{"title":"Social learning and diffusion of new foraging techniques in goats, Capra hircus","authors":"Laurie Castro , Myriam Amari , Raymond Nowak , Valérie Dufour","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123082","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123082","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Social learning, which enables the transfer of information between individuals, is a key adaptive function. Many ungulate species live in large gregarious groups, where there is potential to obtain vital information from conspecifics. However, there is a lack of data concerning the cognitive processes involved in social learning in the ungulates. This study focuses on a domestic species, the goat, <em>Capra hircus</em>, and tests whether they are capable of using social information to solve a foraging problem and if successive transmissions can corrupt information. We used a diffusion-chain paradigm, in which an individual can learn by observing a single trained model and subsequently become a model for the following individual in the chain. We used a puzzle box that could be opened using two different techniques. A majority of goats were able to use the same technique that they saw performed by a conspecific, with a positive association between opening success and rate of observation of the model. Unsuccessful goats also made more attempts to open the box with the technique previously used by the model. This is the first evidence of horizontal diffusion for a new foraging technique in goats. As dispersed foragers, goats may be efficient at monitoring the foraging activities of their conspecifics.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"221 ","pages":"Article 123082"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143096755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"In dogs, social stimuli overshadow nonsocial stimuli and stronger attachment correlates with responses to the latter","authors":"Kumi Shinoda , Nanae Noguchi , Nagi Kondo , Miho Nagasawa , Takefumi Kikusui","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123099","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123099","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The emotional reactivity hypothesis predicts that reduced fear and aggression in domesticated animals improves their social cognitive abilities towards humans. However, no studies in dogs, <em>Canis familiaris</em>, have shown that individual temperament is associated with higher social cognitive abilities, such as response to human pointing. In this study, we first trained dogs to learn correct responses in a two-choice task using a compound stimulus consisting of social (human pointing) and nonsocial stimuli (printed figures on paper) as cues. Social and nonsocial stimuli were presented alone to examine whether the social stimulus overshadowed the nonsocial stimulus. We also examined the correlation between temperament scores (based on the Canine Behavioral Assessment & Research Questionnaire, C-BARQ) and task performance. The results showed that social stimuli overshadowed nonsocial stimuli in most dogs, indicating that dogs used social stimuli from humans more frequently than nonsocial stimuli. Strong positive correlations were observed between attachment scores from the C-BARQ and correct response rates to nonsocial stimuli, but there was no correlation between aggression/fear and correct response rates to social stimuli. We considered that positive correlations between attachment scores and correct response rates to nonsocial stimuli indicate the degree of individual attentiveness to various environmental stimuli, and that responses to human pointing are likely to be reinforced from an early age. In addition, individual fear and aggression and social cognitive abilities can vary separately.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"221 ","pages":"Article 123099"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143096756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-04DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123074
Buddhamas P. Kriengwatana , Ruedi G. Nager , Alex South , Martin Ullrich , Emily L. Doolittle
{"title":"Playing music to animals: an interdisciplinary approach to improving our understanding of animals' responses to music","authors":"Buddhamas P. Kriengwatana , Ruedi G. Nager , Alex South , Martin Ullrich , Emily L. Doolittle","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123074","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123074","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Humans have profoundly changed the global soundscape. Studying how nonhuman animals respond to music can contribute to a better understanding of the effect of sound on animals. Animals are frequently exposed to human music, whether intentionally (for example, in laboratory settings), or unintentionally (for example, when animals live in close proximity to humans). Although several papers examine animals' responses to music, these typically do so from a purely animal behavioural perspective, sometimes missing relevant details about salient features of the music being played. An interdisciplinary approach that places musical and scientific knowledge on equal footing can improve our understanding of how animals respond to music and music-like sounds, in new and exciting ways. Here, we show with a systematic review that crucial factors (intrinsic music properties, listener properties, playback context and producer properties and contexts; ILPP) are not being adequately considered or reported in recently published scientific articles on the effects of music on animals, which hinders scientific reproducibility within this area of study. These problems are caused by improper referencing of music sources, misunderstanding of music and unexamined assumptions about individual variation and preferences between individuals of the same or different species. We then suggest that Berlyne’s psychobiological theory might provide a useful framework for studying how animals respond to human-generated sounds.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"221 ","pages":"Article 123074"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143096749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.018
Naïs Caron Delbosc , Nicolas Boyer , Nicolas Mathevon , Nicolas Grimault
{"title":"Crocodile mothers’ response to hatching calls","authors":"Naïs Caron Delbosc , Nicolas Boyer , Nicolas Mathevon , Nicolas Grimault","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.018","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.018","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In animal species with parental care, caregivers' investment is often driven by offspring solicitation signals. Previous studies, particularly in birds and mammals, show that parents modulate their response to these signals according to characteristics such as their intensity and number per unit of time. In crocodilians, mature embryos still in the egg emit hatching calls perceived by other embryos and by the adult guarding the nest. These calls help to synchronize hatching within the nest and encourage the adult to dig the nest and help the young to hatch. However, the embryos do not start calling all at once, and it can take several hours or even days after embryos have started to vocalize before the adult digs the nest. When and why does the crocodile parent decide to respond to the hatching calls of its offspring? Here we show that Nile crocodile, <em>Crocodylus niloticus</em>, mothers only start digging their nests if the hatching calls are emitted in a sustained manner by the offspring. While females react with head and body orientation movements to isolated calls, only continuous bursts of calls lasting several tens of seconds induce digging behaviour. This high response threshold on the part of the mothers probably limits the untimely digging up of eggs that may not have reached full maturity, thus avoiding damaging them, and is likely to favour the synchronization of offspring hatching. Overall, this study suggests that the parental response of Nile crocodiles to offspring solicitations is not necessarily linearly correlated with the number of begging signals received but may require a sufficient number of begging signals to exceed a threshold.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 123040"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143179405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123078
Etienne Sirot , Thomas Benoit , Frédéric M. Hamelin
{"title":"How coevolution in daily activity rhythms governs encounters between predator and prey","authors":"Etienne Sirot , Thomas Benoit , Frédéric M. Hamelin","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123078","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2025.123078","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Predation risk in the wild varies across the day in a relatively predictable way, as a function of environmental conditions, such as light intensity and temperature, and of predator habits. Prey animals can thus adjust their own activity rhythm to avoid the most dangerous periods. We studied this situation in a coevolutionary perspective, considering that, if prey spread their activity across the day to counter predator temporal preferences, these preferences may in turn change to track prey activity. We therefore built a game-theoretical model to derive evolutionarily stable activity patterns for a predator constantly trying to maximize its chance of capture and a prey trying to minimize this probability. Key parameters concern circadian variations in environmental conditions and their influence on predator hunting efficiency and energy demands of the prey, which dictate its total amount of activity. The model predicts high levels of prey activity during periods of reduced predator efficiency. The predator may then either track these activity peaks and copy the activity patterns of its prey, or concentrate its hunting effort during periods when prey activity is low but conditions favour hunting. In the first case, predator and prey activity patterns will be synchronized. In the second one, they will exhibit strong temporal segregation. We show how these diverging scenarios emerge and how they can help to disentangle the wide variety of situations existing in the wild.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"221 ","pages":"Article 123078"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143480431","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.12.004
Jonathan M. Parrett, Karolina Sobala, Sebastian Chmielewski, Karolina Przesmycka, Jacek Radwan
{"title":"No evidence of negative frequency-dependent selection in alternative reproductive tactics in a bulb mite","authors":"Jonathan M. Parrett, Karolina Sobala, Sebastian Chmielewski, Karolina Przesmycka, Jacek Radwan","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.12.004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.12.004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Alternative reproductive tactics typically involve males aggressively competing for access to females coexisting with males using nonaggressive, often sneaky tactics. When tactics are heritable, game-theoretic models are commonly invoked to explain such coexistence. These models, including the classic Hawk and Dove model, assume that a tactic's success decreases with its frequency in a population; however, evidence supporting this assumption is scarce. In this study, we investigated the frequency dependence of male reproductive success in an acarid mite, <em>Rhizoglyphus robini</em>, where heritable male morphs exhibiting alternative reproductive tactics, aggressive fighters and benign scramblers, coexist. We placed focal fighter or scrambler males in populations dominated by scramblers or fighters or containing even proportions of both morphs and, using the sterile male technique, determined the number of progeny they sired and the number of females inseminated. Although we found that focal male reproductive success differed depending on mating tactics between competitive mating environments, these differences do not support our predictions and the role of negative frequency-dependent selection in this system. These results question the Hawk–Dove model as an explanation and indicate other mechanisms are required to explain the coexistence of alternative reproductive tactics in <em>R. robini</em>.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 123048"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143179418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.002
Teresa Klaus, Bettina Wernisch, Sarah M. Zala, Dustin J. Penn
{"title":"Courtship vocalizations of wild house mice show highly dynamic changes and correlate with male copulatory success","authors":"Teresa Klaus, Bettina Wernisch, Sarah M. Zala, Dustin J. Penn","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Courtship vocalizations can influence mate choice and reproductive success and thus evolve through sexual selection. House mice, <em>Mus musculus</em>, are intensively studied; however, little is known about their vocalizations emitted during courtship sequences and whether and/or how they influence male copulatory success. To address these questions, we recorded the behaviour and vocalizations of pairs of wild house mice across distinct phases of courtship and mating. Over 53 000 vocalizations were detected and classified, and of these ca. 90% were ultrasonic (USV) and 10% were broadband (BBV) vocalizations, presumably emitted by males and females, respectively. Mice altered their vocal rate, composition and repertoire at each stage of courtship and mating. They increased the emissions of all simple USVs while reducing other calls upon contact with a potential mate. Then, once males began mounting and engaging in other sexual interactions, the pairs emitted more complex calls, especially harmonic USVs and BBVs with spectral nonlinearities. Vocalizations were closely associated with male mating behaviour and peaked in rates and complexity just before males approached the female to mount. USV bouts began earlier and contained more complex syllables when mounting attempts ended in copulation. As courtship progressed, the timing of USV and BBV emissions became tightly synchronized, as with duetting of songbirds. We observed several differences in the vocal repertoire and spectral features of calls between mice that successfully copulated with ejaculation and those that did not. USV emission was positively correlated with male sexual behaviours, especially among copulating mice, suggesting that the effect of USV emission on male mating success depends on their sexual behaviour, and vice versa. Our results show that the courtship vocalizations of wild house mice are more complex and dynamic than those previously reported and provide the first evidence for vocalizations that correlate with and predict male copulatory success.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 123024"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143178393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.022
Koutaro Ould Maeno , Ahmed Salem Benahi , Mohamed El Hacen Jaavar
{"title":"Moulting stage is not defenceless: antipredator strategy of gregarious locusts during moulting","authors":"Koutaro Ould Maeno , Ahmed Salem Benahi , Mohamed El Hacen Jaavar","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.022","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.022","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In arthropods, moulting is characterized by immobility and physical vulnerability, making individuals undergoing it sensitive to predation. Here, we examined how gregarious nymphs of desert locusts, <em>Schistocerca gregaria</em>, avoid predation during moulting, in the Sahara Desert. In the field, premoulting, gregarious, last-instar nymphs stopped marching and remained on a roosting bush. In response to an approaching simulated vertebrate predator, both pre- and postmoulting nymphs quickly dropped into the bush and hid themselves under branches. Although middle-moulting nymphs that were shedding an old exocuticle were immobile for approximately 10 min, once the femurs were out of the old exocuticle, the insects started to exhibit dropping/hiding escape behaviour. Behavioural experiments confirmed that pre-, middle- and postmoulting individuals were able to extend their wings by climbing after dropping. However, if dropped locusts were not allowed to climb and stayed on the ground, they failed to extend their wings. Hence, our results suggest that the integration of a physically protected moulting site choice before entering the moulting state, a dropping/hiding escape behaviour, which does not require dynamic locomotion, and flexible movement during the soft-bodied state could act to reduce moulting-associated predation. This is the first report of active behavioural and physical antipredator strategies in moulting gregarious juvenile arthropods.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 123044"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143179419","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Animal BehaviourPub Date : 2025-02-01DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.016
Rachael Miller , Markus Boeckle , Sophie Ridgway , James Richardson , Florian Uhl , Thomas Bugnyar , Christine Schwab
{"title":"Social attention across development in common ravens and carrion/hooded crows","authors":"Rachael Miller , Markus Boeckle , Sophie Ridgway , James Richardson , Florian Uhl , Thomas Bugnyar , Christine Schwab","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.016","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.11.016","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Social attention involves individuals attending to the presence, identity and/or behaviour of others, which may facilitate cooperation, communication and social learning. Individuals may be selective in when and to which individuals they attend, which may be influenced by social context (e.g. observer identity) and development. In 10 carrion/hooded crows, <em>Corvus corone corone/C. c. cornix</em>, and nine common ravens, <em>Corvus corax</em>, we tested the influence of social context (alone, sibling/affiliate, nonsibling/nonaffiliate, heterospecific) on behavioural responses (item manipulation, caching and ‘head and body out of sight’, i.e. barrier use) with familiar food and objects. We tested subjects during development at fledging (1–2 months), juvenile (3–8 months) and subadult (14–18 months old) stages. Subjects were hand reared and housed in comparable conditions. These two species are closely related, generalist corvids, which will routinely cache (i.e. hide food and other items for later recovery) and engage in cache-pilfering (stealing) strategies. Item manipulation and caching may contribute to the development of physical and/or social skills. Subject behaviour was influenced by social context, with birds showing higher frequency of ‘head and body out of sight’ (barrier use) behaviour with (any) observer present than when alone. Observer identity had no effect, suggesting item interaction may have facilitated development of physical (rather than influencing social) skills in this setting. There were developmental effects, including increased manipulation and use of barriers as juveniles, and increased caching with age. Ravens cached more than crows. Objects were manipulated more frequently than food. Barriers were used more with food, indicating food was more actively hidden, while object manipulation may promote low-risk interaction and learning. We discuss our findings in relation to social and developmental influences on behaviour, in relation to social attention across ontogeny in animals.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 123038"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143179421","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}