Behavioural Processes最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Ambient light spectrum affects larval Mexican jumping bean moth (Cydia saltitans) behavior despite light obstruction from host seed 尽管寄主种子阻挡了光线,但环境光谱仍会影响幼虫墨西哥跳豆蛾(Cydia saltitans)的行为。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093
{"title":"Ambient light spectrum affects larval Mexican jumping bean moth (Cydia saltitans) behavior despite light obstruction from host seed","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Spectral differences in ambient light can affect animal behavior and convey crucial information about an individual’s environment. The ability to perceive and respond to differences in ambient light varies widely by taxa and is shaped by a species’ ecology. Mexican jumping bean moths, <em>Cydia saltitans</em>, spend their entire larval period encased in fallen host seeds and contend with potentially lethal environmental temperatures when host seeds are in direct sunlight. We investigate if and how <em>C. saltitans</em> larvae in host seeds respond to lighting conditions associated with these thermal risks. In a temperature-controlled experiment, we identified that larvae demonstrated distinct behavioral (“jumping”) responses corresponding to four lighting treatments (white, red, green, and purple), despite extremely minimal light penetration through host seed walls. Red light induced the greatest larval activity (measured by probability of movement and by displacement from origin), suggesting that larvae have mechanisms to perceive low levels of red light and/or to detect subtle increases in heat produced by red/near infrared-biased light spectra, possibly providing them with an early-warning mechanism against thermal stress. Our findings highlight the interplay of environmental lighting, behavior, and potential thermosensory adaptations in a species with a visually constrained environment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142078951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Play contagion in African elephants: The closest, the better 非洲象的游戏传染:越接近越好。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092
{"title":"Play contagion in African elephants: The closest, the better","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Behavioural contagion is an automatic process through which a behaviour performed by an individual (trigger) is reproduced by an observer (responder) without necessarily replicating the exact motor sequence. It has been linked to inter-individual synchronisation and possibly emotional contagion. Play can convey emotions and enhance social bonding, although its contagiousness is understudied. To verify social play contagion presence and modulating factors, we gathered audio-video data on social play, distance and affiliation on a group of savannah African elephants (15 individuals) at Parque de la Naturaleza de Cabarceno (Cantabria, Spain). Social play was contagious as it was more likely started by uninvolved elephants (within 3-min) in Post-Play Condition (PP) - after that other elephants had started playing - than in Matched-control Condition (MC; no previous play). Social play contagion mostly occurred within 30 m – probably due to elephants’ limited visual acuity – and it was highest between individuals that affiliated the most, with the distance-affiliation interaction having no effect. The most prominent individuals in the social play network were also the most influential in the play contagion network (Eigenvector-centrality measure). Play contagion was socially modulated, thus suggesting it may extend from motor replication to the replication of the underlying affective state.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724001074/pdfft?md5=737d97f49b60a912f7e67d4d5b5f4a0e&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724001074-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sounds in common: Time-frequency as the classification parameters for pulsed sounds produced by Delphinus delphis 共同的声音:时间-频率作为海豚发出的脉冲声的分类参数。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-08-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091
{"title":"Sounds in common: Time-frequency as the classification parameters for pulsed sounds produced by Delphinus delphis","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Sounds produced by dolphins can be grouped into tonal (whistles) and pulsed sounds (e.g., echolocation clicks and burst sounds). Clicks are broadband pulses temporarily spaced to allow echo processing between the sound source and the object. Echolocation is related mainly to prey detection and environmental recognition. Echolocation click trains tend to present a decreasing inter-click interval due to a continuous changing of the target's location when the animal approaches to capture the prey. In addition to foraging and feeding contexts, burst pulsed sounds have been associated with short social communication. Although echolocation clicks are relatively well documented, there is no consensus regarding the broad variety of the burst pulsed signals. The present study analyzed time-frequency characteristics by conducting a clustering and discrimination analysis to classify pulsed sounds. A total of 64 click trains were analyzed from short-beaked common dolphins recorded in the slope region of the western South Atlantic Ocean. Three analyses (time; frequency; and combined time-frequency parameters) were compared through k-means clustering and posterior cluster validation using Random forest analysis. The k-means clustering resulted in four clusters for all groups of analysis. The time parameters were the most accurate among the comparisons, with the first two-dimensional axis corresponding to 87 % (Dim1 = 70.2 % and Dim2 = 17.2 %). The random forest analysis showed that the time-frequency dataset was the best classification of pulsed sounds in <em>D. delphis</em> (Accuracy = 84.6 %; confidence interval CI = 65.1 %-95.6 %; p &lt; 0.01). This result considers the animal an acoustical identity, emphasizing the importance of certain parameters that influence this identity and thus reflecting the energy-cost optimization for sound production.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142035116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expression of exploratory activity of rat offspring depends on the expression of exploratory activity of their parents at the moment of mating 大鼠后代的探索活动表现取决于交配时父母的探索活动表现
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090
{"title":"Expression of exploratory activity of rat offspring depends on the expression of exploratory activity of their parents at the moment of mating","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The aim of this work was to study the, so far, unexplored possibility that non-genetic inheritance of animal behavioral characteristics could depend on the state of the parents at the time of conception. In this study, we measured the levels of motor and exploratory activity in rats at the ages of 2 and 5 months. Male and female rats were mated at the age of 5 months. The following groups were used: male and female rats with high motor activity at ages of 2 and 5 months (ACT+); male and female rats with high activity at the age of 2 months, but low activity at the age of 5 months (ACT–); male and female rats with low activity at the ages of 2 and 5 months (PAS–); male and female rats with low activity at the age of 2 months, but high activity at the age of 5 months (PAS+). It was found that both males and females ACT+ had significantly higher motor activity, which was observed in the first 10 minutes, in the next 20–60 minutes, in the center of the cage and more rearings as compared with PAS– rats. Significant differences in the severity of exploratory activity were found between the male offspring of ACT+ and ACT– rats. Differences between the offspring of PAS+ and PAS– rats were observed in both the male and female rats. The motor activity of animals in the period from 20 minutes after the start of registration did not differ between groups. Thus, it can be considered that individual characteristics of general motor activity are due to genetically inherited factors, while differences in the level of exploratory activity, apparently, are formed due to non-genetic influences from parents during mating.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141888331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Horses can learn to identify joy and sadness against other basic emotions from human facial expressions 马可以从人类的面部表情中学会识别喜悦和悲伤,以及其他基本情绪。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081
{"title":"Horses can learn to identify joy and sadness against other basic emotions from human facial expressions","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recently, horses and other domestic mammals have been shown to perceive and react to human emotional signals, with most studies focusing on joy and anger. In this study, we tested whether horses can learn to identify human joyful and sad expressions against other emotions. We used a touchscreen-based automated device that presented pairs of human portraits and distributed pellets when the horse touched the rewarded face. Six horses were trained to touch the sad face and 5 the joyful face. By the end of training, horses’ performances at the group level were significantly higher than chance level, with higher scores for horses trained with the sad face. At the individual level, evidence of task learning varied among horses, which could be explained by individual variations in horses’ ability to identify different human facial expressions or attention issues during the tests. In a generalization test, we introduced portraits of different humans than those presented during training. Horses trained with the joyful face performed better than chance, demonstrating generalization. Conversely, horses trained with the sad face did not. Horses also showed differences in learning performance according to the non-rewarded emotion, providing insights into horses’ cognitive processing of facial expressions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000962/pdfft?md5=4869edee356afb15282e228aff4f0f06&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000962-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141787190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resurgence mitigation across extended extinction following four and eight cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement 在四次和八次循环的开/关替代性强化后,经过延长的消亡期缓解了复发。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082
{"title":"Resurgence mitigation across extended extinction following four and eight cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Resurgence is an increase in an extinguished operant response resulting from a worsening of conditions (e.g., extinction) for a more recently reinforced alternative behavior. Previous research has shown that exposure to cycles of alternative reinforcement available versus unavailable (i.e., on/off alternative reinforcement) across sessions can reduce subsequent resurgence. Most previous assessments of the procedure have examined target operant responding during only single-session resurgence tests, and it remains unclear if exposure to relatively few cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement can maintain low rates of target behavior across extended exposure to extinction. This experiment with rats examined the effects of 4 or 8 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement on subsequent resurgence during a 10-session extinction test. The results show that exposure to 4 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement is as effective as 8 cycles in producing low rates of target behavior during treatment and across extended extinction. This result is consistent with extant theories of resurgence and suggests that on/off alternative reinforcement could have translational utility following relatively few cycles of exposure.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141787191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of collective behaviour in fish response to visual cues 集体行为在鱼类对视觉线索的反应中的作用。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079
{"title":"The role of collective behaviour in fish response to visual cues","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated the influence of group size (individual, groups of five, and 20) on the response of common minnow to visual cues created by vertical black and white stripes over time. The stripes were displayed on a monitor either at one end of an experimental tank, while the other was uniform white, or both ends simultaneously. Reponses were compared with a control (stripes absent). Visual cues were pseudo-randomly presented every 15-minutes over six-hours. Three predictions were made: first, due to more efficient flow of information, larger groups would respond more rapidly (<em>Rate of response</em>) to the visual cues. Second, assuming visual cues provide a proxy for structure and larger groups experience greater benefits of group membership due to reduced predatory risk, there will be stronger association (<em>Strength of association</em> and <em>Final association</em>) with stripes for individuals and smaller groups compared with larger groups. Consequently, the association with visual cues exhibited by larger groups would diminish over time compared to smaller, more risk averse groups. As expected, larger groups exhibited a faster <em>Rate of response</em> to visual cues, and individual fish a greater <em>Strength of association</em> compared with the largest group size. <em>Final association</em>, however, was more common for larger groups compared to both smaller groups and individuals. Contrary to the final prediction, responses to visual cues did not decrease over time for any group size, suggesting innate behaviour or an experimental duration insufficient to observe habituation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000949/pdfft?md5=e00587c944c5d6103f238ad35ac15bf7&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000949-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141722928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rapid prey manipulation and bite location preferences in three species of wandering spiders 三种游走蜘蛛的快速猎物操纵和咬合位置偏好。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083
{"title":"Rapid prey manipulation and bite location preferences in three species of wandering spiders","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Predator-prey interactions are the interspecific relationships of greatest interest in ecology. Spiders are among the most diverse and ubiquitous terrestrial predators on the planet. Their large dietary breadth is often linked with the development of specific predatory behaviors and morphological adaptations. However, studies on the predatory behavior of spiders have mostly focused on specialist species, leaving behind the ethological variability occurring in generalist species that allow them to respond to the different prey types. For three species of generalist wandering spiders, we searched images of predation events on the Internet to determine the most common prey. Subsequently, the focal predator species were then used in behavioral experiments. Using high-speed videos, handling patterns for different prey types (spider and cricket) were analyzed. Our results show a notable difference in handling patterns between prey types. We found that the spider prey was often rotated around the axis allowing the predator to bite in the ventral region of the prey and thus avoid a counterattack. Contrary, crickets were arbitrarily rotated. Our work may be an indication that these three species of generalist spiders have a preference for manipulating prey differently with a preference to rotate spiders, allowing them to exploit prey with various defensive mechanisms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141878300","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying an eye tracking technique to gibbons: First study using scanpath measurements for visual stimuli 将眼动跟踪技术应用于长臂猿:首次使用扫描路径测量视觉刺激的研究。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080
{"title":"Applying an eye tracking technique to gibbons: First study using scanpath measurements for visual stimuli","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Compared to the abundance of research on cognition in various nonhuman primate species, studies of gibbons -- often called “the small apes” -- remain limited, despite the importance of gibbons for understanding evolutionary processes in humans and other apes. Over the past decade, eye tracking techniques have been established in chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates using the free-participation method, which requires no physical restraint of the subjects. We investigated the feasibility of using the same method to record visual scanpaths in gibbons. We attempted to measure the eye movements of three adult gibbons while they spontaneously viewed images, with no prior fixation training. Calibration was successful in all three individuals, with errors of less than one degree. In total, 24 stimuli were used, with landscape and nonhuman primate face photographs presented on one-quarter of the screen, to test the prediction that gibbons would change their viewing time depending on image category. All three gibbons viewed the images for longer than the background, and primate face images for longer than landscapes. These results are consistent with previous findings in other primate species that faces attract more attention than non-face stimuli, suggesting that this effect is common across primates. This study demonstrates the feasibility of using eye tracking with gibbons. Further studies on gibbon visual exploration and cognition may enhance our understanding of the phylogenetic origins of hominid intelligence as well as the unique evolution of gibbons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141756859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distribution of brood of the acorn ant Temnothorax crassispinus in artificial nests after forced migration 橡子蚁 Temnothorax crassispinus 被迫迁徙后人工巢中雏鸟的分布。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学
Behavioural Processes Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077
Sławomir Mitrus
{"title":"Distribution of brood of the acorn ant Temnothorax crassispinus in artificial nests after forced migration","authors":"Sławomir Mitrus","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Nest sites are important for social insects, as they provide refuge against enemies and ensure optimal conditions for the brood development. In large nests, the different chambers can be used for different reasons; for example, for food storage or as a brood chamber. Acorn ants from the genus <em>Temnothorax</em> dwell in small cavities in acorns and wood; however, even such small chambers can have a high degree of spatial heterogeneity. During this study, the distribution of brood items of the acorn ant <em>Temnothorax crassispinus</em> inside artificial nest cavities composed of three chambers in a linear system was analysed. 29 ant colonies were photographed 13 times during a period of approximately one month: during three consecutive days, and after forced migrations. I found that the distribution of the brood inside the nest cavity was similar during the consecutive days; however, after the forced migration, the distribution typically changed. Almost all the brood items were kept farther from the entrance. Keeping the brood farther from the entrance could be explained as a safer option.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000925/pdfft?md5=fc44114c5dd58dfb2d42d2e6ae534282&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000925-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141578890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信