IntelligencePub Date : 2024-07-18DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101845
David Becker , Thomas R. Coyle , Heiner Rindermann
{"title":"Unraveling the nexus: Culture, cognitive competence, and economic performance across 86 nations (2000–2018)","authors":"David Becker , Thomas R. Coyle , Heiner Rindermann","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101845","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101845","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Numerous studies have explored the complex relationship between culture, cognitive competence, and economic performance globally. However, findings from these investigations vary significantly and occasionally contradict each other. This study delves into this connection by analyzing variables or dimensions from three distinct models of <em>national culture</em> concerning the results of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) and the economic strength and growth of 86 nations between 2000 and 2018. Religious affiliations emerge as a significant statistical explanatory factor, accounting for a substantial portion of the variance in overall PISA performance, ranging from 24% to 40%. Remarkably, an underutilized cultural model, the <em>Axiological Cube</em>, surpasses others, exhibiting explanatory power ranging from 45% to 63%. Path analyses rooted in both <em>Human Capital Theory</em> and <em>Cognitive Capitalism Theory</em> reveal that cultural variables exert their influence on economic growth mainly indirectly through their impact on student competence. Cultural variables exhibit robust predictive capacity for overall student competences, as indicated by PISA mean scores. However, they prove inadequate in explaining certain cognitive competence patterns, such as disparities in inequality and subject-specific variations, like mathematics and reading. This study also highlights uncertainties surrounding the effects of Confucianism and East Asian religions, prompting further discussion and investigation.</p><p>Σ words: 200.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"106 ","pages":"Article 101845"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000394/pdfft?md5=8daffc138a35ca89a06d4eddb1bcaaa9&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000394-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141729758","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-07-01DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101836
Y. Schulz-Zhecheva , M.C. Voelkle , M. Biscaldi , A. Beauducel , C. Klein
{"title":"On the relationships between processing speed, intra-subject variability, working memory, and fluid intelligence – A cross-sectional study","authors":"Y. Schulz-Zhecheva , M.C. Voelkle , M. Biscaldi , A. Beauducel , C. Klein","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101836","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101836","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The developmental cascade model, elaborated by Fry and Hale (2000) emphasizes the role of age-related increases in processing speed and working memory for the development of fluid intelligence. Given the intimate relationships between intra-subject variability and the aforementioned constructs, the present study set out to determine the role of intra-subject variability within the pathways outlined in the developmental cascade model, postulating a fundamental role of intra-subject variability for the development of processing speed, working memory and fluid intelligence. To that end, <em>N</em> = 403 participants aged 8–18 years took a testing battery including choice reaction time tasks to measure processing speed and intra-subject variability as well as span, operation span and coordination tasks to measure working memory within the empirical framework of Oberauer et al. (2003). Cattell's Culture Fair Test (CFT-20 R) was used to measure fluid intelligence. Our results confirm the well-known close relationships between processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence, and show that intra-subject variability is also closely related to these constructs. The results of the present study suggest the extension of the developmental cascade model by the inclusion of intra-subject variability as a fundamental construct.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"105 ","pages":"Article 101836"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141622540","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-07-01DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101844
Ian J. Deary , Simon R. Cox , Judith A. Okely
{"title":"Inspection time and intelligence: A five-wave longitudinal study from age 70 to age 82 in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936","authors":"Ian J. Deary , Simon R. Cox , Judith A. Okely","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101844","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101844","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To test the idea that the slowing of simple information processing contributes to more general cognitive ageing, it is necessary to demonstrate that changes in the two variables are correlated as people grow older. Here, we examine the association between inspection time—a psychophysical measure of visual information processing—and general cognitive ability and the cognitive domains of visuospatial reasoning, processing speed, memory, and crystallised ability across five waves of testing in a 12-year period. The participants were members of the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936; there was a maximum of 1090 people with cognitive data at age 70 (Wave 1) and 426 at age 82 (Wave 5). At each testing wave the participants took the same 12 cognitive tests. Latent growth curve modelling in a structural equation modelling framework was used to examine the associations between intercepts and slopes of inspection time and other cognitive capabilities. Age-related changes (slope) in inspection time correlated 0.898 (<em>p</em> < 0.001) with changes (slope) in general cognitive ability over the 12 years. Inspection time changes correlated with changes in each of the four cognitive domains, but these associations were reduced to non-significance once the domains' loadings on general cognitive ability were taken into account (with the possible exception of memory, whose changes still had a marginal additional association with inspection time changes; <em>β</em> = 0.199, <em>p</em> = 0.030). The results are compatible with the idea that age-related slowing of processing speed contributes causally to the age-related declines in complex cognitive capability, but this is not the only interpretation of the present findings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"105 ","pages":"Article 101844"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000382/pdfft?md5=3041ef85760c41713bebdebba5819347&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000382-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141484573","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-05-16DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101837
Nash Unsworth
{"title":"Variation in general retrieval ability in semantic and autobiographical fluency tasks","authors":"Nash Unsworth","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101837","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Individual differences in general retrieval ability in semantic and autobiographical fluency tasks were examined in the current study. Participants performed multiple fluency tasks requiring retrieval from semantic memory, autobiographical semantic memory, and autobiographical memory. Participants also completed multiple measures of working memory and vocabulary. Confirmatory factor analysis suggested that three distinct, but correlated fluency factors (semantic memory, autobiographical semantic memory, and autobiographical memory) best accounted for the data. These factors loaded onto a higher-order general retrieval factor. Working memory was correlated with the semantic, autobiographical semantic, and the higher-order general retrieval factor. Vocabulary correlated positively with semantic memory, but negatively autobiographical semantic memory, and not significantly with the higher-order factor. These results suggest there is a general retrieval ability that cuts across semantic and autobiographical fluency tasks and this general retrieval factor is correlated with working memory, but not necessarily with vocabulary. These results provide important information on the nature of individual differences in general retrieval ability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"105 ","pages":"Article 101837"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140952448","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-05-09DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101834
Sophie von Stumm , Allie F. Nancarrow
{"title":"New methods, persistent issues, and one solution: Gene-environment interaction studies of childhood cognitive development","authors":"Sophie von Stumm , Allie F. Nancarrow","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101834","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101834","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children's differences in cognitive development stem from the complex interplay of genetic and environmental factors. Identifying gene-environment interactions in cognitive development is key for effectively targeting interventions that improve children's life chances. The advent of polygenic scores, which aggregate DNA variants to index a person's genetic propensities for phenotypic development, has created unprecedented opportunities for pinpointing gene-environment interactions. Yet, the issue of statistical power – the probability of detecting a true effect – prevails, and no replicable gene-environment interactions in child cognitive development have been reported. In this review article, we recapitulate three approaches to studying gene-environment interactions, including twin studies, candidate gene models, and polygenic score methods. We then discuss the issue of statistical power in gene-environment interaction research and conclude that larger samples are key to ushering a new era of replicable gene-environment interaction findings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"105 ","pages":"Article 101834"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016028962400028X/pdfft?md5=cd9adebcc50ca60cf1f5082cffb111b9&pid=1-s2.0-S016028962400028X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900940","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-05-01DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101835
Cody A. Mashburn , Alexander P. Burgoyne , Jason S. Tsukahara , Richard Pak , Joseph T. Coyne , Ciara Sibley , Cyrus Foroughi , Randall W. Engle
{"title":"Knowledge, attention, and psychomotor ability: A latent variable approach to understanding individual differences in simulated work performance","authors":"Cody A. Mashburn , Alexander P. Burgoyne , Jason S. Tsukahara , Richard Pak , Joseph T. Coyne , Ciara Sibley , Cyrus Foroughi , Randall W. Engle","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101835","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101835","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We compare the validity of personnel selection measures and novel tests of attention control for explaining individual differences in synthetic work performance, which required participants to monitor and complete multiple ongoing tasks. In Study 1, an online sample of young adults (<em>N</em> = 474, aged 18–35) based in the United States completed three-minute tests of attention control and two tests that primarily measure acquired knowledge, the Wonderlic and the Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT). Structural equation modeling revealed that acquired knowledge tests did not predict simulated work performance beyond attention control, whereas attention control did predict simulated work performance controlling for other measures. In Study 2, an in-lab sample of young adults from Georgia Tech and the greater Atlanta community (<em>N</em> = 321, aged 18–35) completed tests of attention control, processing speed, working memory capacity, and versions of two U.S. Military selection tests, one assessing acquired knowledge (the AFQT) and one assessing psychomotor ability (the Performance-Based Measures assessment from the Aviation Selection Test Battery). Structural equation modeling revealed that attention control fully mediated the relationship between the Performance Based Measures and simulated work performance, but the AFQT and processing speed retained unique prediction. We also explore possible gender differences. Collectively, these results suggest that tests of attention control may be a useful supplement to existing personnel selection measures when complex cognitive tasks are the criterion variable of interest.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101835"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140893918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-05-01DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101815
Kimmo Sorjonen , Bo Melin , Gustav Nilsonne
{"title":"Inconclusive evidence for an increasing effect of maternal supportiveness on childhood intelligence in Dunkel et al. (2023): A simulated reanalysis","authors":"Kimmo Sorjonen , Bo Melin , Gustav Nilsonne","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101815","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101815","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In a recent study (<em>N</em> = 1075), Dunkel et al. (2023) concluded that maternal supportiveness is important for children's general intelligence. Maternal supportiveness was measured at ages 14, 24, and 36 months while children's intelligence was measured at ages 14, 24, and 36 months and at 4 and 10 years. The effects of maternal supportiveness at time T (β = 0.12), of maternal supportiveness at time T + 1 (β = 0.08), and of the child's intelligence at T + 1 (β = 0.49) on the child's intelligence at T + 2, were all positive and statistically significant when adjusting for one another. However, it is known that such adjusted cross-lagged effects may be biased due to residual confounding and regression to the mean. In the present study, we fitted various models, including latent change score models, on data simulating the data used by Dunkel et al. We found discrepant effects. For example, a positive effect of supportiveness on subsequent increase in children's intelligence (β = 0.04) was accounted for by maternal intelligence (β = 0.01 after adjustment). Another effect indicated that low supportiveness may compensate for having a mother with low intelligence and allow children to achieve the same intelligence as children to more intelligent and supportive mothers (β = 0.34). These divergent findings suggested that it may be premature to assume an increasing effect of maternal supportiveness on children's intelligence. It is important for researchers to bear in mind that correlations, also in superficially more advanced forms like cross-lagged effects, do not prove causality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101815"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140755710","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-04-12DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101829
Elizabeth A.L. Stine-Morrow, Ilber E. Manavbasi, Shukhan Ng, Giavanna S. McCall, Aron K. Barbey, Daniel G. Morrow
{"title":"Looking for transfer in all the wrong places: How intellectual abilities can be enhanced through diverse experience among older adults","authors":"Elizabeth A.L. Stine-Morrow, Ilber E. Manavbasi, Shukhan Ng, Giavanna S. McCall, Aron K. Barbey, Daniel G. Morrow","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101829","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101829","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research with cognitive training for older adults has largely shown that benefits are confined to the skills that are directly practiced with little or no generalization (or “transfer”) to other skills. However, investigations typically rely on pre-post designs in which the effects of training on non-practiced skills can only be revealed in the initial encounter with the novel task after training. The principle of mutualism suggests that growth in one cognitive skill may potentiate plasticity in related skills, such that transfer may only emerge with practice on the novel skill. We introduce a successive enrichment paradigm in which learning on a target skill (here, working memory (WM)) is examined as a function of earlier training experiences. Older adults were randomly assigned to one of four groups who trained on different combinations of tasks before training on a verbal WM task. Practice with any combination of WM tasks accelerated learning of the target task relative to a verbal decision speed control. Furthermore, those who first practiced multiple WM span tasks that were different from the target task showed larger pre- to posttest gain on the target WM task relative to those with prior exposure to only one different WM task or even the exact same WM task as the target. However, these effects only emerged with practice on the novel task. These data provide support for the mutualism principle — a conceptualization of transfer that can explain the emergence of the positive manifold of cognitive abilities, and offers promise for new pathways to promote late-life cognitive health.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101829"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000230/pdfft?md5=bbef3cb8f65becd8afe21bebe8d4f234&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000230-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140548682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-04-09DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101833
Jacob Knyspel, Robert Plomin
{"title":"Comparing factor and network models of cognitive abilities using twin data","authors":"Jacob Knyspel, Robert Plomin","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101833","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101833","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Network models have become a popular alternative to factor models for analysing the phenotypic relationships among cognitive abilities. Studies have begun to compare these models directly to one another using cognitive ability data, although such a comparison has so far not extended to genetics. Our aim with this study was therefore to compare factor and network models of cognitive abilities first at a phenotypic level and then at a genetic level. We analyzed data from the Twins Early Development Study that were collected using 14 cognitive ability measures from 11,290 twins in the UK aged 12 years old. We conducted phenotypic and genetic analyses in which numerous factor and network models were tested, including a novel network twin model. Factor and network models both provided useful representations of the phenotypic and genetic relationships among cognitive abilities. Surprisingly, several relationships among cognitive abilities within the genetic networks were negative, which suggests that these cognitive abilities might share some genetic variants with inverse effects, although more research is currently needed to confirm this. Implications for future genomic research are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101833"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000278/pdfft?md5=4fce9f2261c54e23fad5b161a8d215c5&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000278-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140539967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
IntelligencePub Date : 2024-04-08DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2024.101832
Gilles E. Gignac , Eva T. Szodorai
{"title":"Defining intelligence: Bridging the gap between human and artificial perspectives","authors":"Gilles E. Gignac , Eva T. Szodorai","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2024.101832","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2024.101832","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Achieving a widely accepted definition of human intelligence has been challenging, a situation mirrored by the diverse definitions of artificial intelligence in computer science. By critically examining published definitions, highlighting both consistencies and inconsistencies, this paper proposes a refined nomenclature that harmonizes conceptualizations across the two disciplines. Abstract and operational definitions for human and artificial intelligence are proposed that emphasize maximal capacity for completing novel goals successfully through respective perceptual-cognitive and computational processes. Additionally, support for considering intelligence, both human and artificial, as consistent with a multidimensional model of capabilities is provided. The implications of current practices in artificial intelligence training and testing are also described, as they can be expected to lead to artificial achievement or expertise rather than artificial intelligence. Paralleling psychometrics, ‘AI metrics’ is suggested as a needed computer science discipline that acknowledges the importance of test reliability and validity, as well as standardized measurement procedures in artificial system evaluations. Drawing parallels with human general intelligence, artificial general intelligence (AGI) is described as a reflection of the shared variance in artificial system performances. We conclude that current evidence more greatly supports the observation of artificial achievement and expertise over artificial intelligence. However, interdisciplinary collaborations, based on common understandings of the nature of intelligence, as well as sound measurement practices, could facilitate scientific innovations that help bridge the gap between artificial and human-like intelligence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"104 ","pages":"Article 101832"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289624000266/pdfft?md5=63c65fbb7e59d45a51e8f055e92ca453&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289624000266-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140536691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}