I-Perception最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
The influence of eye position on the animacy impression of a cube-shaped robot in motion.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-03-19 eCollection Date: 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251323769
Takahiro Kawabe, Rintaro Akiyama, Takumi Yokosaka
{"title":"The influence of eye position on the animacy impression of a cube-shaped robot in motion.","authors":"Takahiro Kawabe, Rintaro Akiyama, Takumi Yokosaka","doi":"10.1177/20416695251323769","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251323769","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human observers can sometimes attribute animacy or agency to non-living objects, such as robots, perceiving them as if they were alive. In particular, the movement pattern of non-living things is a key feature for perceiving life. It is also well known that the pattern of the eyes is also an important feature for the perception of the sense of life. The present study investigated how the animacy impression of a cube-shaped robot moving along the Perlin noise trajectory could be influenced by the visual patterns of the eyes, such as eye positions and gaze directions. The eyes were presented on the top surface of the cube-shaped robot. Participants were asked to rate animacy impressions of the robot. These impressions included the impression of a live animal, having intention and moving in a self-propelled manner. These impressions were consistently higher when the eyes were presented on the side of the robot's direction of motion than when they were presented on the side orthogonal to, or opposite to, the robot's direction of motion. In general, the animacy impressions were largely comparable regardless of whether the robot's gaze direction aligned with, was orthogonal to, or opposed its motion direction. However, the impression of intention was stronger when the gaze direction at the front side of the object was consistent with the motion direction than when it was inconsistent. We discuss the evolutionary role of eye position in determining animacy impressions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 2","pages":"20416695251323769"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11931162/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143701819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spatial alignment and the motion bridging effect: Reversals in the direction of an illusory rotation.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-03-17 eCollection Date: 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251318945
Maximilian Stein, Robert Fendrich, Uwe Mattler
{"title":"Spatial alignment and the motion bridging effect: Reversals in the direction of an illusory rotation.","authors":"Maximilian Stein, Robert Fendrich, Uwe Mattler","doi":"10.1177/20416695251318945","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251318945","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When a stationary ring of points precedes or follows an \"inducing ring\" of points that spins so rapidly it appears to be a steady outline circle the stationary ring often appears to momentarily rotate in the direction of the inducing ring's spin. In previous studies of this \"motion bridging effect\" (MBE) the start and stop positions of the inducing ring points were spatially aligned with the points of the stationary ring. Here we report that as these start and stop positions are progressively displaced across the spaces separating the points of the stationary test ring the MBE direction congruency effect decreases and then reverses, so that the illusory rotation is predominantly opposite to the direction of the inducing ring spin. This reverse congruency effect peaks when the points of the inducing ring start and stop midway between the points of the stationary test ring, with congruency returning as further displacements bring the point positions back into alignment. We conclude that the MBE is not only determined by the inducing ring's rotation direction, but also by an interaction between the inducing and test ring points at the moment the inducing ring starts or stops. We consider various ways of accounting this effect. Explanations based on direction cuing by apparent motion steps, the motion aftereffect, and biphasic impulse responses are ruled out. A speculative explanation based on perceptual heuristics that interpret the competing motion direction signals generated by a transformation of contour segments of the spinning ring into the points of the stationary ring (or vice-versa) is proposed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 2","pages":"20416695251318945"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11915343/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143659159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Blodgett's (1919) "Ship camouflage" 105 years on: A misperception of dazzle perception revealed and redressed.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-03-14 eCollection Date: 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241312316
Timothy Simon Meese, Samantha Louise Strong
{"title":"Blodgett's (1919) \"Ship camouflage\" 105 years on: A misperception of dazzle perception revealed and redressed.","authors":"Timothy Simon Meese, Samantha Louise Strong","doi":"10.1177/20416695241312316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695241312316","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>During WWI, dazzle camouflage involved painting allied shipping with bold geometric patterns to disrupt the perceptions of enemy submariners. The first experiment to provide quantitative results on this (Blodgett, 1919; <i>MIT Libraries</i>, MA) used scale models and mechanical simulation, and reported enormous perceptual errors for their perceived direction of travel (up to ∼60°), possibly due to a \"twist\" effect from forced perspective. However, Blodgett's work did not meet modern standards and the organisation of his report complicates evaluation. Here, we produce (i) reformatted and (ii) heavily edited versions of the original report to improve readability, and (iii) provide a critical reappraisal of the work including (iv) a detailed reanalysis of Blodgett's data and (v) a new control experiment on edited images of the original stimuli. After addressing problems with Blodgett's analysis and control experiment, we found results indicating a twist of only about 7°, but a much larger \"hysteresis\" effect (∼19-23°) where perceived direction was drawn to the horizon regardless of dazzle. This effect combined both constructively and destructively with \"twist\", depending on the direction of the target ship. These reappraised findings resolve an apparent conflict with the second quantitative experiment on dazzle ships conducted over a century later using computer displays online (Lovell et al., 2024; <i>Royal Society</i> <i>Open Science</i>). We conclude that Blodgett's approach and data remain of interest today, but his conclusions substantially overestimated the effectiveness of dazzle camouflage in biasing the perceived directions of ships. However, other potential benefits of dazzle, including perceptual variance, await systematic investigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 2","pages":"20416695241312316"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11909666/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143651508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Aging does not affect auditory motion discrimination based on interaural level differences.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-03-02 eCollection Date: 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241311206
Shinya Harada, Ryo Teraoka, Naoki Kuroda, Wataru Teramoto
{"title":"Aging does not affect auditory motion discrimination based on interaural level differences.","authors":"Shinya Harada, Ryo Teraoka, Naoki Kuroda, Wataru Teramoto","doi":"10.1177/20416695241311206","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241311206","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well known that aging affects fundamental perceptual functions. Numerous studies have investigated age-related changes in visual motion perception and demonstrated that aging impairs motion processing. However, limited studies have explored age-related changes in auditory motion perception, and whether aging influences auditory motion perception based on interaural level differences remains unknown. This study examined age-related differences in the discrimination of auditory motion direction based on interaural level differences. We conducted two experiments to estimate the signal-to-noise ratio and motion coherence thresholds required to discriminate auditory motion and visual motion directions, respectively, in younger and older adults. Results showed that age significantly impairs visual motion discrimination; however, it does not impair auditory motion discrimination. These findings suggest that aging does not affect auditory motion perception based on interaural level differences, at least with the broadband noise used in this experiment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 2","pages":"20416695241311206"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874038/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143558354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The frog hand illusion: Distortion of hand shape in inverted presentation.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-27 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251319270
Shuichiro Taya, Achille Pasqualotto
{"title":"The frog hand illusion: Distortion of hand shape in inverted presentation.","authors":"Shuichiro Taya, Achille Pasqualotto","doi":"10.1177/20416695251319270","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695251319270","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When a photograph of the back of a hand with the fingers extended to the depth is observed upside-down, the hand appears vertically squashed, with extremely short fingers. The first aim of this study was to quantitatively measure the \"frog hand illusion (FHI)\", named after its bizarre appearance, and the second aim was to examine whether the dominant hand affects the strength of FHI. We measured the apparent shortening of the fingers using the method of constant stimuli. The results showed that the fingers of the inverted hand appeared to be shorter than those of the upright hand by about 5% on average. No effect of the dominant hand was observed. We propose the hypothesis that FHI occurs because of the attenuation of perceptual constancy, which might stem from observing the hand image from an atypical viewpoint.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695251319270"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11869244/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143544005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Monocular blur impairs heading judgements from optic flow.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-26 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251317148
William E A Sheppard, Rachel O Coats, Richard M Wilkie, Rigmor C Baraas
{"title":"Monocular blur impairs heading judgements from optic flow.","authors":"William E A Sheppard, Rachel O Coats, Richard M Wilkie, Rigmor C Baraas","doi":"10.1177/20416695251317148","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251317148","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Monocular blur sometimes impairs locomotion; however, it is not always clear when this will happen. Optic flow (the apparent motion of scene texture elements that occurs during self-motion) provides powerful signals about the direction of travel. Here, we test whether monocular blur impairs heading perception from optic flow compared to full vision under various levels of optic flow degradation. Participants (<i>N</i> = 52, mean age = 30 years) completed contrast sensitivity, visual acuity, and heading perception tasks with rich or degraded optic flow, with or without monocular blur (0.4 logMAR Bangerter filter over the non-dominant eye, full vision in the dominant eye). Heading perception was assessed using a browser-based task where the participants viewed a 3-second video consistent with self-motion over a textured ground plane (moving towards the horizon at an offset heading ranging from -20 to +20°) and identified the point on the horizon towards which they were travelling. The measures of each participant's performance were the absolute and directional angular error between the heading offset and their response. Monocular blur and degraded flow were associated with an increase in absolute heading error and a larger underestimation of heading angle, with the worst performance observed when monocular blur and degraded flow were combined. These results suggest that the impact of monocular blur on heading perception becomes apparent only when optic flow signals are weak (e.g., night-time driving). These findings support the theory that monocular blur and the richness of visual information interact to produce deficits in heading perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695251317148"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11863211/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143516967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
'See what you feel': The impact of visual scale distance in haptic-to-visual crossmodal matching.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-16 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251318591
Olga Daneyko, Francesca Frisco, Angelo Maravita, Daniele Zavagno
{"title":"'See what you feel': The impact of visual scale distance in haptic-to-visual crossmodal matching.","authors":"Olga Daneyko, Francesca Frisco, Angelo Maravita, Daniele Zavagno","doi":"10.1177/20416695251318591","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251318591","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two experiments were conducted to explore the impact of the distance of a visual scale employed in the crossmodal matching method dubbed <i>See What You Feel</i> (SWYF) used to study the Uznadze haptic aftereffect. Previous studies reported that SWYF leads to a general underestimation of out-of-sight handheld spheres, which seems to increase with visual scale distance. Experiment 1 tested the effect of visual scale distance in haptic-to-visual crossmodal matching. A 19-step visual scale, made of actual 3D spheres (diameters ranging from 2.0 to 5.6 cm), was set at one of three possible distances (30, 160, 290 cm); participants' task was to find the matching visual spheres for four out-of-sight handheld test spheres (diameters 3.0, 3.8, 4.6, 5.0 cm). Results confirmed the underestimation effect and only partially confirmed the role of scale distance. Experiment 2 investigated the role of scale distance in a visual-to-visual matching task in which the same visual scale was employed, set at one of three distances (37, 160, 290 cm). Participants' task was to find a match for the same four test stimuli. Results showed no statistical difference between matched and actual sphere sizes with distance 37 cm; underestimations were observed with the far distances, thus reflecting overestimations of scale sphere sizes. Results from both experiments allow us to conclude that the underestimation effect observed with SWYF is a general feature of haptic-to-visual crossmodal matching, and that the SWYF method is a valuable tool for measuring haptic size perception with handheld stimuli when the visual scale is set at a visually comfortable peripersonal distance.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695251318591"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11860285/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143516970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing aesthetic impressions with pictorial measures: A novel approach in empirical aesthetics.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-14 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241309780
Ivan Z Stojilović
{"title":"Assessing aesthetic impressions with pictorial measures: A novel approach in empirical aesthetics.","authors":"Ivan Z Stojilović","doi":"10.1177/20416695241309780","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241309780","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study introduces pictorial technique (PT) as an innovative method in empirical aesthetics to assess aesthetic impressions of visual artworks. Forty participants, drawn from general and artistic populations, evaluated nine paintings representing abstract, traditional figural, and modern figural styles using the PT and aesthetic rating scales. The PT enabled participants to mark impactful areas within artworks, transforming subjective impressions into spatial data visualized as heatmaps. Results showed that, on average, participants marked 17% of the painting's surface, with notable stylistic differences in attention distribution. Abstract paintings exhibited dispersed attention, focusing on geometric shapes and color contrasts, while traditional figural works concentrated on narrative elements. Modern figural paintings demonstrated a hybrid pattern, emphasizing both individual details and broader compositions. The study also tested the hypothesis that dimensional characteristics of marked areas correspond to aesthetic preferences. Findings revealed that the size of marked regions modestly predicted ratings on Interestingness and Comprehensibility scales, though the explained variance was limited. The study highlights the PT's potential for visualizing aesthetic engagement and suggests its integration with physiological methods like eye-tracking to explore the interaction between spontaneous attention and reflective aesthetic judgments. These findings underscore PT's adaptability and value as a tool for investigating aesthetic experiences across diverse art forms and cultural contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695241309780"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11826878/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143433656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chikamatsu, Mori, and the uncanny valley.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-06 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251317469
Karl F MacDorman
{"title":"Chikamatsu, Mori, and the uncanny valley.","authors":"Karl F MacDorman","doi":"10.1177/20416695251317469","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251317469","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In Japan, robotics projects like Geminoid, modeled after Hiroshi Ishiguro, exhibit a fascination with creating human doubles. Yet, warnings against this also thread through Japanese thought, from the Edo-period playwright Chikamatsu Monzaemon (1653-1724) to the robotics professor Mori Masahiro (1927-2025). Though centuries apart, they describe the same <i>uncanny valley</i> phenomenon-eerie, cold, repellent feelings that arise when confronting the imperfectly human. In an interview with Hozumi Ikan, translated here, Chikamatsu presents a theory of realism exemplified through puppet theater and kabuki. He divides realism into four zones: the unreal, conceptual realism, surface realism, and the real. The unreal lacks authenticity, surface realism lacks soul, and the real lacks expressiveness. For Chikamatsu, it is conceptual realism that captivates an audience. A play's unfolding events evoke empathy and emotion through their meaning for the characters. Similarly, Mori divides realism into four zones: industrial, humanoid, and android robots, and real people. Industrial robots evoke little affinity, and androids risk appearing eerie. Though real people evoke the most affinity, androids cannot become indistinguishable from them. For Mori, only humanoid robots evoke affinity without risking uncanniness. By exploring anthropomorphism, both Chikamatsu and Mori illuminate principles for designing robots that do not unsettle but delight.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695251317469"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11800272/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143366236","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sound effects have only minor contribution to perceptions of anthropomorphism and animacy of simple animated shapes.
IF 2.4 4区 心理学
I-Perception Pub Date : 2025-02-02 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251315382
K Collins, Maya Murad, Adel Manji
{"title":"Sound effects have only minor contribution to perceptions of anthropomorphism and animacy of simple animated shapes.","authors":"K Collins, Maya Murad, Adel Manji","doi":"10.1177/20416695251315382","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695251315382","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While studies of anthropomorphism have spanned many decades, there is little evidence of the role that sound effects may play. We present two studies into sound's influence on perceptions of anthropomorphism and animacy using simple geometric animated shapes. For the first study, conducted on 149 participants, we simplified the animation to just two \"bumping\" squares. Study Two recreated the Heider-Simmel study of 1944, and was conducted on 250 participants under five conditions: without sound, and with one of two different sound types (interface sounds and \"anthropomorphic\" robot sounds) with two stereo modes (fixed in stereo position, or binaurally panned with the movement). We had participants answer both the Individual Differences in Anthropomorphism Questionnaire and the Godspeed Questionnaire, with three additional questions added. Results showed that the sound had a minor impact on anthropomorphism and potency in Study One, but did not impact animacy. Study Two showed no significant effect on anthropomorphism or animacy, but did show an impact on perceived intelligence and perceptions of activity.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"16 1","pages":"20416695251315382"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11800268/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143366240","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信