Journal of Experimental Social Psychology最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Group and personal rejection are similarly linked to extreme intergroup attitudes 群体和个人的排斥同样与极端的群体间态度有关
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104788
Luis Marcos-Vidal , Boryana Todorova , Scott Atran , Clara Pretus
{"title":"Group and personal rejection are similarly linked to extreme intergroup attitudes","authors":"Luis Marcos-Vidal ,&nbsp;Boryana Todorova ,&nbsp;Scott Atran ,&nbsp;Clara Pretus","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104788","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104788","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Perceived marginalization of social groups has been identified as one of the main drivers of violent extremism across countries. However, most psychological research has focused on interpersonal rather than intergroup processes to understand the link between social exclusion and aggression. We developed a new paradigm, RateME, which dissociates the effects of group rejection and personal rejection, two types of social exclusion that involve negative attention. We compared the psychological effects of group rejection with those of personal rejection using RateME as well as ostracism using Cyberball in a sample of more than 1200 UK residents. Experiencing group rejection, personal rejection, and ostracism was independently associated with increased psychological distress and state hostility, regardless of participants' degree of identification with the group. Exclusion of either type also indirectly increased group supremacist attitudes by undermining psychological needs and indirectly increased extreme intergroup attitudes by increasing state hostility. Our work reveals similar detrimental psychological effects of group-level and personal-level exclusion, highlighting group rejection as a risk factor for mental health with potential implications for violent extremism.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"121 ","pages":"Article 104788"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144680641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The evolved psychology of mate preferences: Sexual desire underlies the prioritization of attractiveness in long-term partners 进化的伴侣偏好心理学:性欲是长期伴侣优先考虑吸引力的基础
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104791
Sierra D. Peters , Jon K. Maner , Andrea L. Meltzer
{"title":"The evolved psychology of mate preferences: Sexual desire underlies the prioritization of attractiveness in long-term partners","authors":"Sierra D. Peters ,&nbsp;Jon K. Maner ,&nbsp;Andrea L. Meltzer","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104791","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104791","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Although existing evolutionary psychological research provides insight into the ultimate reproductive functions underlying long-term romantic partner preferences, such research has generally stopped short of identifying the proximate affective and motivational mechanisms that drive those preferences. Findings from three studies comprising four independent samples (total <em>N</em> = 2099) provide evidence that sexual desire is a proximate mechanism underlying prioritization of attractiveness in long-term partners. Sex differences and individual differences in sociosexuality were associated with prioritization of long-term partner attractiveness and those associations were statistically mediated by levels of chronic sexual desire (Study 1). Experimentally activating a state of high sexual desire increased males' and females' prioritization of attractiveness to an equivalent degree (Study 2). Experimentally reducing the relevance of sexual desire in people's mate preferences decreased prioritization of attractiveness in both males and females (Study 3). These studies integrate ultimate and proximate perspectives to provide novel insight into the role situationally activated motivational states play in shaping long-term partner preferences.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"121 ","pages":"Article 104791"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144655483","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Common ingroup meta-identification: A meta-perception perspective to dynamically promote intergroup attitudes in status asymmetry contexts 共同群体内元认同:地位不对称背景下动态促进群体间态度的元感知视角
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104793
Wenlin Ke , Fangfang Wen , Bin Zuo
{"title":"Common ingroup meta-identification: A meta-perception perspective to dynamically promote intergroup attitudes in status asymmetry contexts","authors":"Wenlin Ke ,&nbsp;Fangfang Wen ,&nbsp;Bin Zuo","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104793","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104793","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Building upon the application of common ingroup identity in intergroup relations and the meta-perception perspective, this study innovatively introduces the concept of Common Ingroup Meta-Identification (CIMI), defined as individuals' perceptions regarding the identification of various subgroups with a common ingroup. Through four experiments, the research systematically examines both the effects of CIMI on intergroup attitudes and the moderating role of group status, using Agent-Based Modeling to simulate the underlying dynamic processes. The results indicated that manipulating CIMI effectively affected intergroup attitudes in both real groups and minimal groups (Study 1 and Study 2). Notably, CIMI from high-status groups exhibited a more pronounced effect, particularly when low-status groups perceived low level CIMI from high-status groups (Study 3A). The ABM simulation results in Study 3B revealed that at the group level, even a small number of individuals with strong shared group identity could trigger widespread improvements in intergroup attitudes, as their positive influence spreads through mutual CIMI. The starting level of this shared identity, especially in higher-status groups, played a pivotal role in determining whether attitudes became sustainably positive or regressed to negativity. These findings suggest that manipulating CIMI serves as an effective strategy for enhancing intergroup relations, providing a flexible approach to fostering mutual understanding among diverse nations, ethnicities, and cultures of different status.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"121 ","pages":"Article 104793"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144655484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing “quarantined” metarepresentational accounts of Theory of Mind: Are we biased by others' false beliefs? 测试“隔离”的心智理论的元表征:我们是否被他人的错误信念所偏见?
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104785
Steven Samuel , Robert Lurz , Daizi Davies , Harry Axtell , Sarah K. Salo
{"title":"Testing “quarantined” metarepresentational accounts of Theory of Mind: Are we biased by others' false beliefs?","authors":"Steven Samuel ,&nbsp;Robert Lurz ,&nbsp;Daizi Davies ,&nbsp;Harry Axtell ,&nbsp;Sarah K. Salo","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104785","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104785","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>An important component of Theory of Mind is the ability to understand the beliefs (true or false) of others. Arguably the most widely-held view is that this is performed by a detached <em>belief-representation</em> process (e.g., metarepresenting that another agent has a belief about the world which one does not share). The standard belief-representation account posits a separation between one's own first-order representations of the physical environment and one's second-order representations of another agent's mental states, preventing the latter from infecting the former. An alternative process is engaged <em>belief-simulation</em> (e.g., imaginatively adopting another agent's belief about the world) which, in contrast to standard belief-representation, posits a correspondence in the mental states shared by oneself and another agent and predicts an influence of the other agent's beliefs on one's own first-order representations and egocentric actions. In the first two of three studies, a participant and an agent watched an object buried in a continuous space (sandbox). The participant then watched the same object moved from the first location to a new location. When participants were asked to search for the object, they demonstrated a bias towards the first location when the agent falsely believed the object to be there but not when the agent knew, like the participant, that the object was in the new location. Reasoning that the strength of this bias may have been limited by participants' knowledge of the object's true whereabouts, in a third and final study we hid the movement of the object so that participants did not know its true location. We also recruited a greater number of participants to increase statistical power. Contrary to expectations, there was now no evidence of belief infection. Overall, these results are more consistent with a belief-representation account for (human) adults' understanding of others' belief states.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"121 ","pages":"Article 104785"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144633663","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predicting that birds of a feather will flock together: Expectations of homophily for others but not the self 预测物以类聚:对他人而非自己的同质性的期望
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104784
Miriam E. Schwyck, Carolyn Parkinson
{"title":"Predicting that birds of a feather will flock together: Expectations of homophily for others but not the self","authors":"Miriam E. Schwyck,&nbsp;Carolyn Parkinson","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104784","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104784","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Similarity among friends – or other socially connected individuals – is a ubiquitous characteristic of social networks. There are several, often simultaneous, mechanisms (e.g., social influence, shared environments) through which such links between social connection and similarity arise, including homophily, or the tendency for similar people to attract one another. While past research has found that people use similarity heuristics to structure their mental representations of social networks (predicting that friends are likely to be similar to each other), it is unknown if people assume such similarity arises through homophily, specifically. Here, we tested if people assume that homophily will shape their own and others' future friendships. Participants (<em>N</em><sub>Total</sub> = 560) learned how (i) trustworthy and (ii) trusting various partners were through repeated trust games. Participants predicted which partners would become friends with one another and which partners they would become friends with themselves if they were to meet in person. Across two studies and both trait measures, we found that participants were significantly more likely to predict that partners who behaved similarly would later become friends compared to those who behaved dissimilarly. Interestingly, we found that participants were significantly more likely to predict that they would become friends with highly trustworthy and highly trusting partners compared to highly untrustworthy or highly untrusting partners, regardless of their own behavioral tendencies or even their own self-perceptions. These findings suggest that for trust-related traits, people assume homophily will govern others' relationships but not necessarily their own. Such expectations likely shape how people approach or foster new friendships for themselves and between others.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104784"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144595723","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“I wanna thank me”: Reputational consequences of attribution locus depend on outcome valence “我要感谢我”:归因点的名誉后果取决于结果效价
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104789
Ignazio Ziano , Deming Wang , Ovul Sezer
{"title":"“I wanna thank me”: Reputational consequences of attribution locus depend on outcome valence","authors":"Ignazio Ziano ,&nbsp;Deming Wang ,&nbsp;Ovul Sezer","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104789","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104789","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Five studies (four preregistered; total <em>n</em> = 2948, French and U.S. adult participants) show that the reputational consequences of attributing an outcome to internal or external sources depend on the outcome's valence. Individuals are liked more when they attribute successes to external sources (such as their teammates) and when they attribute failures to internal sources (such as themselves) (Studies 1, 2, and 3). Consequently, participants donate more to others who fit this attribution profile (Study 4). This preference is associated with participants' belief that targets attributing successes internally and failures externally are misrepresenting their contribution to the outcome (Studies 1 and 3). The effect is observed for attributions made both in public and in private but is slightly weaker for those made in private settings (Study 5). We discuss theoretical implications of the results for impression formation and the psychology of perceived contribution, and practical implications for communication in organizational and workplace settings.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104789"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144589141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I am, I am not: Strategies to cope with negative group labels 我是,我不是:应对消极群体标签的策略
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104790
Gloria Danqiao Cheng , Jennifer Whitson , Cynthia S. Wang
{"title":"I am, I am not: Strategies to cope with negative group labels","authors":"Gloria Danqiao Cheng ,&nbsp;Jennifer Whitson ,&nbsp;Cynthia S. Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104790","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104790","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Negative group labels (i.e., slurs) can reinforce stigma and perpetuate oppression of stigmatized groups. This research compares two label-coping strategies that stigmatized group members employ to cope with negative group labels: <em>self-labeling</em> (“I am X”) and <em>label-rejection</em> (“I am not X”). Seven studies demonstrate that self-labeling more effectively reduces outgroup observers' perceptions of a label's negativity compared to label-rejection. However, outgroup observers impose higher social costs on self-labelers (in terms of reduced likability, hireability, and promotability) than label-rejecters. These effects generalize across multiple stigmatized groups and contexts (Studies 1a-d). Moreover, the effect of label-coping strategy on label negativity is mediated by observers' perceptions of the stigmatized group's control over the label (Study 2), while the effect of label-coping strategy on social costs is mediated by observers' experience of symbolic threat (Study 3). Finally, observers who more strongly believe that status differences between groups are justified are more likely to penalize self-labelers vs. label-rejecters due to heightened threat (Study 4).</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104790"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144581242","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rethinking rose-tinted glasses: A cognitive-ecological explanation of prospective self-comparisons 重新思考玫瑰色眼镜:前瞻性自我比较的认知生态学解释
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104787
Samantha Zaw, Alex Koch
{"title":"Rethinking rose-tinted glasses: A cognitive-ecological explanation of prospective self-comparisons","authors":"Samantha Zaw,&nbsp;Alex Koch","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104787","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104787","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>We examined prospective (i.e., future-oriented) self-comparisons in an ecology with three empirically validated principles of impression formation. First, people <em>can</em> have more negative than positive attributes (diversity). But second, individuals <em>do</em> have more positive than negative attributes (frequency). Third, over time people generally improve by losing negative attributes and gaining positive attributes (growth). Three studies found that present-future similarities were most positive, the present self's unique attributes were most negative, and the future self's unique attributes were in between. In Study 1, this pattern emerged for both self-comparisons and acquaintance-comparisons. In Study 2, it emerged for both prospective and retrospective (i.e., past-oriented) self-comparisons. In Study 3, the pattern vanished when middle-aged people compared their present self to their old age self. Taken together, these findings largely support a realistic view of one's own future. The findings also lend some support for a self-enhancing view of what the future holds.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104787"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144581241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From ostracized to pleased: How fair and unfair social exclusion activates schadenfreude 从排斥到高兴:公平和不公平的社会排斥如何激发幸灾乐祸
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-07-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104786
Sarah Mohammadi, Andrew H. Hales
{"title":"From ostracized to pleased: How fair and unfair social exclusion activates schadenfreude","authors":"Sarah Mohammadi,&nbsp;Andrew H. Hales","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104786","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104786","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>People sometimes derive pleasure from the misfortune of others. Here, we investigate social ostracism as a multifaceted trigger of this complex emotion: <em>schadenfreude.</em> Building on the theories of fairness perception, ostracism, and justice-based schadenfreude, we propose that both experiencing and merely observing ostracism can elicit schadenfreude toward an ostracizer. This response is primarily driven by two factors: (1) the (un)fairness of the ostracism itself and (2) the perceived deservingness of an ostracizer to face negative consequences. Four preregistered experiments support these predictions. People who are ostracized (Study 1; <em>N</em> = 338) experience schadenfreude specifically toward their excluders but not toward uninvolved individuals, indicating that this response is directed rather than generalized. Not just targets but also third-party observers (Study 2; <em>N</em> = 82) report schadenfreude toward the ostracizer. Supporting the justice-based account, among both targets and observers (Study 3; <em>N</em> = 624), ostracism-induced schadenfreude is statistically accounted for by the perceived <em>deservingness</em> of the ostracizer to experience negative outcomes. Finally, providing direct causal evidence, ostracism elicited schadenfreude, but only when experimentally manipulated to be unfair (Study 4; <em>N</em> = 479): excluders who unfairly ostracized well-behaved individuals elicited greater schadenfreude than those who fairly ostracized provocative norm-violators. This indicates the fairness of exclusion influences perceptions of how much an ostracizer deserves misfortune, and thus the subsequent schadenfreude. Together, findings document a previously unexplored consequence of ostracism: the emergence of schadenfreude as a moral emotion in response to unfair exclusion.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104786"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144563103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vicarious kin derogation—when and why people mock the innocent family members of political leaders 代亲属贬损——人们何时以及为何嘲笑政治领导人的无辜家庭成员
IF 3.2 2区 心理学
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Pub Date : 2025-06-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104781
Simone Tang , Kurt Gray
{"title":"Vicarious kin derogation—when and why people mock the innocent family members of political leaders","authors":"Simone Tang ,&nbsp;Kurt Gray","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104781","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2025.104781","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Despite the widespread human aversion to harming the innocent, people often mock or attack the children and spouses of public figures they dislike, a pattern we call vicarious kin derogation (VKD). Targeting the vulnerable kin of powerful figures has been used to inflict psychological pain, from ancient China to modern-day North Korea, to social media in the West. Six studies explore VKD with both experimental and field data. Across 562,066 tweets directed at US presidential candidates, people derogate politicians' loved ones when those loved ones seem more emotionally vulnerable than the candidate themselves (Study 1). In experiments, people engage in VKD when a politician seems more emotionally affected by attacks on family than on himself (Studies 2 and 4), and when people are motivated to inflict suffering on the public figure (Study 5). At the same time, people are less likely to endorse VKD when reminded that it targets innocent individuals (Studies 3 and 5). Together, these studies reveal a central psychological tension: VKD is satisfying when it seems to hurt an otherwise invulnerable leader, but people still dislike harming the innocent. This dynamic helps explain a persistent, toxic feature of modern political discourse: when people seek to harm public figures by attacking their loved ones.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"120 ","pages":"Article 104781"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"144481071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信