Developmental Science最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Longitudinal links from attachment with mothers and fathers to adolescent substance use: Internalizing and externalizing pathways 从与母亲和父亲的依恋到青少年药物使用的纵向联系:内化和外化途径
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13539
Claudia Clinchard, Kirby Deater-Deckard, Brooks Casas, Jungmeen Kim-Spoon
{"title":"Longitudinal links from attachment with mothers and fathers to adolescent substance use: Internalizing and externalizing pathways","authors":"Claudia Clinchard,&nbsp;Kirby Deater-Deckard,&nbsp;Brooks Casas,&nbsp;Jungmeen Kim-Spoon","doi":"10.1111/desc.13539","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13539","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The present study examined whether internalizing and externalizing symptoms may mediate the association between adolescent–mother and adolescent–father attachment and substance use. The sample included 167 adolescents (47% girls) who were assessed at five time points with approximately 1 year between each assessment, beginning in middle adolescence (<i>M</i><sub>age </sub>= 14.07) and ending in the transition to young adulthood (<i>M</i><sub>age </sub>= 18.39). The adolescents reported their perceived attachment with both their mother and father during middle adolescence (Times 1 and 2), their internalizing and externalizing symptoms during late adolescence (Times 3 and 4), and their alcohol use during the transition to young adulthood (Time 5). The results showed that less secure adolescent–father attachment, but not adolescent–mother attachment, was predictive of heightened externalizing and internalizing symptoms. In turn, heightened externalizing symptoms were predictive of heightened alcohol use. Despite the nonsignificant direct association between adolescent–father attachment and alcohol use, less secure adolescent–father attachment was indirectly predictive of greater alcohol use, mediated through heightened externalizing symptoms. The findings highlight the importance of close and trusting father–adolescent relationships in the development of psychopathology and substance use behaviors. The developmental cascade from a less secure adolescent–father attachment to greater externalizing symptoms and heightened substance use, as well as implications for prevention and intervention of young adult substance use, are discussed.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The differential pathways from adolescent–mother and adolescent–father attachment to substance use during the transition to young adulthood are not well known.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Longitudinal data were used to test whether internalizing and externalizing symptoms may mediate the association between adolescent–mother and adolescent–father attachment and substance use.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Less secure adolescent–father attachment predicted heightened internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and less secure adolescent–father attachment predicted greater alcohol use, mediated through heightened externalizing symptoms.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The findings suggest that addressing insecure attachment with fathers during adolescence may reduce unhealthy substance use during the transition to young adulthood.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13539","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141503879","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High consistency of cheating and honesty in early childhood 幼儿期的欺骗和诚实行为高度一致。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13540
Yi Zheng, Kang Lee, Li Zhao
{"title":"High consistency of cheating and honesty in early childhood","authors":"Yi Zheng,&nbsp;Kang Lee,&nbsp;Li Zhao","doi":"10.1111/desc.13540","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13540","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Three preregistered studies examined whether 5-year-old children cheat consistently or remain honest across multiple math tests. We observed high consistency in both honesty and cheating. All children who cheated on the first test continued cheating on subsequent tests, with shorter cheating latencies over time. In contrast, 77% of initially honest children maintained honesty despite repeated failure to complete the tests successfully. A brief integrity intervention helped initially honest children remain honest but failed to dissuade initially cheating children from cheating. These findings demonstrate that cheating emerges early and persists strongly in young children, underscoring the importance of early prevention efforts. They also suggest that bolstering honesty from the start may be more effective than attempting to remedy cheating after it has occurred.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Our research examines whether 5-year-old children, once they have started cheating, will continue to do so consistently.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We also investigate whether 5-year-old children who are initially honest will continue to be honest subsequently.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We discovered high consistency in both honesty and cheating among 5-year-old children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Almost all the children who initially cheated continued this behavior, while those who were honest stayed honest.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A brief integrity-boosting intervention successfully helped 5-year-old children maintain their honesty. However, the same intervention failed to deter cheaters from cheating again.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>These findings underscore the importance of implementing integrity intervention as early as possible, potentially before children have had their first experience of cheating.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in internalizing symptoms in late childhood: A variance decomposition into cortical thickness, genetic and environmental differences 儿童晚期内化症状的个体差异:皮层厚度、遗传和环境差异的方差分解。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13537
Anneli D. Tandberg, Andreas Dahl, Linn B. Norbom, Lars T. Westlye, Eivind Ystrom, Christian K. Tamnes, Espen M. Eilertsen
{"title":"Individual differences in internalizing symptoms in late childhood: A variance decomposition into cortical thickness, genetic and environmental differences","authors":"Anneli D. Tandberg,&nbsp;Andreas Dahl,&nbsp;Linn B. Norbom,&nbsp;Lars T. Westlye,&nbsp;Eivind Ystrom,&nbsp;Christian K. Tamnes,&nbsp;Espen M. Eilertsen","doi":"10.1111/desc.13537","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13537","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The brain undergoes extensive development during late childhood and early adolescence. Cortical thinning is a prominent feature of this development, and some researchers have suggested that differences in cortical thickness may be related to internalizing symptoms, which typically increase during the same period. However, research has yielded inconclusive results. We utilized a new method that estimates the combined effect of individual differences in vertex-wise cortical thickness on internalizing symptoms. This approach allows for many small effects to be distributed across the cortex and avoids the necessity of correcting for multiple tests. Using a sample of 8763 children aged 8.9 to 11.1 from the ABCD study, we decomposed the total variation in caregiver-reported internalizing symptoms into differences in cortical thickness, additive genetics, and shared family environmental factors and unique environmental factors. Our results indicated that individual differences in cortical thickness accounted for less than 0.5% of the variation in internalizing symptoms. In contrast, the analysis revealed a substantial effect of additive genetics and family environmental factors on the different components of internalizing symptoms, ranging from 06% to 48% and from 0% to 34%, respectively. Overall, while this study found a minimal association between cortical thickness and internalizing symptoms, additive genetics, and familial environmental factors appear to be of importance for describing differences in internalizing symptoms in late childhood.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We utilized a new method for modelling the total contribution of vertex-wise individual differences in cortical thickness to internalizing symptoms in late childhood.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The total contribution of individual differences in cortical thickness accounted for &lt;0.5% of the variance in internalizing symptoms.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Additive genetics and shared family environmental variation accounted for 17% and 34% of the variance in internalizing symptoms, respectively.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Our results suggest that cortical thickness is not an important indicator for internalizing symptoms in childhood, whereas genetic and environmental differences have a substantial impact.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13537","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141318588","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensitivity to psychosocial influences at age 3 predicts mental health in middle childhood 3 岁时对社会心理影响的敏感性可预测中年期的心理健康。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13531
Cassidy L. McDermott, Katherine Taylor, Sophie D. S. Sharp, David Lydon-Staley, Julia A. Leonard, Allyson P. Mackey
{"title":"Sensitivity to psychosocial influences at age 3 predicts mental health in middle childhood","authors":"Cassidy L. McDermott,&nbsp;Katherine Taylor,&nbsp;Sophie D. S. Sharp,&nbsp;David Lydon-Staley,&nbsp;Julia A. Leonard,&nbsp;Allyson P. Mackey","doi":"10.1111/desc.13531","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13531","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Children vary in how sensitive they are to experiences, with consequences for their developmental outcomes. In the current study, we investigated how behavioral sensitivity at age 3 years predicts mental health in middle childhood. Using a novel repeated measures design, we calculated child sensitivity to multiple psychological and social influences: parent praise, parent stress, child mood, and child sleep. We conceptualized sensitivity as the strength and direction of the relationship between psychosocial influences and child behavior, operationalized as toothbrushing time, at age 3 years. When children were 5–7 years old (<i>n</i> = 60), parents reported on children's internalizing and externalizing problems. Children who were more sensitive to their parents’ praise at age 3 had fewer internalizing (<i>r</i> = −0.37, <i>p</i> = 0.016, <i>p<sub>FDR</sub></i> = 0.042) and externalizing (<i>r</i> = −0.35, <i>p</i> = 0.021, <i>p<sub>FDR</sub></i> = 0.042) problems in middle childhood. Higher average parent praise also marginally predicted fewer externalizing problems (<i>r</i> = −0.33, <i>p</i> = 0.006, <i>p<sub>FDR</sub></i> = 0.057). Child sensitivity to mood predicted fewer internalizing (<i>r</i> = −0.32, <i>p</i> = 0.013, <i>p<sub>FDR</sub></i> = 0.042) and externalizing (<i>r</i> = −0.38, <i>p</i> = 0.003, <i>p<sub>FDR</sub></i> = 0.026) problems. By capturing variability in how children respond to daily fluctuations in their environment, we can contribute to the early prediction of mental health problems and improve access to early intervention services for children and families who need them most.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children differ in how strongly their behavior depends on psychosocial factors including parent praise, child mood, child sleep, and parent stress.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children who are more sensitive to their parents’ praise at age 3 have fewer internalizing and externalizing problems at age 5–7 years.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Child sensitivity to mood also predicts fewer internalizing and externalizing problems.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13531","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141307185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct mechanisms for online and offline motor skill learning across human development 人类发育过程中在线和离线运动技能学习的不同机制
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13536
Mikkel Malling Beck, Frederikke Toft Kristensen, Gitte Abrahamsen, Meaghan Elizabeth Spedden, Mark Schram Christensen, Jesper Lundbye-Jensen
{"title":"Distinct mechanisms for online and offline motor skill learning across human development","authors":"Mikkel Malling Beck,&nbsp;Frederikke Toft Kristensen,&nbsp;Gitte Abrahamsen,&nbsp;Meaghan Elizabeth Spedden,&nbsp;Mark Schram Christensen,&nbsp;Jesper Lundbye-Jensen","doi":"10.1111/desc.13536","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13536","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The human central nervous system (CNS) undergoes tremendous changes from childhood to adulthood and this may affect how individuals at different stages of development learn new skills. Here, we studied motor skill learning in children, adolescents, and young adults to test the prediction that differences in the maturation of different learning mechanisms lead to distinct temporal patterns of motor learning during practice and overnight. We found that overall learning did not differ between children, adolescents, and young adults. However, we demonstrate that adult-like skill learning is characterized by rapid and large improvements in motor performance during practice (i.e., online) that are susceptible to forgetting and decay over time (i.e., offline). On the other hand, child-like learning exhibits slower and less pronounced improvements in performance during practice, but these improvements are robust against forgetting and lead to gains in performance overnight without further practice. The different temporal dynamics of motor skill learning suggest an engagement of distinct learning mechanisms in the human CNS during development. In conclusion, adult-like skill learning mechanisms favor online improvements in motor performance whereas child-like learning mechanisms favors offline behavioral gains.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Many essential motor skills, like walking, talking, and writing, are acquired during childhood, and it is colloquially thought that children learn better than adults.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We investigated dynamics of motor skill learning in children, adolescents, and young adults.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Adults displayed substantial improvements during practice that was susceptible to forgetting over time. Children displayed smaller improvements during practice that were resilient against forgetting.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The distinct age-related characteristics of these processes of acquisition and consolidation suggest that skill learning relies on different mechanisms in the immature and mature central nervous system.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13536","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141312040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants’ abilities to segment word forms from spectrally degraded speech in the first year of life 婴儿出生后第一年从频谱降低的语音中分辨词形的能力。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-09 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13533
Irene de la Cruz-Pavía, Monica Hegde, Laurianne Cabrera, Thierry Nazzi
{"title":"Infants’ abilities to segment word forms from spectrally degraded speech in the first year of life","authors":"Irene de la Cruz-Pavía,&nbsp;Monica Hegde,&nbsp;Laurianne Cabrera,&nbsp;Thierry Nazzi","doi":"10.1111/desc.13533","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13533","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Infants begin to segment word forms from fluent speech—a crucial task in lexical processing—between 4 and 7 months of age. Prior work has established that infants rely on a variety of cues available in the speech signal (i.e., prosodic, statistical, acoustic-segmental, and lexical) to accomplish this task. In two experiments with French-learning 6- and 10-month-olds, we use a psychoacoustic approach to examine if and how degradation of the two fundamental acoustic components extracted from speech by the auditory system, namely, temporal (both frequency and amplitude modulation) and spectral information, impact word form segmentation. Infants were familiarized with passages containing target words, in which frequency modulation (FM) information was replaced with pure tones using a vocoder, while amplitude modulation (AM) was preserved in either 8 or 16 spectral bands. Infants were then tested on their recognition of the target versus novel control words. While the 6-month-olds were unable to segment in either condition, the 10-month-olds succeeded, although only in the 16 spectral band condition. These findings suggest that 6-month-olds need FM temporal cues for speech segmentation while 10-month-olds do not, although they need the AM cues to be presented in enough spectral bands (i.e., 16). This developmental change observed in infants’ sensitivity to spectrotemporal cues likely results from an increase in the range of available segmentation procedures, and/or shift from a vowel to a consonant bias in lexical processing between the two ages, as vowels are more affected by our acoustic manipulations.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Although segmenting speech into word forms is crucial for lexical acquisition, the acoustic information that infants’ auditory system extracts to process continuous speech remains unknown.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We examined infants’ sensitivity to spectrotemporal cues in speech segmentation using vocoded speech, and revealed a developmental change between 6 and 10 months of age.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We showed that FM information, that is, the fast temporal modulations of speech, is necessary for 6- but not 10-month-old infants to segment word forms.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Moreover, reducing the number of spectral bands impacts 10-month-olds’ segmentation abilities, who succeed when 16 bands are preserved, but fail with 8 bands.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13533","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141297084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's gender essentialism and prejudice: Testing causal links via an experimental manipulation 儿童的性别本质论和偏见:通过实验操作测试因果关系。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13532
E. B. Gross, Rachel D. Fine, Selin Gülgöz, Kristina R. Olson, Susan A. Gelman
{"title":"Children's gender essentialism and prejudice: Testing causal links via an experimental manipulation","authors":"E. B. Gross,&nbsp;Rachel D. Fine,&nbsp;Selin Gülgöz,&nbsp;Kristina R. Olson,&nbsp;Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1111/desc.13532","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13532","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;div&gt;\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 &lt;section&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;p&gt;Despite increases in visibility, gender-nonconforming young people continue to be at risk for bullying and discrimination. Prior work has established that gender essentialism in children correlates with prejudice against people who do not conform to gender norms, but to date no causal link has been established. The present study investigated this link more directly by testing whether children's gender essentialism and prejudice against gender nonconformity can be reduced by exposure to anti-essentialist messaging. Children ages 6–10 years of age (&lt;i&gt;N &lt;/i&gt;= 102) in the experimental condition viewed a short video describing similarities between boys and girls and variation within each gender; children in the control condition (&lt;i&gt;N &lt;/i&gt;= 102) viewed a corresponding video describing similarities between two types of climate and variation within each. Children then received measures of gender essentialism and prejudice against gender nonconformity. Finally, to ask whether manipulating children's gender essentialism extends to another domain, we included assessments of racial essentialism and prejudice. We found positive correlations between gender essentialism and prejudice against gender nonconformity; both also correlated negatively with participant age. However, we observed no differences between children in the experimental versus control conditions in overall essentialism or prejudice, indicating that our video was largely ineffective in manipulating essentialism. Accordingly, we were unable to provide evidence of a causal relationship between essentialism and prejudice. We did, however, see a difference between conditions on the discreteness measure, which is most closely linked to the wording in the video. This finding suggests that specific aspects of essentialism in young children may be modifiable.&lt;/p&gt;\u0000 &lt;/section&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;section&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;h3&gt; Research Highlights&lt;/h3&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;div&gt;\u0000 &lt;ul&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;li&gt;Consistent with prior research, we found that greater gender essentialism was associated with greater prejudice against gender-nonconforming children; both decreased with age.&lt;/li&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;li&gt;We randomly assigned children to view either an anti-essentialist video manipulation or a control video to test if this relation was causal in nature.&lt;/li&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;li&gt;The anti-essentialist video did not reduce overall essentialism as compared to the control, so we did not find support for a causal link.&lt;/li&gt;\u0000 \u0000 &lt;li&gt;We observed a reduction in the dimension of essentialism most closely linked to the anti-essentialist video language, suggesting the potential utility of anti-essentialist messaging.&lt;/li&gt;\u0000 &lt;/ul&gt;\u0000 ","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13532","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On executive functioning and childcare: The moderating role of parent–child interactions 关于执行功能和儿童保育:亲子互动的调节作用。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13534
Annie Bernier, Sylvana M. Côté, Rose Lapolice Thériault, Gabrielle Leclerc
{"title":"On executive functioning and childcare: The moderating role of parent–child interactions","authors":"Annie Bernier,&nbsp;Sylvana M. Côté,&nbsp;Rose Lapolice Thériault,&nbsp;Gabrielle Leclerc","doi":"10.1111/desc.13534","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13534","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Childcare services are widely used by families and thereby exert an important influence on many young children. Yet, little research has examined whether childcare may impact the development of child executive functioning (EF), one of the pillars of cognitive development in early childhood. Furthermore, despite persisting hypotheses that childcare may be particularly beneficial for children who have less access to optimal developmental resources at home, research has yet to address the possibility that putative associations between childcare and EF may vary as a function of family factors. Among a sample of 180 mostly White middle-class families (91 girls), we examined if childcare participation in infancy was related to two aspects of EF (Delay and Conflict) at 3 years, and whether two aspects of maternal parenting behavior (sensitivity and autonomy support) moderated these associations. The results showed positive associations between participation in group-based childcare and Delay EF specifically among children of relatively less autonomy-supportive mothers. These findings suggest that out-of-home childcare services may play a protective role for children exposed to parenting that is less conducive to their executive development.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Little research has considered effects of childcare in infancy on executive functioning (EF).</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Long-standing hypothesis that childcare is more beneficial for children exposed to less sensitive and supportive parenting.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We test interactions between maternal parenting and childcare participation in infancy in relation to EF at age 3 years.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We find positive associations between participation in group-based childcare and Delay EF specifically among children of relatively less autonomy-supportive mothers.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13534","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141176682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beliefs about social mobility in young American children 美国幼儿对社会流动性的看法。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13527
Yuchen Tian, Gorana T. González, Tara M. Mandalaywala
{"title":"Beliefs about social mobility in young American children","authors":"Yuchen Tian,&nbsp;Gorana T. González,&nbsp;Tara M. Mandalaywala","doi":"10.1111/desc.13527","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13527","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Although actual experiences of upward social mobility are historically low, many adolescents and adults express a <i>belief</i> in social mobility (e.g., that social status can change). Although a belief in upward mobility (e.g., that status can improve) can be helpful for economically disadvantaged adolescents and adults, a belief in upward social mobility in adults is also associated with greater acceptance of societal inequality. While this belief might have similar benefits or consequences in children, no previous work has examined whether children are even capable of reasoning about social mobility. This is surprising, given that elementary-aged children exhibit sophisticated reasoning about both social status, as well as about the fixedness or malleability of properties and group membership. Across an economically advantaged group of 5- to 12-year-old American children (<i>N</i> = 151, <i>M<sub>age</sub></i> = 8.91, 63% racial majority, 25% racially marginalized; <i>M</i><sub>household income </sub>= $133,064), we found evidence that children can reason about social mobility for their own families and for others. Similar to research in adults, children believe that others are more likely to experience upward than downward mobility. However, in contrast to adult's typical beliefs—but in line with economic realities—between 7- and 9-years-old, children become less likely to expect upward mobility for economically disadvantaged, versus advantaged, families. In sum, children are capable of reasoning about social mobility in nuanced ways; future work should explore the implications of these beliefs.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Despite harsh economic realities, a belief in upward social mobility and the American Dream is alive and well.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Between 7 and 9 years of age, economically advantaged, American children begin to expect economically disadvantaged families to experience less upward mobility than economically advantaged families.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children's beliefs about social mobility better accord with reality than adults’ do.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparison of speech and music input in North American infants’ home environment over the first 2 years of life 比较北美婴儿出生后头两年在家庭环境中的语音和音乐输入。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学
Developmental Science Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13528
Lindsay Hippe, Victoria Hennessy, Naja Ferjan Ramirez, T. Christina Zhao
{"title":"Comparison of speech and music input in North American infants’ home environment over the first 2 years of life","authors":"Lindsay Hippe,&nbsp;Victoria Hennessy,&nbsp;Naja Ferjan Ramirez,&nbsp;T. Christina Zhao","doi":"10.1111/desc.13528","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13528","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Infants are immersed in a world of sounds from the moment their auditory system becomes functional, and experience with the auditory world shapes how their brain processes sounds in their environment. Across cultures, speech and music are two dominant auditory signals in infants’ daily lives. Decades of research have repeatedly shown that both quantity and quality of speech input play critical roles in infant language development. Less is known about the music input infants receive in their environment. This study is the first to compare music input to speech input across infancy by analyzing a longitudinal dataset of daylong audio recordings collected in English-learning infants’ home environments, at 6, 10, 14, 18, and 24 months of age. Using a crowdsourcing approach, 643 naïve listeners annotated 12,000 short snippets (10 s) randomly sampled from the recordings using Zooniverse, an online citizen-science platform. Results show that infants overall receive significantly more speech input than music input and the gap widens as the infants get older. At every age point, infants were exposed to more music from an electronic device than an in-person source; this pattern was reversed for speech. The percentage of input intended for infants remained the same over time for music while that percentage significantly increased for speech. We propose possible explanations for the limited music input compared to speech input observed in the present (North American) dataset and discuss future directions. We also discuss the opportunities and caveats in using a crowdsourcing approach to analyze large audio datasets. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/lFj_sEaBMN4</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study is the first to compare music input to speech input in infants’ natural home environment across infancy.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We utilized a crowdsourcing approach to annotate a longitudinal dataset of daylong audio recordings collected in North American home environments.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Our main results show that infants overall receive significantly more speech input than music input. This gap widens as the infants get older.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Our results also showed that the music input was largely from electronic devices and not intended for the infants, a pattern opposite to speech input.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141072019","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信