Cognitive Neuroscience最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Examination of the Neural Responses to Emotional and Movement-Related Images. 对情绪和运动相关图像的神经反应的事件相关电位(ERP)检查。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597
Christine E MacKay, Amy S Desroches, Stephen D Smith
{"title":"An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Examination of the Neural Responses to Emotional and Movement-Related Images.","authors":"Christine E MacKay, Amy S Desroches, Stephen D Smith","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has suggested that the perception of emotional images may also activate brain regions related to the preparation of motoric plans. However, little research has investigated whether these emotion-movement interactions occur at early or later stages of visual perception. In the current research, event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to examine the time course of the independent - and combined - effects of perceiving emotions and implied movement. Twenty-five participants viewed images from four categories: 1) emotional with implied movement, 2) emotional with no implied movement, 3) neutral with implied movement, and 4) neutral with no implied movement. Both emotional stimuli and movement-related stimuli led to larger N200 (200-300 ms) waveforms. Furthermore, at frontal sites, there was a marginal interaction between emotion and implied movement, such that negative stimuli showed greater N200 amplitudes vs. neutral stimuli, but only for images with implied movement. At posterior sites, a similar effect was observed for images without implied movement. The late positive potential (LPP; 500-1000 ms) was significant for emotion (at frontal sites) and movement (at frontal, central, and posterior sites), as well as for their interaction (at parietal sites), with larger LPPs for negative vs. neutral images with movement only. Together, these results suggest that the perception of emotion and movement interact at later stages of visual perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139740585","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
TMS must not harm participants: guidelines for evaluating TMS protocol safety. TMS不得伤害参与者:TMS协议安全性评估指南。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553
Scott D Slotnick
{"title":"TMS must not harm participants: guidelines for evaluating TMS protocol safety.","authors":"Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can modulate a targeted brain region to assess whether that region is involved in a cognitive process. When TMS is employed in cognitive neuroscience, participants are typically healthy volunteers, and the technique is described as noninvasive. However, TMS parameters can be set such that stimulation produces long-lasting effects. Critically, TMS effects that have any possibility of lasting beyond a participant's time in the lab are potentially harmful. In this editorial, evidence is considered that indicates a 20-Hz multi-day TMS protocol has long-lasting effects, and a continuous theta-burst stimulation protocol needs further testing before it is deemed noninvasive. The following guidelines are provided for TMS protocol evaluation: 1) Effects must be shown to completely dissipate before participants leave the lab by testing well beyond the expected duration. 2) Participants should complete a cognitive test battery before TMS and after the effects are expected to dissipate. 3) Protocols should not be employed that produce effects lasting longer than the time in the lab. 4) The number of participants should ensure error bars are small, and results generalize to the population. 5) Results should be assessed at the group and individual-participant level, and effects should dissipate for every participant. 6) Bayesian analysis should be conducted to evaluate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. 7) Effects should be assessed in multiple cortical regions. It is hoped that these guidelines will be employed to ensure the continued use of TMS as a valuable tool in the field of cognitive neuroscience.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"121-126"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10233906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No convincing evidence the hippocampus is associated with working memory. 没有令人信服的证据表明海马体与工作记忆有关。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919
Scott D Slotnick
{"title":"No convincing evidence the hippocampus is associated with working memory.","authors":"Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a previous discussion paper , twenty-six working memory fMRI studies that reported activity in the hippocampus were systematically analyzed. None of these studies provided convincing evidence that the hippocampus was active during the late delay phase, the only period in which working memory can be isolated from long-term memory processes. Based on these results, it was concluded that working memory does not activate the hippocampus. Six commentaries on the discussion paper were received from Courtney (2022), Kessels and Bergmann (2022), Peters and Reithler (2022), Rose and Chao (2022), Stern and Hasselmo (2022), and Wood et al. (2022). Based on these commentaries, the present response paper considered whether there is evidence of sustained hippocampal activity during the working memory delay period based on depth-electrode recording, whether there are activity-silent working memory mechanisms in the hippocampus, and whether there is hippocampal lesion evidence indicating this region is important for working memory. There was no convincing electrophysiological or neuropsychological evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory maintenance, and activity-silent mechanisms were arguably speculative. Given that only a small fraction (approximately 5%) of working memory fMRI studies have reported hippocampal activity and lesion evidence indicates the hippocampus is not necessary for working memory, the burden of proof is on proponents of the view that the hippocampus is important for working memory to provide compelling evidence to support their position. To date, from my perspective, there is no convincing evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 3","pages":"96-106"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9743570","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Spatiotemporal dynamics of selective attention and visual conflict monitoring using a Stroop task. 选择性注意的时空动力学和使用Stroop任务的视觉冲突监测。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554
Rawan Jarrar, Colleen Monahan, Johanna Shattuck, Peter Teale, Eugene Kronberg, Benzi M Kluger, Isabelle Buard
{"title":"Spatiotemporal dynamics of selective attention and visual conflict monitoring using a Stroop task.","authors":"Rawan Jarrar, Colleen Monahan, Johanna Shattuck, Peter Teale, Eugene Kronberg, Benzi M Kluger, Isabelle Buard","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Selective attention and conflict monitoring are daily human phenomena, yet the spatial and temporal neurological underpinnings of these processes are not fully understood. Current literature suggests these executive functions occur via diverse and highly interconnected neural networks, including top-down, bottom-up, and conflict-control loops. To investigate the spatiotemporal activity of these processes, we collected neuromagnetic data using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in 28 healthy adults (age 19-36), while they performed a computerized Stroop task based on color naming. We focused on low-frequency oscillations in the context of top-down control and hypothesized that conflict monitoring-related activity would first be observed in the left anterior cingulate cortex, followed by the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and subsequently in the parietal and temporal lobes. Significant activity between 600-1000 ms post-stimulus onset was found for incongruent vs. congruent/neutral contrasts. Interestingly, spatiotemporal analysis did not provide evidence for a top-down pattern of activation, instead revealing a simultaneous pattern of activation in the frontal and temporal lobes. Most notable is the involvement of the left posterior inferior temporal cortex (pITC) and the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ), which have not conventionally been considered active players in attentional control. These results may be largely driven by alpha and beta oscillations from our sample population. Our findings challenge early theoretical models of top-down processing in the context of cognitive control from an attention perspective and also suggest a need to investigate attentional centers in the temporal lobe. Furthermore, the study highlights the valuable temporal data provided by MEG, which has been missing from previous studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"127-136"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10591874/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10287301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Degree of abstraction rather than ambiguity is crucial for driving mentalizing involvement commentary on "A-EM: a neurocognitive model for understanding mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality". 在“a - em:一种理解混合和模糊情感和道德的神经认知模型”的评论中,抽象程度(而非模糊性)是驱动心智介入的关键。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322
Kris Baetens, Ning Ma
{"title":"Degree of abstraction rather than ambiguity is crucial for driving mentalizing involvement <i>commentary on \"A-EM: a neurocognitive model for understanding mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality\"</i>.","authors":"Kris Baetens,&nbsp;Ning Ma","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Willems (this issue) proposes a neurocognitive model with a central role allotted to ambiguity in perceived morality and emotion in driving involvement of reflective/mentalizing processes. We argue that abstractness of representation has more explanatory power in this respect. We illustrate this with examples from the verbal and non-verbal domain showing a) concrete-ambiguous emotions processed through reflexive systems and b) abstract-unambiguous emotions processed through the mentalizing system, counter to MA-EM model predictions. However, due to the natural correlation between ambiguity and abstractness, both accounts will typically make convergent predictions.","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"70-72"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9250382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can we distinguish mixed from ambiguous emotions and morality? 我们能区分混杂的、模棱两可的情感和道德吗?
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321
Kobie van Krieken, Anneke de Graaf, Enny Das
{"title":"Can we distinguish mixed from ambiguous emotions and morality?","authors":"Kobie van Krieken,&nbsp;Anneke de Graaf,&nbsp;Enny Das","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The neurocognitive model of Mixed and Ambiguous Emotions and Morality (MA-EM) makes a relevant case for putting non-unidimensional emotions and morality more prominently on the research agenda. However, existing research challenges its assumptions about the distinction between mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality, and how they relate to reflective versus simulative processing routes, in three respects. First, the emotional state of <i>being moved</i> is generally conceptualized as a non-ambiguous rather than an ambiguous emotion. Second, mixed emotions have been found to elicit reflection rather than simulation. Third, the morality of narrative characters is typically perceived as mixed rather than ambiguous.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"68-69"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9620414","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mixed and ambiguous emotions can be studied with verbal irony. 混合和模棱两可的情绪可以用言语反讽来研究。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320
Valeria A Pfeifer, Penny M Pexman
{"title":"Mixed and ambiguous emotions can be studied with verbal irony.","authors":"Valeria A Pfeifer,&nbsp;Penny M Pexman","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this commentary we draw attention to a context involving mixed and ambiguous emotions: verbal irony. Irony is frequently used, evokes mixed emotional responses (e.g., criticism and amusement), and has been the focus of recent cognitive neuroscience research. Yet, irony has primarily been studied as a linguistic device, and has rarely been considered by emotion researchers. Similarly, linguistics has not considered mixed and ambiguous emotion when studying verbal irony. We argue that verbal irony offers rich opportunities to evoke and study mixed and ambiguous emotions, and might provide advantages for testing the MA-EM model.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"65-67"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9250384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Switching between emotions in the twenty-first century attention economy. 21世纪注意力经济中的情绪转换。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2170339
David Peeters, Siebe Bluijs
{"title":"Switching between emotions in the twenty-first century attention economy.","authors":"David Peeters,&nbsp;Siebe Bluijs","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2170339","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2170339","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The use of naturalistic stimuli in cognitive neuroscience experiments inspires and requires theoretical foundations that bring together different cognitive domains, such as emotion, language, and morality. By zooming in on the digital environments in which we often perceive emotional messages today, and inspired by the Mixed and Ambiguous Emotions and Morality model, we here argue that successfully processing emotional information in the twenty-first century will often have to rely not only on simulation and/or mentalizing, but also on executive control and attention regulation.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"61-62"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9250385","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Half-listening or zoned out? It's about the same: the impact of attentional state on word processing in context. 半听还是全神贯注?差不多:注意状态对语境中文字处理的影响。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-22 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2224959
Megan A Boudewyn
{"title":"Half-listening or zoned out? It's about the same: the impact of attentional state on word processing in context.","authors":"Megan A Boudewyn","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2224959","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2224959","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Language comprehension must require some degree of attentional focus, but how do periods of inattention and/or split attention impact how language is processed? Here EEG was recorded while participants listened to full-length stories, and were periodically asked about whether they were fully attentive, were completely inattentive, or felt that they were in a split attention state. The ERP response to the words immediately preceding these attention questions was examined as a function of participant response, which allowed for the comparison of word processing in each of these attentional states. When participants were on-task, typical N400 effects of lexical frequency (smaller N400 for common compared to less common words), word position (smaller N400 for words appearing late in a sentence compared to words appearing with less preceding context), and surprisal (smaller N400 for relatively expected words compared to relatively unexpected words) were observed. When participants were in a fully inattentive state, the word-level effect of frequency was intact, but the context-dependent effects of word position and surprisal were significantly reduced. Interestingly, the pattern of results when participants were in a split attention state closely matched that of the fully inattentive state. Overall, the results demonstrate how attentional state influences sensitivity to language context during comprehension, and show that the consequences of inattention and split attention on word processing in context are quite similar, at least on the indices measured here.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 3","pages":"107-114"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11276090/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9733830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural bases of motivated forgetting of autobiographical memories. 自传体记忆动机性遗忘的神经基础。
IF 2 4区 医学
Cognitive Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2022.2136150
FengYing Lu, WenJing Yang, Jiang Qiu
{"title":"Neural bases of motivated forgetting of autobiographical memories.","authors":"FengYing Lu,&nbsp;WenJing Yang,&nbsp;Jiang Qiu","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2022.2136150","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2022.2136150","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is important for mental health to be able to control unwanted intrusive memories. Previous studies suggest that middle frontal gyrus (MFG) down regulates pathways underlie the suppression of retrieval of general memories. However, the neural basis of motivated forgetting of autobiographical memories is unclear. Therefore, this study used two samples to explore the neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting of self-referential memories. Every participant provided 40 life events (20 negative and 20 neutral) from their past personal experience, and then completed the Think/No-Think task while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The first sample showed a significant reduction in recall in the No-Think condition relative to the Think condition. Attempting to exclude negative autobiographical memories from awareness was associated with increased activity in the right MFG, superior frontal gyrus (SFG), and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), while reduced activity was observed in the bilateral Brodmann areas BA18 and BA19, bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), bilateral precuneus, bilateral post cingulate cortex (PCC), the left parahippocampus, and the left hippocampus. Functional connectivity analyses showed that the right MFG projected into the bilateral mPFC, bilateral precuneus, and bilateral middle occipital gyrus (MOG) for negative autobiographical memories. The second sample replicated the results of the first sample at both the behavioral and brain levels. These results suggest that retrieval suppression of autobiographical memories involve the pathway between the MFG and the mPFC and precuneus to exclude self-referential memories. These results reveal how people engage in motivated forgetting of negative events in their daily lives.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 1","pages":"15-24"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9255715","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信