Vandhana Choenni , Carlinde W. Broeks , Anne Tharner , Maartje P.C.M. Luijk , Frank C. Verhulst , Mijke P. Lambregtse-van den Berg , Rianne Kok
{"title":"Attachment security and disorganization in infants of mothers with severe psychiatric disorder: Exploring the role of comorbid personality disorder","authors":"Vandhana Choenni , Carlinde W. Broeks , Anne Tharner , Maartje P.C.M. Luijk , Frank C. Verhulst , Mijke P. Lambregtse-van den Berg , Rianne Kok","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101974","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101974","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The aim of this preliminary study was to explore infant-mother attachment quality in a Dutch clinical sample of mothers with severe psychiatric disorder, with or without comorbid personality disorder. Thirty-two mothers were recruited through specialized secondary and tertiary outpatient clinics and mental health institutions. Maternal psychiatric and personality diagnoses were verified with structured clinical interviews during pregnancy. Maternal concurrent level of psychiatric symptoms was assessed by self-report and infant-mother attachment quality by observation in the Strange Situation Procedure at 14 months postpartum. In the full sample, almost half of the infants were classified as disorganized. All infants of mothers with a comorbid personality disorder were classified as either insecure or disorganized. Infants of mothers with a comorbid personality disorder had a significantly higher disorganization score than infants of mothers with a psychiatric disorder only. Continuous attachment security scores did not differ significantly between groups. In the full sample, continuous infant attachment security and disorganization score were not significantly correlated with the level of maternal concurrent psychiatric symptoms. Our exploratory findings suggest a specific link between maternal psychiatric and comorbid personality disorder and attachment disorganization. Moreover, chronicity of symptoms appears more relevant for attachment behaviors than the severity of concurrent psychiatric symptoms. Maternal personality disorder may have a strong formative impact on infant attachment security and disorganization, which warrants further research to inform clinical practice, in order to reduce the risk of intergenerational transmission of maternal psychopathology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101974"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000535/pdfft?md5=2fb29358aaa0c66e7d8640c4328ef281&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000535-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141422941","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Maylli Daiani Graciosa , Priscilla Augusta Monteiro Ferronato , Rene Drezner , Edison de Jesus Manoel
{"title":"Emergence of locomotor behaviors: Associations with infant characteristics, developmental status, parental beliefs, and practices in typically developing Brazilian infants aged 5 to 15 months","authors":"Maylli Daiani Graciosa , Priscilla Augusta Monteiro Ferronato , Rene Drezner , Edison de Jesus Manoel","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101965","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101965","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Independent locomotion provides autonomy for infants, drastically changing their relationship with their surroundings. From a dynamic systems perspective, the interaction between environment, tasks, and organismic constraints leads to the emergence of new behaviors over time. This 6-month longitudinal study aimed to verify associations between the emergence of locomotor behaviors and infants' characteristics, developmental status, parental beliefs, and practices. This observational study remotely assessed 37 full-term Brazilian infants aged 5 to 15 months, divided into two groups (G1: 5 to 11 months, n = 19; and G2: 9 to 15 months, n = 18). The motor developmental status of infants was closely associated with the emergence of behaviors (p < 0.05). Infants in G2 whose parents agreed with the statement \"In typically developing infants, motor development occurs naturally and there is no need to actively stimulate it\" started to walk later than those whose parents disagreed. Infants whose parents expected them to walk around 10–11 months walked earlier compared to those expected to walk after 11 months (G2, p = 0.011). Infants in G2 with a high frequency of staying in the supine position started to walk, both with and without support, later than those with a low frequency (p < 0.05). For infants in G1 with a high frequency of playing on the floor, locomotion (p = 0.041) and crawling on hands-and-knees (p = 0.007) started sooner compared to those with a low frequency. Staying in the cradle more frequently was related to a later emergence of supported walk (p = 0.046) among infants in G2. The emergence of locomotor behaviors is associated with motor developmental status, the surfaces where the infant plays, and body position. Parental beliefs and expectations influence how infants are stimulated and, consequently, the emergence of independent walking.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101965"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141313431","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Context of digital media use in early childhood: Factors associated with cognitive development up to 36 months of age","authors":"Bruna Gabriella Pedrotti, Denise Ruschel Bandeira, Giana Bitencourt Frizzo","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101963","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101963","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The influence of digital media on child cognitive development is a complex factor that goes beyond screen time. This study investigates the broader impact of digital media on child cognitive development, considering contextual variables such content type, parental mediation, and maternal mental health. Brazilian mothers (N = 212) of children 4–36 months old answered an online survey. Results showed that children’s screen time increases with age. The model with unmediated use, non-educational content, and mother’s screen time explains 28.7 % of children’s screen time variance. Non-educational content and simultaneous screen media use explained 5.3 % of the variance in toddlers’ cognitive development. Mothers with common mental disorders tended not to mediate their children’s media use. We discuss the influence of caregivers' availability to mediate infants' and toddlers' digital media use and its impact on children's cognitive development. These findings underscore the necessity for educational and awareness campaigns aimed at fostering access to high-quality digital content for children during critical stages of development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101963"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141291861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Infant sensorimotor decoupling from 4 to 9 months of age: Individual differences and contingencies with maternal actions","authors":"Zhuojun Ying, Betina Karshaleva, Gedeon Deák","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101957","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101957","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Triadic interactions, wherein infants coordinate attention between caregivers and objects of shared focus, are believed to facilitate infant learning, and emerge around 9–12 months of age (Carpenter et al., 1998). Sensorimotor decoupling, wherein infants look at one percept while manipulating another, or use each hand for different actions, was hypothesized (de Barbaro et al., 2016) to contribute to triadic skills by allowing infants to smoothly shift attention between objects and social partners. We explored the development of Hand-Hand (H-H) and Gaze-Hand (G-H) decoupling in 38 infants at 4, 6, and 9 months. We also tested contingencies between maternal behaviors and infant decoupling: i.e., whether decoupling events followed maternal object-directed actions. Both overall and contingent infant decoupling increased from 4 to 9 months. Decoupling rates (both G-H and H-H) predicted variance in infants' fine and gross motor scores. Contingent G-H decoupling at 6 months predicted BSID-III communication scores at 18 months. Thus the development of infant sensorimotor skills, including decoupling, allows infants to smoothly shift attention and participate in triadic interactions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101957"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000365/pdfft?md5=9768a90a7403758cd2a3e8b0475cb639&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000365-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141186729","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Hush, little baby: The role of C-tactile afferents in babywearing infants with neonatal opioid withdrawal","authors":"Lela Rankin , Lisa M. Grisham , Catherine Ingbar","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101960","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101960","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Social touch through infant holding, skin-to-skin contact, and infant carrying (babywearing) decreases infant distress and promotes secure attachment. Unknown is the extent to which these effects are the result of the activation of C-Tactile afferents (CTs), the constellation of nerve fibers associated with affective touch, primarily located in the head and trunk of the body. The purpose of the present study was to compare dynamic touch (CTs activated) to static touch (CTs less activated) during a babywearing procedure among infants experiencing Neonatal Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome (NOWS). NOWS is a spectrum of clinical symptoms, including elevated heart rate (HR), associated with withdrawal from intrauterine opioid exposure. We hypothesized that stroking an infant’s head during babywearing would amplify the pleasurable effect of babywearing as measured by changes in infant HR. Twenty-nine infants in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) in the Southwestern USA were worn in an infant carrier starting at five days old (<em>M =</em> 5.4, SD = 2.6; 46.2 % White, 26.9 % Latinx, 11.5 % Native American) and physiological readings were conducted daily; heart rates of infants and caregivers were taken every 15-seconds for 5-minutes, <em>before</em>, <em>during</em>, and <em>after</em> babywearing (30 min per phase). Each day infants alternated (randomly) in a static touch (hands-free babywearing) or dynamic touch condition (stroking the top of the infants’ head at a velocity of 3 cm/s while babywearing). On average, infants completed 3 dynamic and 3 static babywearing sessions. Hospital and research staff participated in babywearing when a parent was not available (31.0 % of infants were exclusively worn by volunteers, 27.6 % were exclusively worn by parents). We analyzed the data using Hierarchical Linear Models due to the 3-level nested design (<em>N</em> = 29 infants, <em>N</em> = 191 readings, <em>N</em> = 11,974 heart rates). Compared to baseline (infant calm/asleep and without contact), infant’s HRs significantly declined during and after babywearing, controlling for pharmacological treatment. These effects were significantly stronger during the dynamic touch condition (reduction in HR of 11.17 bpm) compared to the static touch condition (reduction in HR of 3.74 bpm). These effects did not significantly vary by wearer (mother, father, volunteer). However, differences between the dynamic and static conditions were significantly stronger in earlier babywearing sessions, potentially indicating a learning effect. There was evidence for a calming effect among caregivers as well, particularly in the dynamic touch condition, when caregivers were engaged in active touch. Activation of CTs appears to be an important mechanism in the physiological benefits of babywearing and in the symbiotic role of caregiver-infant attachment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101960"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141184527","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Zoe C.G. Cloud , Alicia Spittle , Jeanie Cheong , Lex W. Doyle , Peter J. Anderson , Karli Treyvaud
{"title":"Predicting externalizing behaviors in typically developing toddlers at 24 months: Insights from parenting at 12 months","authors":"Zoe C.G. Cloud , Alicia Spittle , Jeanie Cheong , Lex W. Doyle , Peter J. Anderson , Karli Treyvaud","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101964","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101964","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Early observational assessment of parent and child behaviors may identify risk factors associated with the development of early child externalizing behaviors. This study aimed to examine factors associated with child externalizing behaviors at 24-months of age, including early maternal depression, family social risk and the parent-child relationship. Using a longitudinal design in 89 mother-child dyads (<em>n</em> = 43 female), maternal depressive symptoms and social risk were measured post-birth, and 12-months later parent-child interaction was assessed using the Emotional Availability Scales. To assess child externalizing behaviors, a parent-report questionnaire was administered when children were 24-months old. Increased early maternal depressive symptoms (<em>p</em> = .03), but not higher social risk (<em>p</em> = 0.17), were associated with higher child externalizing behaviors in children at age 24-months. After adjusting for early maternal depressive symptoms and familial social risk, lower levels of observed maternal structuring (β = −2.60, 95 %CI = −4.56, −0.64, <em>p</em> = .01) and lower levels of non-hostility (β = −3.39, 95 %CI −6.64, −0.14, <em>p</em> = .04) when the child was 12-months old were associated with higher parent-report of externalizing behaviors. However, the child’s observed interaction behavior was not associated with the mother’s report of child externalizing behavior. Interventions targeting specific post-natal maternal mental health and early parenting behaviors may reduce the risk for the development of later child externalizing problems.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101964"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000432/pdfft?md5=06bf6ab51ebffb1eb12b75b45ae46602&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000432-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141184561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Alexander LaTourrette , Cynthia Blanco , Naz Deniz Atik , Sandra R. Waxman
{"title":"Navigating accent variability: 24-month-olds recognize known words spoken in an unfamiliar accent but require additional support to learn new words","authors":"Alexander LaTourrette , Cynthia Blanco , Naz Deniz Atik , Sandra R. Waxman","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101962","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101962","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As infants learn their native languages, they must also learn to contend with variability across speakers of those languages. Here, we examine 24-month-olds’ ability to process speech in an unfamiliar accent. We demonstrate that 24-month-olds successfully identify the referents of known words in unfamiliar-accented speech but cannot use known words alone to infer new word meanings. However, when the novel word occurs in a supportive referential context, with the target referent visually available, 24-month-olds successfully learn new word-referent mappings. Thus, 24-month-olds recognize and learn words in unfamiliar accents, but unfamiliar-accented speech may pose challenges for more sophisticated language processing strategies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101962"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141184559","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Victoria L. Mousley , Mairéad MacSweeney , Evelyne Mercure
{"title":"Revisiting perceptual sensitivity to non-native speech in a diverse sample of bilinguals","authors":"Victoria L. Mousley , Mairéad MacSweeney , Evelyne Mercure","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101959","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101959","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Werker and Tees (1984) prompted decades of research attempting to detail the paths infants take towards specialisation for the sounds of their native language(s). Most of this research has examined the trajectories of monolingual children. However, it has also been proposed that bilinguals, who are exposed to greater phonetic variability than monolinguals and must learn the rules of two languages, may remain perceptually open to non-native language sounds later into life than monolinguals. Using a visual habituation paradigm, the current study tests this question by comparing 15- to 18-month-old monolingual and bilingual children’s developmental trajectories for non-native phonetic consonant contrast discrimination. A novel approach to the integration of stimulus presentation software with eye-tracking software was validated for objective measurement of infant looking time. The results did not support the hypothesis of a protracted period of sensitivity to non-native phonetic contrasts in bilingual compared to monolingual infants. Implications for diversification of perceptual narrowing research and implementation of increasingly sensitive measures are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101959"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000389/pdfft?md5=c00ad3bef85ef1512c611b6fe529c288&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000389-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141083145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Ana M. Carmiol , Susan Castro , María Dolores Castro-Rojas , Adriana Weisleder , Juan Robalino
{"title":"Links between booksharing and early vocabulary development in Costa Rica","authors":"Ana M. Carmiol , Susan Castro , María Dolores Castro-Rojas , Adriana Weisleder , Juan Robalino","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101958","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101958","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Early vocabulary development is crucial for future cognitive and academic outcomes, and parent-child booksharing has been recognized as a powerful home literacy practice to promote word learning. However, evidence about the link between booksharing and language development in diverse cultural and socioeconomic settings is currently limited, hindering the formulation of a broadly applicable framework to understand the favorable conditions for early vocabulary development. This study explores the relationship between booksharing and early receptive and expressive vocabulary in a sample of 183 mothers and their toddlers in Costa Rica, a context where reading is not a common practice and children have limited access to books. Mothers completed an interview about their booksharing practices and reported children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary. Results demonstrated a positive link between maternal booksharing and children’s expressive vocabulary. Child gender moderated the link between booksharing and receptive vocabulary, exhibiting a stronger association in girls than in boys. Mothers with lower education levels reported higher expressive vocabulary scores for their children than mothers with higher education levels. These findings underscore the significance of booksharing in the home literacy environment, even in cultural contexts with distinct reading practices. Moreover, they highlight the need to incorporate sociocultural factors into comprehensive accounts concerning the role of booksharing in early word learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 101958"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141072908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jorge Alejandro Santos , Federico Giovannetti , Mariana Celeste Smulski , Maria Julia Hermida , Daniel Roberto Petetta , María Soledad Segretin , Sebastián Lipina
{"title":"A systematic review of the concept of self-regulation in infants between 0 and 36 months in Latin America","authors":"Jorge Alejandro Santos , Federico Giovannetti , Mariana Celeste Smulski , Maria Julia Hermida , Daniel Roberto Petetta , María Soledad Segretin , Sebastián Lipina","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101954","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101954","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper aims to identify how cognitive and emotional self-regulation (SR) processes in infants from 0 to 36 months are defined within the Latin American academic context. A systematic review based on the PRISMA methodology was implemented to review the conceptual and operational definition of SR, the type of study, the country of origin of the authors, and the reference to the adequacy of the research to the specific cultural context of Latin America. Twenty-two papers that met the selection criteria were selected. The study identified four types of conceptual definitions for SR, each associated with different constructs or sets of constructs: executive functions, temperament, the integration of executive functions and temperament, and physiological homeostasis. These definitions were based on mainstream approaches to SR rather than being specific to the Latin American region. The study also found compatibility between the sample and some observed trends. On one hand, there was an underrepresentation of the Latin American population in high-impact publications on the subject. However, from 2010 to the present, there is evidence of growth in publications on SR in the analyzed sample. On the other hand, the sample also indicates a disparate representation of the countries of Latin America and the Caribbean in existing publications. Finally, concerning the adaptation to the cultural context of the research, a small number of studies addressed this variable in a specific and significant way. However, even in these cases, the approach is based on models and hypotheses that are limited to understanding the Latin American region's cultural, socioeconomic, and demographic diversity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"75 ","pages":"Article 101954"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141066588","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}