Journal of Experimental Social Psychology最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Religiosity predicts the delegation of decisions between moral and self-serving immoral outcomes 宗教信仰可预测道德结果和自私自利的不道德结果之间的决策授权
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605
Alexa Weiss , Matthias Forstmann
{"title":"Religiosity predicts the delegation of decisions between moral and self-serving immoral outcomes","authors":"Alexa Weiss ,&nbsp;Matthias Forstmann","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Studies support an association between religious belief and prosocial behavior. Such <em>religious prosociality</em> has been attributed to fear of supernatural punishment and enhanced concern for a prosocial reputation and self-image. Hence, religious individuals may be more prone to pursue their self-interest indirectly, thereby averting personal responsibility. We conducted 12 studies (<em>N</em><sub>total</sub> = 4468) to examine whether religiosity predicts delegation in incentivized deception, dictator, and die-roll cheating games and in realistic scenarios. Participants could choose between an immoral (e.g., lying) and a moral, prosocial (e.g., honest/fair) option or leave this decision to another individual (the agent) who equally benefited from the immoral option. Across all studies, religiosity positively predicted delegation, even though participants could directly implement prosocial outcomes. Employing experimental manipulations of participants' interests, we found that the predictive effect of religiosity on delegation only emerged when participants could expect to benefit from the agent's decision, but not when they were not affected by it or could be harmed by it. At the same time, religiosity predicted prosocial decisions among non-delegating participants. Moreover, delegating participants felt less bad and responsible about their decisions and victims' outcomes. Taken together, these findings suggest that delegation is strategically employed by individuals who would otherwise act prosocially to pursue selfish interests while avoiding responsibility and blame. They further support the notion of religious prosociality as a multi-faceted, context-dependent phenomenon.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104605"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140778917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) 不确定性、专业知识和说服力:Karmarkar 和 Tormala(2010 年)的复制与扩展
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619
Erik Løhre , Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar , Lewend Mayiwar , Thorvald Hærem
{"title":"Uncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010)","authors":"Erik Løhre ,&nbsp;Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar ,&nbsp;Lewend Mayiwar ,&nbsp;Thorvald Hærem","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>If you are trying to persuade someone, expressing your opinion with certainty intuitively seems like a good strategy to maximize your influence. However, Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) found that the effectiveness of this tactic depends on expertise. In three experiments, Karmarkar and Tormala found support for an incongruity hypothesis, whereby non-expert sources can gain interest and influence by expressing certainty, while expert sources can increase persuasion by expressing uncertainty. In this Registered Report, we conducted a high-powered (<em>N</em> = 1018) direct replication of Experiment 2 by Karmarkar and Tormala (2010). In a consumer behaviour context, the original study examined whether source expertise moderated the positive effect of source certainty on the persuasive impact of a restaurant recommendation. The present replication failed to find support for the incongruity hypothesis, <em>η</em><sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]: expressing certainty had a positive but non-significant effect for non-experts, <em>d</em> = 0.10 [−0.10, 0.34], and a positive effect for experts, <em>d</em> = 0.28 [0.03, 0.52]. Instead, the results supported the competing <em>confidence heuristic</em> hypothesis that expressed certainty would have a positive effect on persuasion, irrespective of source expertise, <em>d</em> = 0.18 [0.01, 0.36]. Extending the original work, we (1) controlled for the reason given for (un)certainty, and (2) examined need for closure as a potential individual difference moderator. The results indicated robust support for the confidence heuristic <em>d</em> = 0.25, [0.12, 0.37], but neither reason for (un)certainty nor need for closure moderated the effect as hypothesized. All materials, data, and code are available on: <span>https://osf.io/hbjyv/</span><svg><path></path></svg>.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104619"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000313/pdfft?md5=8234e9b0b3218c8ce87a65d11bab3ec6&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000313-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140618356","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the bounded generalized reciprocity model: Ingroup favoritism and concerns about negative evaluation 重新审视有界广义互惠模型:同类偏爱和对负面评价的担忧
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618
Yutaka Horita, Shun Hamada
{"title":"Revisiting the bounded generalized reciprocity model: Ingroup favoritism and concerns about negative evaluation","authors":"Yutaka Horita,&nbsp;Shun Hamada","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The bounded generalized reciprocity (BGR) model, grounded in reputation management, predicts that the motivation underlying ingroup favoritism (favoring one's own group over other groups) is driven by avoiding a negative reputation within one's own group. This research conducted two economic games with minimal groups in which reputational concerns (partners' knowledge of participants' group membership) were manipulated. We aimed to verify the replicability of the experimental results in support of the BGR model. A study (<em>N</em> = 394) using a dictator game (in which participants unilaterally determined their partners' payoffs) indicated the following: (1) participants were more likely to behave cooperatively with ingroup partners than with outgroup partners, regardless of whether their partners knew the participants' group membership; and (2) individual differences in fear of negative evaluation by others were not associated with cooperation toward the ingroup. Similar results were found in another study (<em>N</em> = 429) using a prisoner's dilemma game (in which participants' payoffs were determined by their partners). However, while sharing knowledge about group membership facilitated cooperation with the outgroup in the dictator game, no such tendency was observed in the prisoner's dilemma game. These findings suggest that concerns about a bad reputation may not play a relatively important role in ingroup favoritism, and that generosity toward outgroup members is influenced by the presence or absence of interdependence (i.e., whether the partner's behavior affects the participant's payoffs). This research proposes a reconsideration of the motivations behind cooperation within or between groups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104618"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140552449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rude or just blunt? Honor, dignity, and spontaneous trait inferences from potentially offensive behaviors 粗鲁还是直率?从潜在的冒犯行为中推断荣誉、尊严和自发特质
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617
Ceren Günsoy , Irmak Olcaysoy Okten , A. Demaske
{"title":"Rude or just blunt? Honor, dignity, and spontaneous trait inferences from potentially offensive behaviors","authors":"Ceren Günsoy ,&nbsp;Irmak Olcaysoy Okten ,&nbsp;A. Demaske","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To restore their reputation, people from honor cultures (e.g., U.S. South) are more likely than people from dignity cultures (e.g., U.S. North) to retaliate against conflict partners who insult them. If a conflict partner does not insult them, however, they are more polite than dignity culture individuals, so that they don't provoke the person unnecessarily. Previous research has not examined the implicit person perception phase in these interactions. In this research, we focused on spontaneous trait inferences (STIs) that people can make from others' potentially offensive behaviors. In four studies (<em>n</em> = 1126), we tested whether being from a U.S. honor or dignity culture or the endorsement of these values was associated with hostile and nonhostile STIs, and whether honor and dignity influenced the relationship between STIs and behavioral intentions. In Study 1 and 2, honor culture participants made both types of STIs, whereas dignity culture participants only made hostile STIs. Study 3 revealed a positive association between individual honor endorsement and nonhostile STIs. In Study 4, we replicated most of these results and also found a positive association between hostile STIs and confrontation intentions in the honor (but not dignity) group. These results suggest that people from honor (vs. dignity) cultures tend to make rather simultaneous inferences of hostile and nonhostile traits from potentially offensive behaviors and making spontaneous hostile inferences can be more consequential for them. This research highlights the complexity of culture's influence on interpersonal processes and can have implications for diverse social contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104617"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140542516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To beckon or not to beckon: Testing a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment: A registered report 招还是不招?测试道德判断的因果-评价建模方法:注册报告
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616
Cillian McHugh , Kathryn B. Francis , Jim A.C. Everett , Shane Timmons
{"title":"To beckon or not to beckon: Testing a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment: A registered report","authors":"Cillian McHugh ,&nbsp;Kathryn B. Francis ,&nbsp;Jim A.C. Everett ,&nbsp;Shane Timmons","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Moral judgments are increasingly being understood as showing context dependent variability. A growing literature has identified a range of specific contextual factors (e.g., emotions, intentions) that can influence moral judgments in predictable ways. Integrating these diverse influences into a unified approach to understanding moral judgments remains a challenge. Recent work by Railton (2017) attempted to address this with a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment. In support of this model Railton presents evidence from novel variations of classic trolley type dilemmas. We present results from a pre-registered pilot study that highlight a significant confound and demonstrate that it likely influenced Railton's results. Building on this, our registered report presents a replication-extension of Railton's study, using larger more diverse samples, and more rigorous methods and materials, specifically controlling for potential confounds. We found that participants' judgments in sacrificial dilemmas are influenced by both direct personal force, and by whether harm occurs as a means or as a side-effect of action. We also show the relationship between a range of individual difference variables and responses to sacrificial moral dilemmas. Our results provide novel insights into the factors that influence people's moral judgments, and contribute to ongoing theoretical debates in moral psychology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104616"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000283/pdfft?md5=64c886f733e92422f57027ea21a78ac0&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000283-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140536277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Most people do not “value the struggle”: Tempted agents are judged as less virtuous than those who were never tempted 大多数人并不 "重视斗争":受到诱惑的人被认为不如从未受到诱惑的人有德行
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104615
Ryan M. McManus , Helen Padilla Fong , Max Kleiman-Weiner , Liane Young
{"title":"Most people do not “value the struggle”: Tempted agents are judged as less virtuous than those who were never tempted","authors":"Ryan M. McManus ,&nbsp;Helen Padilla Fong ,&nbsp;Max Kleiman-Weiner ,&nbsp;Liane Young","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104615","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104615","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Do people judge those who overcome temptation as more virtuous than those who don't feel tempted in the first place? Because prior research provides conflicting answers to this question, the current paper uses an expanded set of methodological and statistical tools to solve this puzzle. First, we replicated results of prior research showing that agents who overcome temptation are seen as less virtuous than non-tempted agents, with 74–78% of people making this judgment. Second, we used participant-generated stimuli and one measure from each of two published papers to rule out stimulus and measurement sampling as explanations for the previous opposite effects. We replicated our original results: 72–75% of people judged agents who overcame temptation as less virtuous than non-tempted agents. Third, we investigated whether judgments were moderated by relationship context. Again, the majority of people judged agents who overcame temptation–that would harm strangers or close others–as less virtuous than non-tempted agents. Additionally, the following interaction effect was the most common (modal) pattern: While judging tempted agents as less virtuous than non-tempted agents within each relationship context, 39% of people judged agents who were tempted to act in a way that would harm close others as even less virtuous than those agents whose temptations would harm strangers. Together, these results provide a detailed moral psychological account of temptation by: resolving a puzzle in the literature, revealing moderation by relationship context, and documenting the pervasiveness of this effect across stimuli, measures, and persons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104615"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140328695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judging the guilt of the un-guilty: The roles of “false positive” guilt and empathy in moral character perception 判断无罪者是否有罪:假阳性 "内疚感和移情在道德品格认知中的作用
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104613
Danielle E. Wahlers, William Hart, Joshua T. Lambert
{"title":"Judging the guilt of the un-guilty: The roles of “false positive” guilt and empathy in moral character perception","authors":"Danielle E. Wahlers,&nbsp;William Hart,&nbsp;Joshua T. Lambert","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104613","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104613","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>When people accidentally harm others, some theory anticipates that expressing normatively unexpected (“false positive”) guilt is socially functional because it signals a positive moral character and likability. Although previous evidence shows anticipated effects of false positive guilt on these outcomes, it is possible these effects result from perceiving aspects specific to empathy (vs. guilt). We address this possibility in three preregistered studies. Participants answered questions regarding their perceptions of agents of accidental harm. In Experiment 1 (<em>N</em> = 299), agents that expressed guilt (vs. no guilt) received higher moral character and likability ratings; mediation evidence suggested these effects resulted via perceptions that the agent experienced empathy-specific (e.g., concern, understanding), not guilt-specific (e.g., self-blame) sentiments; if anything, guilt-specific sentiments reduced some moral character evaluations. Experiment 2 (<em>N</em> = 503) was a conceptual replication with more ecologically valid methods; it provided similar conclusions. Experiment 3 (<em>N</em> = 653) crossed an agent's expression of guilt-specific sentiments (present vs. absent) with empathy-specific sentiments (present vs. absent). Main effects of empathy-specific sentiments on moral character and likability judgments were at least seven times larger than those attributed to guilt-specific sentiments. Additionally, when empathy-specific sentiments were expressed, the expression of guilt-specific sentiments had no positive effects on moral character and likability judgments; however, when empathy-specific sentiments were <em>not</em> expressed, the expression of guilt-specific sentiments enhanced some but not all moral character judgments. We discuss how our findings contribute to understanding the social benefits of expressing false positive guilt and cohere with some impression formation perspectives.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104613"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140135052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How pledges reduce dishonesty: The role of involvement and identification 承诺如何减少不诚实行为:参与和认同的作用
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104614
Eyal Peer , Nina Mazar , Yuval Feldman , Dan Ariely
{"title":"How pledges reduce dishonesty: The role of involvement and identification","authors":"Eyal Peer ,&nbsp;Nina Mazar ,&nbsp;Yuval Feldman ,&nbsp;Dan Ariely","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104614","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104614","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Authorities and managers often rely on individuals and businesses' self-reports and employ various forms of honesty declarations to ensure that those individuals and businesses do not over-claim payments, benefits, or other resources. While previous work has found that honesty pledges have the potential to decrease dishonesty, effects have been mixed. We argue that understanding and predicting when honesty pledges are effective has been obstructed due to variations in experimental designs and operationalizations of honesty pledges in previous research. Specifically, we focus on the role of whether and how an ex-ante honesty pledge asks individuals to identify (by ID, name, initials) and how much involvement the pledge requires from the individual (low: just reading vs. high: re-typing the text of the pledge). In four pre-registered online studies (<em>N</em> &gt; 5000), we systematically examine these two dimensions of a pledge to find that involvement is often more effective than identification. In addition, low involvement pledges, without any identification, are mostly ineffective. Finally, we find that the effect of a high (vs. low) involvement pledge is relatively more persistent across tasks. Yet, repeating a low involvement pledge across tasks increases its effectiveness and compensates for the lower persistency across tasks. Taken together, these results contribute both to theory by comparing some of the mechanisms possibly underlying honesty pledges as well as to practice by providing guidance to managers and policymakers on how to effectively design pledges to prevent or reduce dishonesty in self-reports.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104614"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140122299","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of dominance and prestige on children's resource allocation: What if they coexist? 支配地位和威望对儿童资源分配的影响:如果它们同时存在会怎样?
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104604
Xuran Zhang , Xia Zhang , Ranzhi Yang , Yanfang Li
{"title":"The influence of dominance and prestige on children's resource allocation: What if they coexist?","authors":"Xuran Zhang ,&nbsp;Xia Zhang ,&nbsp;Ranzhi Yang ,&nbsp;Yanfang Li","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104604","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104604","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The antagonistic relation between the two ways of reaching the top, i.e., dominance and prestige, has generally been accepted in recent decades. People perceive dominance as a “negative” trait that reduces the quantity of resources that should be allocated to individuals who exhibit such a trait. In contrast, prestige is viewed as a “positive” trait, that increases the appropriate amount of resources for such allocation. However, the situation is somewhat complicated because dominance and prestige can serve as different evaluative dimensions for the same person since that person could be esteemed for their expertise yet simultaneously critiqued to their assertive personality. This article first investigated how children aged 3- to 8-year-old weigh prestige and dominance when those traits coexist within individuals. The results of Study 1 revealed that children exhibited a developmental pattern of resource allocation, progressing from favoring the high-dominance to the low-dominance individual. Their theory of mind capacity predicted their preference for low-dominance individual. One professional prestige situation was also investigated which showed that children stably favor characters with high prestige. Children begin to distinguish between dominance and prestige in resource allocation at approximately 5 years. Study 2 further explored how 5- to 8-year-olds weigh the rewarding high-prestige individuals against compensating low-dominance individuals when these traits clash within the same person, which showed that children at this stage prioritize prestige rather than dominance. Taken together, these findings suggest that children are capable to differentiate between dominance and prestige as two distinct ways when perceiving social ranks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104604"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140103767","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
(Not) showing you feel good, can be bad: The consequences of breaking expressivity norms for positive emotions (不)表现出你感觉良好,可能是坏事:打破积极情绪表达规范的后果
IF 3.5 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104600
Kunalan Manokara , Alisa Balabanova , Mirna Đurić , Agneta H. Fischer , Disa A. Sauter
{"title":"(Not) showing you feel good, can be bad: The consequences of breaking expressivity norms for positive emotions","authors":"Kunalan Manokara ,&nbsp;Alisa Balabanova ,&nbsp;Mirna Đurić ,&nbsp;Agneta H. Fischer ,&nbsp;Disa A. Sauter","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104600","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104600","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Are there optimal levels of showing one feels good? Examining four positive emotions (<em>gratitude, interest, feeling moved, triumph</em>), we demonstrate in two pre-registered experiments (<em>n</em> = 901) that even for pleasant feelings, showing too much – or too little – can lead to negative social consequences. Expressers who downplay their gratitude, and to a lesser degree interest, are deprived of social contact and power. Restrained displays of feeling moved are also met with reduced contact. For triumph, amplified expressers are socially avoided, yet at the same time, those who downplay their victory are seen to be less powerful. We demonstrate the role of person-perception mechanisms (warmth and competence) as underlying explanators for these effects. Taken together, our findings contribute to the growing literature on the social consequences of emotional expressions, by pointing to divergent outcomes for norm violations relating to different positive emotions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104600"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140013969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信