Venera Gashaj , Qendresa Thaqi , Fred W. Mast , Claudia M. Roebers
{"title":"Foundations for future math achievement: Early numeracy, home learning environment, and the absence of math anxiety","authors":"Venera Gashaj , Qendresa Thaqi , Fred W. Mast , Claudia M. Roebers","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100217","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100217","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Mathematics achievement is pivotal in shaping children's future prospects. Cognitive skills (numeracy), feelings (anxiety), and the social environment (home learning environment) influence early math development.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>A longitudinal study involved 85 children (mean age T1 = 6.4 years; T2 = 7.9) to explore these predictors holistically. Data were collected on early numeracy skills, home learning environment, math anxiety, and their impact on various aspects of math.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The study found that early numeracy skills, home learning environment, and math anxiety significantly influenced math school achievement. However, they affected written computation, sequences, and comparisons differently. Early numeracy skills strongly predicted overall achievement and comparison subtest performance.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>These findings underscore the substantial role of math anxiety and home learning environment in children's math achievement. The study emphasizes the need to consider the selective impacts of these factors in future research, shedding light on the multifaceted nature of mathematics achievement determinants.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100217"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2211949323000200/pdfft?md5=4a0181a49d808d23cc687f20f6af3c05&pid=1-s2.0-S2211949323000200-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92131801","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Nuria Vita-Barrull , Verónica Estrada-Plana , Jaume March-Llanes , Núria Guzmán , Carlos Fernández-Muñoz , Rosa Ayesa , Jorge Moya-Higueras
{"title":"Board game-based intervention to improve executive functions and academic skills in rural schools: A randomized controlled trial","authors":"Nuria Vita-Barrull , Verónica Estrada-Plana , Jaume March-Llanes , Núria Guzmán , Carlos Fernández-Muñoz , Rosa Ayesa , Jorge Moya-Higueras","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100216","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100216","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>This research intends to increase the knowledge about the use of board games in the classroom to train executive functions and academic skills. 99 children from rural schools were assessed in executive functions and academic skills.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Through a randomized controlled trial, they were assigned to a playing group (<em>n</em> = 51) and an active control group (regular classes without games, <em>n</em> = 48). Play program consisted of 12 sessions for 6 weeks with eight commercial board games.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>In flexibility, the playing group was significantly faster after the program (<em>p</em>= = .01, <em>d</em> = 0.76), but not the control group (<em>p</em> = .23; <em>d</em> = 0.35). Both groups improved in the academic tasks, but the significance in calculus was greater in the playing group (<em>p</em> = .00; <em>d</em> = 2.19) than in the control group (<em>p</em> = .01; <em>d</em> = 0.97).</p></div><div><h3>Discusion</h3><p>The use of board games during school hours could be as good or better methodology for cognitive training and learning academic skills than regular classes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100216"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92122470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Verónica Adriana Ramírez , Sebastián Javier Lipina , Eliana Ruetti
{"title":"Cognitive and emotional processing in tasks with emotional valence: Analysis of age and gender role on child development variations","authors":"Verónica Adriana Ramírez , Sebastián Javier Lipina , Eliana Ruetti","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100212","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100212","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cognitive control consists of high-level cognitive processes regulating thoughts and actions during goal-directed behavior and problem-solving. This study analyzes the performance of 4- to 8-year-old children in Stroop-like and ToL tasks using stimuli with different emotional valence. Significant differences were found in the performance in the congruent block of the Stroop-like task, where 5-year-old children presented a higher performance in the neutral condition. Also, a significant difference was only found in the incongruent block (with higher demand for inhibition), which indicates that girls performed better than boys in both task conditions. Variations in the Stroop-like task performance were found in preschoolers but not in older children, especially in girls than in boys. Specifically, these variations were found between age groups with at least two years of difference. No statistically significant differences were found in performance nor planning time in ToL between the age and gender groups in any of the task conditions. The findings highlight the need to analyze the interaction between cognitive and emotional processing, individual differences, and task demands.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100212"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49737574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Mirna Nachouki, Elfadil A. Mohamed, Riyadh Mehdi, Mahmoud Abou Naaj
{"title":"Student course grade prediction using the random forest algorithm: Analysis of predictors' importance","authors":"Mirna Nachouki, Elfadil A. Mohamed, Riyadh Mehdi, Mahmoud Abou Naaj","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100214","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100214","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Universities need to find strategies for improving student retention rates. Predicting student academic performance enables institutions to identify underachievers and take appropriate actions to increase student completion and lower dropout rates.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>In this work, we proposed a model based on random forest methodology to predict students' course performance using seven input predictors and find their relative importance in determining the course grade. Seven predictors were derived from transcripts and recorded data from 650 undergraduate computing students.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Our findings indicate that grade point average and high school score were the two most significant predictors of a course grade. The course category and class attendance percentage have equal importance. Course delivery mode does not have a significant effect.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our findings show that courses students at risk find challenging can be identified, and appropriate actions, procedures, and policies can be taken.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100214"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49724794","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Delfina María Martelletti , Mariana Luzuriaga , Melina Furman
{"title":"‘What makes you say so?’ Metacognition improves the sustained learning of inferential reading skills in English as a second language","authors":"Delfina María Martelletti , Mariana Luzuriaga , Melina Furman","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100213","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100213","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>This quasi-experimental study investigates the impact of enhancing metacognition in learning inferential reading skills in English as a second language.</p></div><div><h3>Procedures</h3><p>Six Grade 4 classes were randomly assigned to two groups. The “Control group” received an instructional unit on inferential reading skills. The “Metacognition group” received the same unit, including metacognitive activities. Students were assessed in metacognitive and inferential reading skills before (pre-test), immediately after (post-test) and four weeks after the intervention (deferred test).</p></div><div><h3>Findings</h3><p>Metacognitive strategy instruction enhanced student learning of inferential reading skills and its sustainability in time. The Metacognition group attained a significantly higher average score in deploying metacognitive skills both in the post and deferred tests, indicating that the intervention was effective, as intended, to this end. While both groups significantly improved their proficiency in inferential reading skills after working with the provided instructional unit, there was a significant difference in the Metacognition group, which outperformed the Control one, even more strongly in the deferred test.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Findings support the importance of deliberately promoting metacognition as it positively impacts learning outcomes and sustainability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100213"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49751674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jack S. Fogarty , Alicia M. Goodwill , Aik Lim Tan , S.H. Jessica Tan
{"title":"Student arousal, engagement, and emotion relative to Physical Education periods in school","authors":"Jack S. Fogarty , Alicia M. Goodwill , Aik Lim Tan , S.H. Jessica Tan","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100215","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100215","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Exercise has transient effects on cognition and mood, however the impact of Physical Education (PE) on cognitive and affective processes across the school day has not been examined.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>This study used wearables and questionnaires to track student arousal, engagement, and emotion across school days/periods following PE. Skin conductance, heart rate, heart rate variability, and self-reported engagement, arousal, and valence were analyzed for 23 students (age 15–17 years) on days with and without PE.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Sympathetic arousal was significantly higher for two hours following PE and there were stronger decreases in arousal across other classes relative to days without PE. On days with PE, engagement decreased, whereas valence increased from morning to afternoon.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>These findings highlight the importance of considering acute effects of PE on learning across the entire school day, and demonstrates the feasibility of wearables to clarify how the timing of PE could positively or negatively affect self-regulation and learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100215"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49724790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"The interplay between math performances, spatial abilities, and affective factors: The role of task","authors":"Sarit Ashkenazi, Hagar Velner","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100211","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100211","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many studies have suggested that cognitive and affective abilities (such as math anxiety- MA and math self-efficacy) explain individual differences in math.</p></div><div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>The present study explores the interplay between MA, math self-efficacy, spatial anxiety and spatial abilities in explaining individual differences on two complex math tasks.</p></div><div><h3>Procedures</h3><p>Ninety-three college students took part in the experiment and completed 3 emotional questionnaires, in addition to 2 math tasks and a mental rotation task.</p></div><div><h3>Findings</h3><p>The interplay between math performances and cognitive and affective factors is related to task demand. MA and spatial abilities affected math performances directly, regardless of task. Spatial anxiety had only an indirect effect on math performances via MA, regardless of task.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These finding suggest that for math performances, contrary to MA, real spatial abilities rather than perceived spatial anxiety play a significant role in explaining individual differences. Hence, the present result dissociates cognitive and emotional factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100211"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49737569","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Balancing academics and athletics: School-level athletes’ results are positively associated with their academic performance","authors":"Yuta Kuroda , Toru Ishihara , Keita Kamijo","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100210","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Using data from a nationwide high-school sample, the present study aimed to examine whether school-level athletes’ sporting results are positively or negatively associated with their academic performance.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Athletic performance was assessed using the results of spring and summer regional qualifying interschool tennis tournaments in Japan among both male and female tournaments across all prefectures in Japan. Academic performance was assessed using the standardized rank scores for academic performance at the school level, with higher scores denoting superior academic levels; this is the most common measure of academic performance in Japanese high schools. Linear mixed models were conducted to compare the academic performance of high schools with winning and losing records, respectively, for all games together and for the spring and summer tournaments separately. The results of a total of 4,870 games were analyzed.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>High schools with winning records showed a significantly higher academic-performance score than those with losing records, and this association was stronger among boys than girls. The observed difference in academic-performance scores between the schools with winning and losing records, respectively, was replicated in both tournament settings (i.e., spring and summer).</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Athletic results are positively associated with academic performance at the school level. Given the cross-sectional nature of the present study, the present results do not necessarily imply that sports participation can improve academic performance; rather, the present study suggests that balancing academic and athletic commitments is feasible.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100210"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49751679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Joshua A. Cuevas , Gina Childers , Bryan L. Dawson
{"title":"A rationale for promoting cognitive science in teacher education: Deconstructing prevailing learning myths and advancing research-based practices","authors":"Joshua A. Cuevas , Gina Childers , Bryan L. Dawson","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100209","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2023.100209","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>Cognitive science is essential to designing, implementing, and evaluating instruction for enhancing student learning. However, there may not be sufficient focus on the principles of cognitive science, as some educators hold learning beliefs that may be considered cognitive myths.</p></div><div><h3>Procedures</h3><p>This review article analyzes examples of five <em>learning myths</em> (learning styles, pure discovery learning, digital natives, extrinsic motivation, multitasking) and five <em>research-based learning strategies</em> (dual coding, direct instruction, summarization, retrieval practice, spacing). It details the research evidence for each to explain those misconceptions of learning and also those underutilized or misunderstood but effective strategies shown to benefit student learning.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Educational practices related to learning myths are widespread in education with potentially detrimental effects on student learning. We recommend that colleges of education be restructured to ensure greater emphasis on cognitive science in educator preparation programs to better promote research-based instructional strategies to meet students’ learning needs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"33 ","pages":"Article 100209"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49751688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Decoding of spatial proportions using somatosensory feedback in sighted and visually impaired children","authors":"Christian Leukel , Timo Leuders , Juliane Leuders","doi":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100207","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.tine.2023.100207","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background and purpose</h3><p>Humans can naturally operate with ratios of continuous magnitudes (proportions). We asked if sighted children (S) and visually impaired children (VI) can discriminate proportions via somatosensory feedback.</p></div><div><h3>Procedures</h3><p>Children formed a proportion by tracing a pair of straight lines with their finger, and compared this proportion with a second proportion resulting from the tracing of another pair of lines.</p></div><div><h3>Main findings</h3><p>Performance was 68% in S, thus significantly lower (<em>p</em> < 0.001) compared to VI (75%). Tracing velocity (<em>p</em> < 0.01) and trial-to-trial variability of tracing velocity (<em>p</em> < 0.05) was higher in S compared to VI.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Operating with proportions solely from somatosensory feedback is possible, thus tracing lines might support learning in mathematics education. Kinematic variables point to the reason for the difference between S and VI, in that higher trial-to-trial variability in velocity in S leads to biased estimation of absolute line lengths.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":46228,"journal":{"name":"Trends in Neuroscience and Education","volume":"32 ","pages":"Article 100207"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10221012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}