Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Blurred Lines: Memory, Perceptions, and Consciousness: Commentary on "Consciousness as a Memory System" by Budson et al (2022). 模糊的线条:记忆、感知和意识:布德森等人对“意识作为记忆系统”的评论(2022)。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000325
Hinze Hogendoorn
{"title":"Blurred Lines: Memory, Perceptions, and Consciousness: Commentary on \"Consciousness as a Memory System\" by Budson et al (2022).","authors":"Hinze Hogendoorn","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000325","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the previous issue, Budson, Richman, and Kensinger (2022) put forth the intriguing proposal that consciousness may have evolved from the episodic memory system. In addition to providing a possible evolutionary trajectory for consciousness, I believe that viewing consciousness as an extension of memory in this way is particularly useful for understanding some of the puzzling temporal complexities that are inherent to consciousness. For example, due to neural transmission delays, our conscious experience must necessarily lag the outside world, which creates a paradox for both conscious perception (Do we see the past, rather than the present?) and action (How can we make rapid decisions if it takes so long to become conscious of something?). These paradoxes can be elegantly solved by treating consciousness as a memory system. Finally, the proposal put forth by Budson and colleagues (2022) aligns with the emerging perspective that consciousness, like memory, represents a narrative time line of events rather than any single instant. However, I believe that this conceptualization can be further extended to include not only the past, but also the future. In this way, consciousness can be provocatively viewed as the remembered past, present, and future.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"36 1","pages":"54-58"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9304192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consciousness as a Memory System. 意识作为一个记忆系统。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000319
Andrew E Budson, Kenneth A Richman, Elizabeth A Kensinger
{"title":"Consciousness as a Memory System.","authors":"Andrew E Budson,&nbsp;Kenneth A Richman,&nbsp;Elizabeth A Kensinger","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000319","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000319","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We suggest that there is confusion between why consciousness developed and what additional functions, through continued evolution, it has co-opted. Consider episodic memory. If we believe that episodic memory evolved solely to accurately represent past events, it seems like a terrible system-prone to forgetting and false memories. However, if we believe that episodic memory developed to flexibly and creatively combine and rearrange memories of prior events in order to plan for the future, then it is quite a good system. We argue that consciousness originally developed as part of the episodic memory system-quite likely the part needed to accomplish that flexible recombining of information. We posit further that consciousness was subsequently co-opted to produce other functions that are not directly relevant to memory per se, such as problem-solving, abstract thinking, and language. We suggest that this theory is compatible with many phenomena, such as the slow speed and the after-the-fact order of consciousness, that cannot be explained well by other theories. We believe that our theory may have profound implications for understanding intentional action and consciousness in general. Moreover, we suggest that episodic memory and its associated memory systems of sensory, working, and semantic memory as a whole ought to be considered together as the conscious memory system in that they, together, give rise to the phenomenon of consciousness. Lastly, we suggest that the cerebral cortex is the part of the brain that makes consciousness possible, and that every cortical region contributes to this conscious memory system.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"263-297"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9708083/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9249414","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Somatoform Symptoms in Parkinson Disease. 帕金森病的躯体形式症状。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000322
David Glovinsky, Ann L Gruber-Baldini, Seth Himelhoch, Karen E Anderson, Lisa M Shulman
{"title":"Somatoform Symptoms in Parkinson Disease.","authors":"David Glovinsky,&nbsp;Ann L Gruber-Baldini,&nbsp;Seth Himelhoch,&nbsp;Karen E Anderson,&nbsp;Lisa M Shulman","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000322","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Co-occurring somatoform symptoms complicate the diagnosis and treatment of Parkinson disease (PD).</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To learn more about the relationship between somatoform symptoms and PD by comparing demographic and clinical features across PD groups differing in somatoform symptom severity.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using standardized Brief Symptom Inventory-18 (BSI-18) scores to measure somatoform symptom severity, we assigned 1093 individuals with PD to one of four subgroups using comparisons to normative means: low (M < -½ SD), average (M ± ½ SD), high (M +½ SD to +1 SD), very high (M > +1 SD). We used demographics and disease severity measures to assess each subgroup.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Most of the individuals with PD (56%) had high or very high somatoform symptom levels. Increased somatoform symptom levels were associated with female gender, lower socioeconomic status, greater disease duration, increased PD severity (Total Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale), greater disability (Older Americans Resource and Services Disability subscale), increased BSI-18 Depression and Anxiety subscale scores, lower cognitive function (Mini-Mental State Examination), lower self-efficacy scores (Self-Efficacy to Manage Chronic Disease Scale), lower quality of life scores (SF-12 Health Status Survey), and greater medical comorbidity (Cumulative Illness Rating Scale-Geriatrics) (all comparisons: P < 0.001). We found no significant between-group differences for age, race, or marital status.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Somatoform symptom severity in individuals with PD is associated with greater PD severity and disability and is more common in females and in individuals with low socioeconomic status. Greater awareness of somatoform symptoms should help improve PD treatment.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"255-262"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10686080","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Account of the Effect of Switch Probability on Switch and Mixing Costs: An ERP Study in a Cued Task-switching Paradigm. 转换概率对转换和混合成本的影响:线索任务转换范式下的ERP研究。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317
Cheng Wenwen, Yan Yang, Liyan Cui, Ying Chen, Weiguo Zhang, Xiong Zhang, Shu Zhou
{"title":"The Account of the Effect of Switch Probability on Switch and Mixing Costs: An ERP Study in a Cued Task-switching Paradigm.","authors":"Cheng Wenwen,&nbsp;Yan Yang,&nbsp;Liyan Cui,&nbsp;Ying Chen,&nbsp;Weiguo Zhang,&nbsp;Xiong Zhang,&nbsp;Shu Zhou","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000317","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Whether the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by an activation or preparation account is unclear.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate the account of the effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used a cued task-switching paradigm with three switch probabilities (high, 75%; medium, 50%; and low, 25%) with 19 healthy young adults and recorded the cue- and target-locked event-related potentials (ERPs) and behavioral performance. The task included switch and stay trials under high, medium, and low switch conditions, as well as pure trials.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was no significant difference in reaction time (RT) in switch and mixing costs between the high and medium switch conditions. The RT in switch and mixing costs in the high and medium switch conditions was significantly less and more than in the low switch condition, respectively. The cue-locked ERPs revealed significant effects on mixing costs (stay - pure) that were cue early frontal positivity (260-300 ms) in the high and medium switch conditions, and on switch costs (switch - stay) that were cue early central positivity (240-260 ms) in the low switch condition. Moreover, the target-locked ERPs of the mixing costs revealed significant effects on mixing costs that were target P3b (440-540 ms) in all three switch conditions, and on switch costs that were target P3b in the medium and low switch conditions.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The effect of switch probability on switch and mixing costs is explained by the activation account.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"230-246"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10633172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Constructional Impairments and Their Neural Correlates in Nondemented Adults With Cerebral Autosomal-dominant Arteriopathy With Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy. 伴有皮质下梗死和白质脑病的大脑常染色体显性动脉病变的非痴呆成人的结构损伤及其神经相关性。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318
Natascia De Lucia, Michele Ragno, Cristina Paci, Gabriella Cacchiò, Anna Rita Caiazzo, Sara Tiberi, Anna De Rosa, Riccardo Navarra, Massimo Caulo, Giuseppe De Michele, Luigi Trojano
{"title":"Constructional Impairments and Their Neural Correlates in Nondemented Adults With Cerebral Autosomal-dominant Arteriopathy With Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy.","authors":"Natascia De Lucia,&nbsp;Michele Ragno,&nbsp;Cristina Paci,&nbsp;Gabriella Cacchiò,&nbsp;Anna Rita Caiazzo,&nbsp;Sara Tiberi,&nbsp;Anna De Rosa,&nbsp;Riccardo Navarra,&nbsp;Massimo Caulo,&nbsp;Giuseppe De Michele,&nbsp;Luigi Trojano","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000318","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) is a monogenic small-vessel disease that is characterized by a wide range of neurologic and neuropsychological impairments. Constructional impairments have been reported in some cases but have never been assessed systematically.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To evaluate constructional abilities and their cognitive and neural correlates in nondemented individuals with CADASIL.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty individuals with CADASIL who were not affected by clinically relevant cognitive deterioration and 30 healthy controls (HC) underwent an extensive cognitive assessment and paper-and-pencil visuoconstructional tasks in order to detect constructional impairments. Performance on the visuoconstructional tasks was correlated with the cognitive assessment scores and with quantitative indices of regional gray matter atrophy (obtained via FreeSurfer image analysis) and white matter involvement.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The individuals with CADASIL achieved significantly lower scores on the cognitive assessment compared with the HC. Poor visuoconstructional abilities were observed in seven (23.3%) of the individuals with CADASIL when performing the copy drawing task and in nine (30%) when performing the Rey Complex Figure Test. Logistic regression revealed that visuoconstructional impairments were significantly associated with scores on the Frontal Assessment Battery and the Attentional Matrices Test. Morphometric results revealed that scores on the visuoconstructional tasks were related to gray matter atrophy of the left frontal lobe and right parietal lobe.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Impairments on visuoconstructional tasks are quite common in individuals with CADASIL, even in the lack of clinically relevant cognitive deterioration, and are critically related to frontal and parietal atrophy.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"247-254"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9199164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Memory Loss, Alzheimer's Disease, and Dementia: A Practical Guide for Clinicians, 3rd ed. 失忆、阿尔茨海默病和痴呆症:临床医生实用指南,第3版。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323
Howard S Kirshner
{"title":"Memory Loss, Alzheimer's Disease, and Dementia: A Practical Guide for Clinicians, 3rd ed.","authors":"Howard S Kirshner","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000323","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 4","pages":"298-299"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Graph Theory Analysis of Semantic Fluency in Russian-English Bilinguals. 俄英双语者语义流利度的图论分析。
IF 1.3 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312
Vidushi Sinha, Frances Lissemore, Alan J Lerner
{"title":"Graph Theory Analysis of Semantic Fluency in Russian-English Bilinguals.","authors":"Vidushi Sinha, Frances Lissemore, Alan J Lerner","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Semantic category fluency is a widely used task involving language, memory, and executive function. Previous studies of bilingual semantic fluency have shown only small differences between languages. Graph theory analyzes complex relationships in networks, including node and edge number, clustering coefficient, average path length, average number of direct neighbors, and scale-free and small-world properties.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To shed light on whether the underlying neural processes involved in semantic category fluency testing yield substantially different networks in different languages.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We compared languages and methods using both network analysis and conventional analysis of word production. We administered the animal naming task to 51 Russian-English bilinguals in each language. We constructed network graphs using three methods: (a) simple association of unique co-occurring neighbors, (b) corrected associations between consecutive words occurring beyond chance, and (c) a network community approach using planar maximally filtered graphs. We compared the resultant network analytics as well as their scale-free and small-world properties.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants produced more words in Russian than in English. Small-worldness metrics were variable between Russian and English but were consistent across the three graph theory analytical methods.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The networks had similar graph theory properties in both languages. The optimal methodology for creating networks from semantic category fluency remains to be determined.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"179-187"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10154949/pdf/nihms-1814366.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10130340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinguishing Between Genuine and Feigned Dementia Using Event-related Potentials. 利用事件相关电位区分真实痴呆和假装痴呆。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311
August M Price, Rocco Palumbo, Anna Marin, Prayerna Uppal, Cheongmin Suh, Andrew E Budson, Katherine W Turk
{"title":"Distinguishing Between Genuine and Feigned Dementia Using Event-related Potentials.","authors":"August M Price,&nbsp;Rocco Palumbo,&nbsp;Anna Marin,&nbsp;Prayerna Uppal,&nbsp;Cheongmin Suh,&nbsp;Andrew E Budson,&nbsp;Katherine W Turk","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311","DOIUrl":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Individuals with probable Alzheimer disease (AD) may perform below cutoffs on traditional, memory-based performance validity tests. Previous studies have found success using event-related potentials (ERPs) to detect feigned neurocognitive impairment in younger populations.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To evaluate the utility of an auditory oddball task in conjunction with the P3b peak amplitude to distinguish probable AD from simulated dementia.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Twenty individuals with probable AD and 20 older healthy controls (HC) underwent an ERP auditory oddball protocol and the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). The HC were asked to perform honestly for one condition and to simulate dementia for the other. The individuals with probable AD were asked to perform honestly. The P3b peak amplitude and button press accuracy were collected from each participant and were analyzed to determine their effectiveness in detecting performance validity.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The P3b peak amplitude remained stable regardless of behavioral condition in the HC group. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude further improved the ability to correctly differentiate individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia with 100% sensitivity and 90% specificity.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The P3b peak amplitude was found to be an effective physiologic measure of cognitive impairment in individuals with probable AD compared with HC simulating dementia. When combined with the TOMM Trial 2 score, the P3b peak amplitude served as a promising performance validity measure for differentiating individuals with probable AD from HC simulating dementia.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"188-197"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9444996/pdf/nihms-1813003.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10505581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
BNT-15: Revised Performance Validity Cutoffs and Proposed Clinical Classification Ranges. BNT-15:修订的绩效有效性截止值和建议的临床分类范围。
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304
Kaitlyn Abeare, Laura Cutler, Kelly Y An, Parveen Razvi, Matthew Holcomb, Laszlo A Erdodi
{"title":"BNT-15: Revised Performance Validity Cutoffs and Proposed Clinical Classification Ranges.","authors":"Kaitlyn Abeare,&nbsp;Laura Cutler,&nbsp;Kelly Y An,&nbsp;Parveen Razvi,&nbsp;Matthew Holcomb,&nbsp;Laszlo A Erdodi","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000304","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Abbreviated neurocognitive tests offer a practical alternative to full-length versions but often lack clear interpretive guidelines, thereby limiting their clinical utility.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>To replicate validity cutoffs for the Boston Naming Test-Short Form (BNT-15) and to introduce a clinical classification system for the BNT-15 as a measure of object-naming skills.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected data from 43 university students and 46 clinical patients. Classification accuracy was computed against psychometrically defined criterion groups. Clinical classification ranges were developed using a z -score transformation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Previously suggested validity cutoffs (≤11 and ≤12) produced comparable classification accuracy among the university students. However, a more conservative cutoff (≤10) was needed with the clinical patients to contain the false-positive rate (0.20-0.38 sensitivity at 0.92-0.96 specificity). As a measure of cognitive ability, a perfect BNT-15 score suggests above average performance; ≤11 suggests clinically significant deficits. Demographically adjusted prorated BNT-15 T-scores correlated strongly (0.86) with the newly developed z -scores.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Given its brevity (<5 minutes), ease of administration and scoring, the BNT-15 can function as a useful and cost-effective screening measure for both object-naming/English proficiency and performance validity. The proposed clinical classification ranges provide useful guidelines for practitioners.</p>","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 3","pages":"155-168"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41118587","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Volumetric Assessment of Hippocampus and Subcortical Gray Matter Regions in Alzheimer Disease and Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment 阿尔茨海默病和轻度认知障碍患者海马和皮质下灰质区域的容量评估
IF 1.4 4区 医学
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296
Tuğberk Andaç Topkan, Nesrin Erdoğan, Berrak Barutcu, E. Cindil, E. T. Tali, Y. Karaman
{"title":"Volumetric Assessment of Hippocampus and Subcortical Gray Matter Regions in Alzheimer Disease and Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment","authors":"Tuğberk Andaç Topkan, Nesrin Erdoğan, Berrak Barutcu, E. Cindil, E. T. Tali, Y. Karaman","doi":"10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000296","url":null,"abstract":"Background: Quantitative MRI assessment methods have limited utility due to a lack of standardized methods and measures for Alzheimer disease (AD) and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Objective: To employ a relatively new and easy-to-use quantitative assessment method to reveal volumetric changes in subcortical gray matter (GM) regions, hippocampus, and global intracranial structures as well as the diagnostic performance and best thresholds of total hippocampal volumetry in individuals with AD and those with aMCI. Method: A total of 74 individuals—37 with mild to moderate AD, 19 with aMCI, and 18 with normal cognition (NC)—underwent a 3T MRI. Fully automated segmentation and volumetric measurements were performed. Results: The AD and aMCI groups had smaller volumes of amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and hippocampus compared with the NC group. These same two groups had significantly smaller total white matter volume than the NC group. The AD group had smaller total GM volume compared with the aMCI and NC groups. The thalamus in the AD group showed a subtle atrophy. There were no significant volumetric differences in the caudate nucleus, putamen, or globus pallidus between the groups. Conclusion: The amygdala and nucleus accumbens showed atrophy comparable to the hippocampal atrophy in both the AD and aMCI groups, which may contribute to cognitive impairment. Hippocampal volumetry is a reliable tool for differentiating between AD and NC groups but has substantially less power in differentiating between AD and aMCI groups. The loss of total GM volume differentiates AD from aMCI and NC.","PeriodicalId":50671,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology","volume":"35 1","pages":"95 - 103"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41437948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信