Affective science最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Within-Person Dynamics of Job Boredom and Counterproductive Work Behavior: A Latent Change Score Modeling Approach 工作厌倦和工作反作用行为的人际动态:潜在变化分数建模法》。
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y
JeongJin Kim, Seth A. Kaplan, John A. Aitken, Lida P. Ponce
{"title":"Within-Person Dynamics of Job Boredom and Counterproductive Work Behavior: A Latent Change Score Modeling Approach","authors":"JeongJin Kim,&nbsp;Seth A. Kaplan,&nbsp;John A. Aitken,&nbsp;Lida P. Ponce","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00256-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Job boredom is one of the most common negative affective states experienced in the workplace, yet also among the least well-understood. One stream of research suggests that employees frequently react to job boredom by engaging in counterproductive work behaviors (CWB). However, recent studies show the converse—that engaging in CWB relates to job boredom. As studies on the job boredom-CWB relationship primarily have been cross-sectional and at the between-person level of analysis, the directionality between these constructs remains in question. Therefore, research examining the within-person dynamics of job boredom and CWB within a short timeframe is needed. In the current study, we explore whether job boredom influences subsequent changes in CWB and vice versa. We examined these relationships using latent change score (LCS) modeling with 10-day experience sampling data (<i>N</i> = 120 individuals providing 1,161 observations). Findings supported a reciprocal relationship. Employees’ level of job boredom on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in CWB on the next day, and the level of CWB on a given day was associated with a subsequent increase in job boredom on the next day. We discuss the implications of our findings, study limitations, and future research directions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"273 - 279"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11461387/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC) and Anxiety in Adolescence: Preference for CMC, Social Media Burden, and Attention Bias to Threat 计算机媒介传播与青少年焦虑:计算机媒介传播偏好、社交媒体负担和威胁注意偏向
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00253-1
Jennifer Lynn de Rutte, Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary, Amy K. Roy
{"title":"Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC) and Anxiety in Adolescence: Preference for CMC, Social Media Burden, and Attention Bias to Threat","authors":"Jennifer Lynn de Rutte,&nbsp;Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary,&nbsp;Amy K. Roy","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00253-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00253-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite societal and empirical interest in the impact of computer-mediated communication (CMC) on anxiety in adolescents, little is known about the associations between specific aspects of CMC use and anxiety severity and the role of individual vulnerability factors. In this study, we examined the links between two contexts of CMC, preference for CMC over face-to-face interactions and perceived social media social media burden, along with an anxiety-related cognitive vulnerability factor and attention bias to threat. Participants were mildly to severely anxious 12- to 14-year-olds (<i>N</i> = 78, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 12.89, 55% female). They self-reported on CMC preferences and social media burden and on anxiety symptoms in two domains (generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and social anxiety) and completed an eye-tracking assessment of attention bias. We tested the hypothesis that preferring CMC over face-to-face communications and perceiving greater social media burden would predict more severe anxiety symptoms, particularly among those with greater attention bias to threat. As predicted, greater feelings of social media burden predicted more severe anxiety symptom severity (GAD only) but only among those with greater attention bias to threat. The potential role of attention bias in associations between CMC and adolescent anxiety and the specificity of effects on GAD symptom severity is discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"377 - 388"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142778207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complex Emotion Dynamics Contribute to the Prediction of Depression: A Machine Learning and Time Series Feature Extraction Approach 复杂情绪动态有助于预测抑郁症:机器学习和时间序列特征提取方法。
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-08-03 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x
Mackenzie Zisser, Jason Shumake, Christopher G. Beevers
{"title":"Complex Emotion Dynamics Contribute to the Prediction of Depression: A Machine Learning and Time Series Feature Extraction Approach","authors":"Mackenzie Zisser,&nbsp;Jason Shumake,&nbsp;Christopher G. Beevers","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00249-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotion dynamics have demonstrated mixed ability to predict depressive symptoms and outperform traditional metrics like the mean and standard deviation of emotion reports. Here, we expand the types of emotion dynamic features used in prior work and apply a machine learning algorithm to predict depression symptoms. We obtained seven ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies from previous work on depression and emotion dynamics (<i>N</i> = 890). These studies measured self-reported sadness, positive affect, and negative affect 5 to 10 times per day for 7 to 21 days (schedule varied across studies). These data were fed through a feature extraction routine to generate hundreds of emotion dynamic features. A gradient boosting machine (GBM) using all available emotion dynamics features was the best of all models assessed. This model’s out-of-sample prediction (<i>R</i><sup>2</sup><sub>pred</sub>) for depression severity ranged from .20 to .44 depending on EMA interpolation method and samples included in the analysis. It also explained significantly more variance than a benchmark model of individuals’ mean emotion ratings over the assessment period, <i>R</i><sup>2</sup><sub>pred</sub> = .089. Comprehensive feature mining of emotion dynamics obtained during EMA may be necessary to identify processes that predict depression symptoms beyond mean emotion ratings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"259 - 272"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402161","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Media Activities and Affective Well-being in the Daily Life of Emerging Adults 新兴成人日常生活中的社交媒体活动与情感幸福感
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00251-3
Giselle Ferguson, Mariah T. Hawes, Jacqueline Mogle, Stacey B. Scott, Daniel N. Klein
{"title":"Social Media Activities and Affective Well-being in the Daily Life of Emerging Adults","authors":"Giselle Ferguson,&nbsp;Mariah T. Hawes,&nbsp;Jacqueline Mogle,&nbsp;Stacey B. Scott,&nbsp;Daniel N. Klein","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00251-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00251-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous work suggests that some social media (SM) activities may have detrimental effects on users’ affective well-being, whereas other activities can be more adaptive. SM use is typically assessed with global or retrospective measures; it remains unclear how its relation with affect may play out in real-time and in regard to specific SM activities, as opposed to general SM use. The current study investigated the association between specific SM activities (posting, viewing others’ posts, liking/commenting, checking replies to one’s own posts, direct messaging) and concurrent positive and negative affect in a sample of <i>n</i> = 349 18-year-old emerging adults. Participants reported SM activities and affect up to five times per day for 14 days. Using parallel multilevel models, we found significant within-person associations between positive affect and certain SM activities: participants’ positive affect was lower at times when they reported liking/commenting or viewing, and was higher when they reported direct messaging or posting, than at times when they were not engaging in these SM activities. In between-persons, only viewing was related to positive affect; individuals who more frequently viewed others’ posts had lower positive affect on average. Negative affect did not relate to any SM activities within-persons or between-persons. In sum, these results suggest specificity—in which SM activities link with affective well-being, in the correlational direction of those links, and in links mostly with positive affect—and that effects unfold within-persons in daily life but may not be detectable in terms of individual differences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"358 - 365"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-024-00251-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142778174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in Online Moral Discourse About Public Figures During #MeToo #MeToo运动期间关于公众人物的网络道德话语的变化
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00250-4
Benjamin M. Silver, Kevin N. Ochsner
{"title":"Changes in Online Moral Discourse About Public Figures During #MeToo","authors":"Benjamin M. Silver,&nbsp;Kevin N. Ochsner","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00250-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00250-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>During the #MeToo movement, the perceived morality of public figures changed in light of sexual assault allegations against them. Here, we asked how these changes were influenced by the perceived severity of alleged actions and by how well-known and well-liked were the public figures. Perceived morality was assessed by measuring (im)moral language usage in 1.4 million tweets about 50 male public figures accused of sexual assault. Using natural language processing to analyze the tweets, we found that liking of public figures mitigated perceived immorality for less severe allegations, but had little effect on perceived immorality for more severe allegations. The persistence of negative perceptions 1 year later was related to liking and familiarity for the public figure, not allegation severity. These results suggest that in real-world contexts, we can forgive less harmful actions for people we like, but may not be able to if their actions are more harmful; over time, however, liking for others predicts lasting negative impressions of their moral misdeeds.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"346 - 357"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142778173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Emergence of Organized Emotion Dynamics in Childhood 童年时期有组织情绪动态的出现。
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y
Mira L. Nencheva, Erik C. Nook, Mark A. Thornton, Casey Lew-Williams, Diana I. Tamir
{"title":"The Emergence of Organized Emotion Dynamics in Childhood","authors":"Mira L. Nencheva,&nbsp;Erik C. Nook,&nbsp;Mark A. Thornton,&nbsp;Casey Lew-Williams,&nbsp;Diana I. Tamir","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00248-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotions change from one moment to the next. They have a <i>duration</i> from seconds to hours and then <i>transition</i> to other emotions. Here, we describe the early ontology of these key aspects of emotion dynamics. In five cross-sectional studies (<i>N</i> = 904) combining parent surveys and ecological momentary assessment, we characterize how caregivers’ perceptions of children’s emotion duration and transitions change over the first 5 years of life and how they relate to children’s language development. Across these ages, the duration of children’s emotions increased, and emotion transitions became increasingly organized by valence, such that children were more likely to transition between similarly valenced emotions. Children with more mature emotion profiles also had larger vocabularies and could produce more emotion labels. These findings advance our understanding of emotion and communication by highlighting their intertwined nature in development and by charting how dynamic features of emotion experiences change over the first years of life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"246 - 258"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within-Person Fluctuations in Objective Smartphone Use and Emotional Processes During Adolescence: An Intensive Longitudinal Study 青春期客观智能手机使用和情感过程的人际波动:强化纵向研究
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00247-z
Alexandra M. Rodman, Jason A. Burns, Grace K. Cotter, Yuri-Grace B. Ohashi, Rachael K. Rich, Katie A. McLaughlin
{"title":"Within-Person Fluctuations in Objective Smartphone Use and Emotional Processes During Adolescence: An Intensive Longitudinal Study","authors":"Alexandra M. Rodman,&nbsp;Jason A. Burns,&nbsp;Grace K. Cotter,&nbsp;Yuri-Grace B. Ohashi,&nbsp;Rachael K. Rich,&nbsp;Katie A. McLaughlin","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00247-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00247-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Since the advent of smartphones, peer interactions over digital platforms have become a primary mode of socializing among adolescents. Despite the rapid rise in digital social activity, it remains unclear how this dramatic shift has impacted adolescent social and emotional experiences. In an intensive, longitudinal design (<i>N</i> = 26, <i>n</i> = 206 monthly observations for up to 12 months, 12–17 years), we used digital phenotyping methods to objectively measure within-person fluctuations in smartphone use (screen time, pickups, notifications) across different categories (social media, communication, entertainment, games) and examined their prospective, bidirectional associations with positive and negative mood. Bayesian hierarchical models showed that when adolescents reported better mood than usual, they subsequently spent more time on communication apps and launched social media and communication apps upon pickup less often. Meanwhile, when adolescents used entertainment apps more than usual, they subsequently reported improved mood. These preliminary findings suggest a pattern where fluctuations in mood relate to subsequent changes in smartphone use that are primarily social, whereas the fluctuations in smartphone use relating to subsequent changes in mood were primarily entertainment-related. We found little evidence that within-person fluctuations in screen time or social media use were associated with increases in negative mood, as frequently theorized. These findings highlight the importance of disentangling the distinct components of smartphone use that relate to affective processes and examining their bidirectional, prospective relationships over time, due to the possibility of differential outcomes. This work is a necessary first step in identifying targets for intervention efforts promoting resilience and wellbeing during adolescence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"332 - 345"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-024-00247-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141806695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
YouTube Kids: Understanding Gender and Emotion through Modern Media YouTube儿童:通过现代媒体理解性别和情感
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00246-0
Lauren T. Lyles, Leslie A. Frankel, Julie C. Dunsmore
{"title":"YouTube Kids: Understanding Gender and Emotion through Modern Media","authors":"Lauren T. Lyles,&nbsp;Leslie A. Frankel,&nbsp;Julie C. Dunsmore","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00246-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00246-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This project examines associations of gender and emotions in videos on YouTube Kids, a virtual environment for socialization during middle childhood. We selected YouTube Kids because of its popularity, newness, and absence in previous research on emotion socialization. The top 20 recommended videos were sampled and coded for gender and emotional content, by two independent teams of coders. This procedure was replicated for a total of 40 videos and 689 characters. By assessing gender and emotion content at both the video and character levels, we found feminine videos and characters depict more positive emotionality than their masculine counterparts. Furthermore, characters presented without any gender-typed information displayed significantly less emotionality than feminine, masculine, and androgynous characters. Emotionality is a gendered aspect of media representations. Nonetheless, evolving forms of media have potential as an avenue towards gender-fair socialization of emotions by proactively presenting and promoting equal representations of people and emotions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"321 - 331"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142778352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bidirectional Associations of Adolescents’ Momentary Social Media Use and Negative Emotions 青少年瞬时社交媒体使用与负性情绪的双向关联
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00244-2
Tyler Colasante, Katie Faulkner, Dana Kharbotli, Tina Malti, Tom Hollenstein
{"title":"Bidirectional Associations of Adolescents’ Momentary Social Media Use and Negative Emotions","authors":"Tyler Colasante,&nbsp;Katie Faulkner,&nbsp;Dana Kharbotli,&nbsp;Tina Malti,&nbsp;Tom Hollenstein","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00244-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00244-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Public discourse and empirical studies have predominantly focused on the negative repercussions of social media on adolescents’ mental health. However, pervasive social media use is a relatively new phenomenon—its apparent harms have been widely accepted before sufficient longitudinal and experimental research has been conducted. The present study used an intensive longitudinal design (four assessments/day × 14 days; <i>N</i> = 154 12- to 15-year-olds (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 13.47, <i>SD</i> = 0.58); <i>N</i> = 6,240 valid measurement occasions) to test the directionality of social media–negative emotion links in early adolescence, accounting for the type of social media usage (i.e., browsing vs. posting). The significance of effects depended on social media type: browsing predicted higher-than-usual negative emotions hours later, whereas no significant directional effects emerged for posting. The browsing effect was small but held after controlling for prior levels of negative emotions. It did not replicate concurrently, underscoring the importance of process-oriented designs with mental health symptoms tested shortly <i>after</i> passive social media usage. The results partially support the active-passive hypothesis, which singles out passively engaging with others’ curated social media content as most detrimental to mental health. Nonetheless, the small browsing effect and overall null-leaning pattern of effects imply that mediators and moderators are needed to further understand when using social media is problematic, beneficial, or neither.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 4","pages":"300 - 309"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-024-00244-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142778350","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
You are How You Eat: Foraging Behavior as a Potential Novel Marker of Rat Affective State 你是怎么吃的觅食行为是大鼠情感状态的潜在新标记。
IF 2.1
Affective science Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4
Vikki Neville, Emily Finnegan, Elizabeth S. Paul, Molly Davidson, Peter Dayan, Michael Mendl
{"title":"You are How You Eat: Foraging Behavior as a Potential Novel Marker of Rat Affective State","authors":"Vikki Neville,&nbsp;Emily Finnegan,&nbsp;Elizabeth S. Paul,&nbsp;Molly Davidson,&nbsp;Peter Dayan,&nbsp;Michael Mendl","doi":"10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-024-00242-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Effective and safe foraging requires animals to behave according to the expectations they have about the rewards, threats, and costs in their environment. Since these factors are thought to be reflected in the animals’ affective states, we can use foraging behavior as a window into those states. In this study, rats completed a foraging task in which they had repeatedly to decide whether to continue to harvest a food source despite increasing time costs, or to forgo food to switch to a different food source. Rats completed this task across two experiments using manipulations designed to induce both positive and negative, and shorter- and longer- term changes in affective state: removal and return of enrichment (Experiment 1), implementation and reversal of an unpredictable housing treatment (Experiment 1), and delivery of rewards (tickling or sucrose) and punishers (air-puff or back-handling) immediately prior to testing (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, rats completed fewer trials and were more prone to switching between troughs when housed in standard, compared to enriched, housing conditions. In Experiment 2, rats completed more trials following pre-test tickling compared to pre-test sucrose delivery. However, we also found that they were prone to disengaging from the task, suggesting they were really choosing between three options: ‘harvest’, ‘switch’, or ‘not work’. This limits the straightforward interpretation of the results. At present, foraging behavior within the context of this task cannot reliably be used as an indicator of an affective state in animals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"5 3","pages":"232 - 245"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11461729/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142402169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信