Cognitive Development最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
The role of sharing and information type in children’s categorization of privileged and conventional information 共享和信息类型在儿童对特权信息和常规信息进行分类中的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-02-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420
Helana Girgis , Douglas A. Behrend
{"title":"The role of sharing and information type in children’s categorization of privileged and conventional information","authors":"Helana Girgis ,&nbsp;Douglas A. Behrend","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One domain that has not been thoroughly investigated is children’s ability to categorize information, specifically conventional (known to others, no restrictions on sharing) and privileged (not known to others, restrictions on sharing). In Study 1, 73 four- and five-year-olds and adults classified conventional and privileged information by how it is shared. All age groups accurately classified the information types, though accuracy improved with age. In Study 2, 68 four- and 6-year-olds and adults were presented with scenarios where information type and how it was shared did not match (privileged but shared) and asked if it was conventional or privileged. Four-year-olds and adults categorized conventional information by its label and did so more than 6-year-olds, while there was no pattern for privileged information. These results support that even 4-year-olds can distinguish between conventional and privileged information, and categorization strategies may differ across age and type of information.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101420"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139714228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschool children’s intuitions of parallelism 学龄前儿童的平行直觉
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423
Shaojing Gao , Qingfen Hu , Yi Shao
{"title":"Preschool children’s intuitions of parallelism","authors":"Shaojing Gao ,&nbsp;Qingfen Hu ,&nbsp;Yi Shao","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Existing evidence has revealed that geometrical intuitions develop spontaneously without formal education. However, empirical research exploring the development of specific intuitive geometric concepts, such as parallelism, is still lacking. To explore preschoolers’ intuition of parallelism, one of the most fundamental concepts in geometry, a series of deviant-detection items were presented to 96 3- to 5-year-old children. Children showed the ability to discriminate between parallel and intersecting lines at least from the age of 4. We grouped children as discriminators and non-discriminators based on their performance in discriminating parallel and intersecting lines without a cross point. For the children who succeeded in this discrimination, they could distinguish parallel lines from intersecting lines both with and without a visible cross point, although they were easily interfered with the visible cross point. Children also showed some naïve understanding of parallel curves and could extract common features from parallel lines and parallel curves, suggesting that the extension of their intuitions of parallelism might be more general than the definition in Euclidean geometry.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101423"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139699589","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of relational language during early childhood: Comprehension and production of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial labels 幼儿期关系语言的发展:心形、序数和空间标签的理解与制作
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421
Alycia M. Hund, Alexis R. Colwell
{"title":"The development of relational language during early childhood: Comprehension and production of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial labels","authors":"Alycia M. Hund,&nbsp;Alexis R. Colwell","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The goal was to specify the developmental trajectory of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial relational language comprehension and production. One hundred sixty-four 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old English-speaking children viewed a row of toy cars and were asked to place the appropriate car(s) into a toy garage based on the label provided (Give Me) or to produce the correct label for the specified car(s) (Tell Me). Children were tested using cardinal (one, three, five), ordinal (first, third, fifth), and spatial (front, middle, back) labels. Language performance improved with age, especially for spatial labels. Language performance was more accurate for cardinal labels than for spatial and ordinal labels. Performance was quite accurate for cardinal labels regardless of condition, whereas comprehension was higher than production for spatial and ordinal labels.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101421"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139669393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Variability in the relationship between parenting and executive functions: The role of environmental sensitivity 养育子女与执行功能之间关系的变异性:环境敏感性的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418
Niamh Oeri , Nora Tilda Kunz , Michael Pluess
{"title":"Variability in the relationship between parenting and executive functions: The role of environmental sensitivity","authors":"Niamh Oeri ,&nbsp;Nora Tilda Kunz ,&nbsp;Michael Pluess","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research suggests that children differ substantially in their sensitivity to positive and negative parenting qualities. In a Swiss sample of <em>N</em> = 264 (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub>: 6.0 years, 50.4% female, 15% migration background), we examined the interaction between parenting and children’s sensitivity on executive functions (EF). Results showed that EF performance tended to be higher for sensitive children whose parents reported more involved parenting and tended to be lower for sensitive children whose parents reported the use of corporal punishment. No such effects emerged for less sensitive children. The results suggest that parenting quality may be more strongly related to EF performance in more sensitive children compared to less sensitive children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101418"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000030/pdfft?md5=6c2099f942214479a3f1d28f3c6c7801&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000030-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139669855","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influences of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control in childhood 社会和非社会奖励对儿童认知控制的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413
Xiaoyu Jin , Da Zhang , Nicolas Chevalier
{"title":"Influences of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control in childhood","authors":"Xiaoyu Jin ,&nbsp;Da Zhang ,&nbsp;Nicolas Chevalier","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The modulation of cognitive control by rewards has long been discussed, but there is scarce evidence of how social and non-social rewards influence cognitive control in childhood, especially in the preschool years. Critically, sociality has often been confounded with other important reward dimensions (e.g., tangibility) in prior studies, hence potentially misestimating the effect of social rewards. Thus, the present study re-examined the effects of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control, particularly on proactive and reactive control engagement during childhood. Thirty 5- to 6-year-olds and thirty 9- to 10-year-olds completed an AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT) during an online session in three conditions: control, social reward, and non-social (i.e., monetary) reward conditions. Social rewards increased younger and older children’s response accuracy, suggesting greater cognitive control. However, no influence on how children engage cognitive control (i.e., proactively or reactively) was observed. The provision of non-social rewards did not influence cognitive performance in either group of children. When controlling for other reward dimension, we found evidence that social rewards, but not non-social rewards, can promote cognitive control performance in childhood.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101413"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139079131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rational number representation, math anxiety, and algebra performance in college students 大学生的有理数表示、数学焦虑和代数成绩
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417
Sangmi Park, Alena G. Esposito
{"title":"Rational number representation, math anxiety, and algebra performance in college students","authors":"Sangmi Park,&nbsp;Alena G. Esposito","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Understanding the magnitude of rational numbers is crucial for mathematical development. However, children do not readily integrate the quantity of fractions and decimals and are even less likely to show a linear representation of fractions and decimals. Thus, the current study examined whether college-aged individuals show spontaneous quantity integration across distinct notations of rational numbers, whether rational number representation would resemble a number line, and whether their rational number representation predicts algebra performance. Further, we examined whether math anxiety plays a role. We found that college students do develop linear representation integrating the quantity between fractions and decimals. Quantitative representation of rational numbers predicted higher algebra scores. Linearity was not a significant predictor for algebra performance. The relations remained even after controlling for math anxiety. Implications for numerical understanding as well as math achievement are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101417"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139561095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-numerical features fail to predict numerical performance in real-world stimuli 非数字特征无法预测真实世界刺激中的数字表现
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415
Emily M. Sanford, Justin Halberda
{"title":"Non-numerical features fail to predict numerical performance in real-world stimuli","authors":"Emily M. Sanford,&nbsp;Justin Halberda","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It has been proposed that humans use non-numerical features (such as convex hull and surface area) to estimate the number of objects in a scene. This would be an evolutionarily advantageous strategy if such features truly patterned with number in the world, but this has never been empirically tested. Here, we quantify the strength of the relationship between number and non-numerical features in two relevant image sets: the illustrations from children’s counting books, and real-world photographs. We find that non-numerical features are much less predictive of the number of objects in counting books than in photographs, despite the former being specifically designed for use in teaching children about numbers. Then, across three behavioral experiments, we ask whether the stronger relationship in photographs predicts better number estimation performance in adults (N = 120) and in children (N = 94; <em>M</em> age = 7;2 years). Our experiments reveal that number estimation is easier from the counting books than the photographs, even though non-numerical features are <em>less</em> predictive of number in books. This analysis uses real-world stimuli and draws into question the claim that non-numerical features are intrinsically involved in number extraction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101415"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S088520142300120X/pdfft?md5=aa4642a2fd32df13965f71913eab3090&pid=1-s2.0-S088520142300120X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139433904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding cognitive and language development in refugees: Evidence from displaced syrian children in Turkey 了解难民的认知和语言发展:来自土耳其流离失所的叙利亚儿童的证据
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412
Özlem Yeter , Ebru Evcen , Hugh Rabagliati , Duygu Özge
{"title":"Understanding cognitive and language development in refugees: Evidence from displaced syrian children in Turkey","authors":"Özlem Yeter ,&nbsp;Ebru Evcen ,&nbsp;Hugh Rabagliati ,&nbsp;Duygu Özge","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present study introduces systematic data on the cognitive and linguistic abilities of refugee children. We tested 9–10 year-old Syrian refugee children (<em>N</em><span> = 25) on their cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, shifting, inhibitory control, and fluid intelligence) and vocabulary knowledge in Arabic and Turkish. We compared their performance to two non-refugee control groups with low socioeconomic status, matched on age and mother’s education: Arabic-Turkish bilinguals (</span><em>N</em> = 29) and Turkish monolinguals (<em>N</em> = 19). Refugee children lagged behind both non-refugee groups in the fluid intelligence task. Compared to their bilingual peers, they showed poorer performance in working memory and shifting tasks. On the other hand, these scores matched their monolingual peers, with only slower performance in the shifting task. Greater exposure to trauma and poverty were predictors for lower scores in refugee children’s cognitive tasks. On the language tests, refugee children exhibited a smaller Turkish vocabulary size compared to both non-refugee controls. Trauma exposure, poverty and kindergarten attendance factors were significant predictors for this difference. As for the Arabic language skills, Syrian children outperformed their bilingual peers in Arabic. Although Syrian children displayed a more balanced bilingual profile, their performance in their dominant language (Arabic) was poorer than the bilingual control group’s performance in their dominant language (Turkish). Overall, the results suggest that refugee children’s working memory, shifting and fluid intelligence abilities, as well as mother tongue development, were negatively affected by forced displacement, but they were able to develop Turkish vocabulary skills and match Turkish monolinguals on both working memory and shifting abilities. This is the first piece of evidence suggesting that while being a refugee has adverse effects on children’s cognitive and linguistic development, holding bilingual status may actually create a protective shield in some cognitive abilities for disadvantaged refugee children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101412"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139061730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers’ ability to build inferences about a film protagonist’s emotional state 学龄前儿童推断电影主人公情绪状态的能力
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414
Marie Sophie Hunze , Franziska Freudenberger , Yvonne Gerigk , Gerhild Nieding , Peter Ohler , Anna-Katharina Diergarten
{"title":"Preschoolers’ ability to build inferences about a film protagonist’s emotional state","authors":"Marie Sophie Hunze ,&nbsp;Franziska Freudenberger ,&nbsp;Yvonne Gerigk ,&nbsp;Gerhild Nieding ,&nbsp;Peter Ohler ,&nbsp;Anna-Katharina Diergarten","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In this study, we investigated whether preschool children can draw inferences about the emotional state of a film protagonist. A sample of 59 children aged 4 and 5 watched 20 scenes from a television show. Their ability to understand the protagonist’s emotional states was assessed using a reaction time task. Media sign literacy, theory of mind abilities, and the general understanding of the scenes were also evaluated. Our results show that even children aged 4 drew emotional inferences but only on a valence-based level (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of different valences). No exact inferences (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of the same valence) were drawn. There were no significant influences of media sign literacy or theory of mind on the inference generation, but general understanding of the film scenes was important.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101414"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139092957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-derivation through memory integration: a longitudinal examination of performance and relations with academic achievements in elementary classrooms 通过记忆整合进行自我激励:对小学课堂表现及其与学业成绩关系的纵向研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学
Cognitive Development Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416
Alena G. Esposito , Patricia J. Bauer
{"title":"Self-derivation through memory integration: a longitudinal examination of performance and relations with academic achievements in elementary classrooms","authors":"Alena G. Esposito ,&nbsp;Patricia J. Bauer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Self-derivation through memory integration is the cognitive process of generating new knowledge by integrating individual facts. Across two studies, we longitudinally examined developmental change, individual stability, and relations with </span>academic performance in a diverse agricultural community. We documented children’s self-derivation in their classrooms and examined the relation with self-derivation and academic performance a year later. In Study 1, we examined self-derivation (</span><em>n</em> = 94; <em>M</em><sub>age</sub>= 6.67; initially grades K and 1) using the same paradigm at both time points. We found evidence of developmental change from Time 1 to Time 2. However, self-derivation accounted for a small portion of the variance in self-derivation (reflecting individual stability) and academic performance measured one year later. In Study 2, we examined self-derivation across two different paradigms with children beginning in Grades 2 and 3 (<em>n</em> = 82; <em>M</em><sub>age</sub>= 8.60). Even across paradigms, we found evidence for individual stability. Year 1 self-derivation also predicted Year 2 academic performance. We posit that self-derivation through integration is a domain-general construct related to academic performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101416"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139480052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信