversity and childhood depression","authors":"Jonas G. Miller , Peter D. Gluckman , Marielle V. Fortier , Yap Seng Chong , Michael J. Meaney , Ai Peng Tan , Ian H. Gotlib","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101392","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101392","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Early life adversity has been posited to influence the pace of structural neurodevelopment. Most research, however, has relied on cross-sectional data, which do not reveal whether the pace of neurodevelopmental change is accelerated or slowed following early exposures. In a birth cohort study that included neuroimaging data obtained at 4.5, 6, and 7.5 years of age (N = 784), we examined associations among a cumulative measure of perinatal adversity relative to resources, nonlinear trajectories of hippocampal and amygdala volume, and children’s subsequent depressive symptoms at 8.5 years of age. Greater adversity was associated with reduced bilateral hippocampal body volume in early childhood, but also to faster growth in the right hippocampal body across childhood. Further, the association between adversity and childhood depressive symptoms was mediated by faster hippocampal body growth. These findings suggest that perinatal adversity is biologically embedded in hippocampal structure development, including an accelerated pace of change in the right hippocampal body that is implicated in children’s psychopathology risk. In addition, our findings suggest that reduced hippocampal volume is not inconsistent with accelerated hippocampal change; these aspects of structural development may typically co-occur, as smaller regional volumes in early childhood were associated with faster growth across childhood.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101392"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000537/pdfft?md5=c208fdb20dde7ce4e417c95b65749c5d&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000537-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140952226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Clarifying the longitudinal factor structure, temporal stability, and construct validity of Go/No-Go task-related neural activation across adolescence and young adulthood","authors":"K.J. Paige , C.R. Colder , L.M. Cope , J.E. Hardee , M.M. Heitzeg , M.E. Soules , A.S. Weigard","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101390","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study aimed to clarify the psychometric properties and development of Go/No-Go (GNG) task-related neural activation across critical periods of neurobiological maturation by examining its longitudinal stability, factor structure, developmental change, and associations with a computational index of task-general cognitive control. A longitudinal sample (<em>N</em>=289) of adolescents from the Michigan Longitudinal Study was assessed at four time-points (mean number of timepoints per participant=2.05; standard deviation=0.89) spanning early adolescence (ages 10−13) to young adulthood (22−25). Results suggested that regional neural activations from the “successful inhibition” (SI>GO) and “failed inhibition” (FI>GO; error-monitoring) contrasts are each described well by a single general factor. Neural activity across both contrasts showed developmental increases throughout adolescence that plateau in young adulthood. Neural activity metrics evidenced low temporal stability across this period of marked developmental change, and the SI>GO factor showed no relations with a behavioral index of cognitive control. The FI>GO factor displayed stronger criterion validity in the form of significant, positive associations with behaviorally measured cognitive control. Findings emphasize the utility of well-validated psychometric methods and longitudinal data for clarifying the measurement properties of functional neuroimaging metrics and improving measurement practices in developmental cognitive neuroscience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101390"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000513/pdfft?md5=7ffeefd86b21d8ea4c906ef607e97f11&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000513-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140952224","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Yvette Grootjans , Anita Harrewijn , Laura Fornari , Tieme Janssen , Ellen R.A. de Bruijn , Nienke van Atteveldt , Ingmar H.A. Franken
{"title":"Getting closer to social interactions using electroencephalography in developmental cognitive neuroscience","authors":"Yvette Grootjans , Anita Harrewijn , Laura Fornari , Tieme Janssen , Ellen R.A. de Bruijn , Nienke van Atteveldt , Ingmar H.A. Franken","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101391","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101391","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The field of developmental cognitive neuroscience is advancing rapidly, with large-scale, population-wide, longitudinal studies emerging as a key means of unraveling the complexity of the developing brain and cognitive processes in children. While numerous neuroscientific techniques like functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), magnetoencephalography (MEG), and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have proved advantageous in such investigations, this perspective proposes a renewed focus on electroencephalography (EEG), leveraging underexplored possibilities of EEG. In addition to its temporal precision, low costs, and ease of application, EEG distinguishes itself with its ability to capture neural activity linked to social interactions in increasingly ecologically valid settings. Specifically, EEG can be measured during social interactions in the lab, hyperscanning can be used to study brain activity in two (or more) people simultaneously, and mobile EEG can be used to measure brain activity in real-life settings. This perspective paper summarizes research in these three areas, making a persuasive argument for the renewed inclusion of EEG into the toolkit of developmental cognitive and social neuroscientists.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101391"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000525/pdfft?md5=fa67bdf05128fc00663a84bbc8e40535&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000525-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140952225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jodi M. Gilman , Jasmeen Kaur , Brenden Tervo-Clemmens , Kevin Potter , Brandon T. Sanzo , Randi M. Schuster , James M. Bjork , A. Eden Evins , Joshua L. Roffman , Phil H. Lee
{"title":"Associations between behavioral and self-reported impulsivity, brain structure, and genetic influences in middle childhood","authors":"Jodi M. Gilman , Jasmeen Kaur , Brenden Tervo-Clemmens , Kevin Potter , Brandon T. Sanzo , Randi M. Schuster , James M. Bjork , A. Eden Evins , Joshua L. Roffman , Phil H. Lee","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101389","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101389","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Impulsivity undergoes a normative developmental trajectory from childhood to adulthood and is thought to be driven by maturation of brain structure. However, few large-scale studies have assessed associations between impulsivity, brain structure, and genetic susceptibility in children. In 9112 children ages 9–10 from the ABCD study, we explored relationships among impulsivity (UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale; delay discounting), brain structure (cortical thickness (CT), cortical volume (CV), and cortical area (CA)), and polygenic scores for externalizing behavior (PGS<sub>EXT</sub>). Both higher UPPS-P total scores and more severe delay-discounting had widespread, low-magnitude associations with smaller CA in frontal and temporal regions. No associations were seen between impulsivity and CV or CT. Additionally, higher PGS<sub>EXT</sub> was associated with both higher UPPS-P scores and with smaller CA and CV in frontal and temporal regions, but in non-overlapping cortical regions, underscoring the complex interplay between genetics and brain structure in influencing impulsivity. These findings indicate that, within large-scale population data, CA is significantly yet weakly associated with each of these impulsivity measures and with polygenic risk for externalizing behaviors, but in distinct brain regions. Future work should longitudinally assess these associations through adolescence, and examine associated functional outcomes, such as future substance use and psychopathology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101389"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000501/pdfft?md5=c52ba45f395280ab0d6684082b00ce5a&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000501-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140946349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Elizabeth A. Hodgdon , Ryan Anderson , Hussein Al Azzawi , Tony W. Wilson , Vince D. Calhoun , Yu-Ping Wang , Isabel Solis , Douglas N. Greve , Julia M. Stephen , Kristina T.R. Ciesielski
{"title":"MRI morphometry of the anterior and posterior cerebellar vermis and its relationship to sensorimotor and cognitive functions in children","authors":"Elizabeth A. Hodgdon , Ryan Anderson , Hussein Al Azzawi , Tony W. Wilson , Vince D. Calhoun , Yu-Ping Wang , Isabel Solis , Douglas N. Greve , Julia M. Stephen , Kristina T.R. Ciesielski","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101385","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101385","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>The human cerebellum emerges as a posterior brain structure integrating neural networks for sensorimotor, cognitive, and emotional processing across the lifespan. Developmental studies of the cerebellar anatomy and function are scant. We examine age-dependent MRI morphometry of the <em>anterior cerebellar vermis</em>, lobules I-V and <em>posterior neocortical lobules</em> VI-VII and their relationship to sensorimotor and cognitive functions.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Typically developing children (TDC; n=38; age 9–15) and healthy adults (HAC; n=31; 18–40) participated in high-resolution MRI. Rigorous anatomically informed morphometry of the vermis lobules I-V and VI-VII and total brain volume (TBV) employed manual segmentation computer-assisted FreeSurfer Image Analysis Program [<span>http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu</span><svg><path></path></svg>]. The neuropsychological scores (WASI-II) were normalized and related to volumes of anterior, posterior vermis, and TBV.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>TBVs were age independent. Volumes of I-V and VI-VII were significantly reduced in TDC. The ratio of VI-VII to I-V (∼60%) was stable across age-groups; I-V correlated with visual-spatial-motor skills; VI-VII with verbal, visual-abstract and FSIQ.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>In TDC neither anterior I-V nor posterior VI-VII vermis attained adult volumes. The <em>\"inverted U\"</em> developmental trajectory of gray matter peaking in adolescence does not explain this finding. The hypothesis of protracted development of oligodendrocyte/myelination is suggested as a contributor to TDC's lower cerebellar vermis volumes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101385"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S187892932400046X/pdfft?md5=9357f0a9eb74d438fdcb73bfc966bf81&pid=1-s2.0-S187892932400046X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140774283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Lorena Jiménez-Sánchez , Manuel Blesa Cábez , Kadi Vaher , Amy Corrigan , Michael J. Thrippleton , Mark E. Bastin , Alan J. Quigley , Sue Fletcher-Watson , James P. Boardman
{"title":"Infant attachment does not depend on neonatal amygdala and hippocampal structure and connectivity","authors":"Lorena Jiménez-Sánchez , Manuel Blesa Cábez , Kadi Vaher , Amy Corrigan , Michael J. Thrippleton , Mark E. Bastin , Alan J. Quigley , Sue Fletcher-Watson , James P. Boardman","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101387","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101387","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Infant attachment is an antecedent of later socioemotional abilities, which can be adversely affected by preterm birth. The structural integrity of amygdalae and hippocampi may subserve attachment in infancy. We aimed to investigate associations between neonatal amygdalae and hippocampi structure and their whole-brain connections and attachment behaviours at nine months of age in a sample of infants enriched for preterm birth. In 133 neonates (median gestational age 32 weeks, range 22.14–42.14), we calculated measures of amygdala and hippocampal structure (volume, fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity, neurite dispersion index, orientation dispersion index) and structural connectivity, and coded attachment behaviours (distress, fretfulness, attentiveness to caregiver) from responses to the Still-Face Paradigm at nine months. After multiple comparisons correction, there were no significant associations between neonatal amygdala or hippocampal structure and structural connectivity and attachment behaviours: standardised β values − 0.23 to 0.18, adjusted p-values > 0.40. Findings indicate that the neural basis of infant attachment in term and preterm infants is not contingent on the structure or connectivity of the amygdalae and hippocampi in the neonatal period, which implies that it is more widely distributed in early life and or that network specialisation takes place in the months after hospital discharge.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101387"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000483/pdfft?md5=07b54396519d99049946b50150a15db2&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000483-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140815511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Ira Marriot Haresign, Emily A.M., Phillips, Sam V., Wass
{"title":"Why behaviour matters: Studying inter-brain coordination during child-caregiver interaction","authors":"Ira Marriot Haresign, Emily A.M., Phillips, Sam V., Wass","doi":"10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101384","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101384","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Modern technology allows for simultaneous neuroimaging from interacting caregiver-child dyads. Whereas most analyses that examine the coordination between brain regions within an individual brain do so by measuring changes relative to observed events, studies that examine coordination between two interacting brains generally do this by measuring average intra-brain coordination across entire blocks or experimental conditions. In other words, they do not examine changes in inter-brain coordination relative to individual behavioural events. Here, we discuss the limitations of this approach. First, we present data suggesting that fine-grained temporal interdependencies in behaviour can leave residual artifact in neuroimaging data. We show how artifact can manifest as both power and (through that) phase synchrony effects in EEG and affect wavelet transform coherence in fNIRS analyses. Second, we discuss different possible mechanistic explanations of how inter-brain coordination is established and maintained. We argue that non-event-locked approaches struggle to differentiate between them. Instead, we contend that approaches which examine how interpersonal dynamics change around behavioural events have better potential for addressing possible artifactual confounds and for teasing apart the overlapping mechanisms that drive changes in inter-brain coordination.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49083,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"67 ","pages":"Article 101384"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1878929324000458/pdfft?md5=5aea8d4e5cb21a904bca8f026c858807&pid=1-s2.0-S1878929324000458-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140638054","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}