SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
School Connectedness and Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviors: A Systematic Meta-Analysis 学校关联与自杀思想和行为的系统元分析
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2017-01-12 DOI: 10.1037/spq0000192
Marisa E. Marraccini, Zoe M F Brier
{"title":"School Connectedness and Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviors: A Systematic Meta-Analysis","authors":"Marisa E. Marraccini, Zoe M F Brier","doi":"10.1037/spq0000192","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000192","url":null,"abstract":"Among the protective factors associated with reduced risk for suicide, scientific inquiries into school connectedness are especially important considering that schools are ideally situated to provide interventions reaching the vast majority of youth. Although there is a wealth of research that supports the association between school connectedness and reduced self-report of adolescents having a suicidal thought or making a suicide attempt, inconsistencies in the way studies have measured and operationalized school connectedness limit synthesis across findings. This meta-analytic study investigates the literature exploring associations between school connectedness and suicidal thoughts and behaviors across general and subpopulations (high risk and sexual minority youth) using a random effects model. Eligible studies examined a measure of school connectedness explicitly referred to as “school connectedness” or “connections at school” in relation to suicidal ideation or suicide attempts among youth enrolled in school (Grades 6–12). Multiple metaregression analyses were conducted to explore the influence of school connectedness measurement variation, as well as participant characteristics. Results, including 16 samples, support that higher school connectedness is associated with reduced reports of suicidal thoughts and behaviors across general (odds ratio [OR] = 0.536), high-risk (OR = 0.603), and sexual minority (OR = 0.608) adolescents. Findings are consistent when analyzed separately for suicidal ideation (OR = 0.529) and suicide attempts (OR = 0.589) and remain stable when accounting for measurement variability. Although limited by its cross-sectional nature, findings support recent calls to increase school connectedness and proffer important implications for screening and intervention efforts conducted in schools.","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"32 1","pages":"5–21"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/spq0000192","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48881972","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 133
Is Perceived Control a Critical Factor in Understanding the Negative Relationship Between Cognitive Test Anxiety and Examination Performance? 认知控制是理解认知考试焦虑与考试成绩负相关关系的关键因素吗?
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2016-11-10 DOI: 10.1037/spq0000183
D. Putwain, Ben Aveyard
{"title":"Is Perceived Control a Critical Factor in Understanding the Negative Relationship Between Cognitive Test Anxiety and Examination Performance?","authors":"D. Putwain, Ben Aveyard","doi":"10.1037/spq0000183","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000183","url":null,"abstract":"A well established finding is that the cognitive component of test anxiety (worry) is negatively related to examination performance. The present study examined how 3 self-beliefs (academic buoyancy, perceived control, and test competence) moderated the strength of the relationship between worry and examination performance in a sample of 270 final year secondary school students. Participants completed self-reports of academic buoyancy, perceived control, test competence, and cognitive test anxiety, that were matched with examination grades in English, science, and mathematics. Results showed an interaction between worry and perceived control. Students with higher perceived control performed better at low levels of worry. As worry increased, the differential advantage offered by higher perceived control diminished. At high levels of worry control made little difference to examination performance. Interventions designed to reduce worry may not necessarily improve examination performance unless they also target improved control.","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"33 1","pages":"65–74"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-11-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/spq0000183","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57352773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 31
Curriculum-based measurement of oral reading: A preliminary investigation of confidence interval overlap to detect reliable growth. 基于课程的口语阅读测量:可信区间重叠的初步调查,以检测可靠的增长。
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2016-02-11 DOI: 10.1037/spq0000146
Ethan R. Van Norman
{"title":"Curriculum-based measurement of oral reading: A preliminary investigation of confidence interval overlap to detect reliable growth.","authors":"Ethan R. Van Norman","doi":"10.1037/spq0000146","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000146","url":null,"abstract":"Curriculum-based measurement of oral reading (CBM-R) progress monitoring data is used to measure student response to instruction. Federal legislation permits educators to use CBM-R progress monitoring data as a basis for determining the presence of specific learning disabilities. However, decision making frameworks originally developed for CBM-R progress monitoring data were not intended for such high stakes assessments. Numerous documented issues with trend line estimation undermine the validity of using slope estimates to infer progress. One proposed recommendation is to use confidence interval overlap as a means of judging reliable growth. This project explored the degree to which confidence interval overlap was related to true growth magnitude using simulation methodology. True and observed CBM-R scores were generated across 7 durations of data collection (range 6-18 weeks), 3 levels of dataset quality or residual variance (5, 10, and 15 words read correct per minute) and 2 types of data collection schedules. Descriptive and inferential analyses were conducted to explore interactions between overlap status, progress monitoring scenarios, and true growth magnitude. A small but statistically significant interaction was observed between overlap status, duration, and dataset quality, b = -0.004, t(20992) =-7.96, p < .001. In general, confidence interval overlap does not appear to meaningfully account for variance in true growth across many progress monitoring conditions. Implications for research and practice are discussed. Limitations and directions for future research are addressed. (PsycINFO Database Record","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"31 3 1","pages":"405-418"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57352640","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Modeling early writing growth with curriculum-based measures 用课程为基础的方法对早期写作成长进行建模
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0026833
David C. Parker, Kristen L. McMaster, Amanuel Medhanie, B. Silberglitt
{"title":"Modeling early writing growth with curriculum-based measures","authors":"David C. Parker, Kristen L. McMaster, Amanuel Medhanie, B. Silberglitt","doi":"10.1037/A0026833","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0026833","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"290-304"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0026833","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57390949","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
Social prominence and the heterogeneity of rejected status in late elementary school 小学后期社会地位与被拒绝地位的异质性
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0025624
Thomas W. Farmer, C. M. Hall, M. Leung, David B. Estell, Debbie Brooks
{"title":"Social prominence and the heterogeneity of rejected status in late elementary school","authors":"Thomas W. Farmer, C. M. Hall, M. Leung, David B. Estell, Debbie Brooks","doi":"10.1037/A0025624","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0025624","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"260-274"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0025624","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57376277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Cognitive Assessment System: Variance Partitions from the Schmid-Leiman (1957) Procedure. 认知评估系统的层次因素结构:来自Schmid-Leiman(1957)程序的方差划分。
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0025973
Gary L. Canivez
{"title":"Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Cognitive Assessment System: Variance Partitions from the Schmid-Leiman (1957) Procedure.","authors":"Gary L. Canivez","doi":"10.1037/A0025973","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0025973","url":null,"abstract":"Orthogonal higher-order factor structure of the Cognitive Assessment System (CAS; Naglieri & Das, 1997a) for the 5–7 and 8 –17 age groups in the CAS standardization sample is reported. Following the same procedure as recent studies of other prominent intelligence tests (Dombrowski, Watkins, & Brogan, 2009; Canivez, 2008; Canivez & Watkins, 2010a, 2010b; Nelson & Canivez, 2011; Nelson, Canivez, Lindstrom, & Hatt, 2007; Watkins, 2006; Watkins, Wilson, Kotz, Carbone, & Babula, 2006), three- and four-factor CAS exploratory factor extractions were analyzed with the Schmid and Leiman (1957) procedure using MacOrtho (Watkins, 2004) to assess the hierarchical factor structure by sequentially partitioning variance to the second- and first- order dimensions as recommended by Carroll (1993, 1995). Results showed that greater portions of total and common variance were accounted for by the second-order, global factor, but compared to other tests of intelligence CAS subtests measured less secondorder variance and greater first-order Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive (PASS) factor variance.","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"305-317"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0025973","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57380902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 35
Spearman's law of diminishing returns and the DAS-II: Do g effects on subtest scores depend on the level of g? 斯皮尔曼收益递减定律和DAS-II: g对子测试分数的影响是否取决于g的水平?
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0026190
Matthew R. Reynolds, Daniel B. Hajovsky, Christopher R. Niileksela, T. Keith
{"title":"Spearman's law of diminishing returns and the DAS-II: Do g effects on subtest scores depend on the level of g?","authors":"Matthew R. Reynolds, Daniel B. Hajovsky, Christopher R. Niileksela, T. Keith","doi":"10.1037/A0026190","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0026190","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"275-289"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0026190","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57383331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Adding a Vocabulary Component to Incremental Rehearsal to Enhance Retention and Generalization 在增量排练中增加词汇组件以增强记忆和泛化
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0024914
Shawna Petersen‐Brown, M. Burns
{"title":"Adding a Vocabulary Component to Incremental Rehearsal to Enhance Retention and Generalization","authors":"Shawna Petersen‐Brown, M. Burns","doi":"10.1037/A0024914","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0024914","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"245-255"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0024914","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57367158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 23
A Multiple Risk Factors Model of the Development of Aggression Among Early Adolescents From Urban Disadvantaged Neighborhoods 城市弱势社区早期青少年攻击行为发展的多风险因素模型
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0024116
Sangwon Kim, P. Orpinas, R. Kamphaus, S. Kelder
{"title":"A Multiple Risk Factors Model of the Development of Aggression Among Early Adolescents From Urban Disadvantaged Neighborhoods","authors":"Sangwon Kim, P. Orpinas, R. Kamphaus, S. Kelder","doi":"10.1037/A0024116","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0024116","url":null,"abstract":"This study empirically derived a multiple risk factors model of the development of aggression among middle school students in urban, low-income neighborhoods, using Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM). RESULTS indicated that aggression increased from sixth to eighth grade. Additionally, the influences of four risk domains (individual, family, community, and media) were tested individually and simultaneously, providing evidence that multiple risk variables at multiple levels influenced the development of aggression with varying degrees. While nearly all risk factors predicted variability in the initial level of aggression, few factors were adequate predictors of changes in aggression. Implications for future research and for evidence-based school interventions are discussed. Language: en","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"215-230"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0024116","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57353101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 20
Improving the Efficacy of First Grade Reading Screening: An Investigation of Word Identification Fluency with Other Early Literacy Indicators. 提高一年级阅读筛选的效果:词语识别流畅性与其他早期识字指标的调查。
SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY Pub Date : 2011-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/A0025173
Nathan H. Clemens, E. Shapiro, F. Thoemmes
{"title":"Improving the Efficacy of First Grade Reading Screening: An Investigation of Word Identification Fluency with Other Early Literacy Indicators.","authors":"Nathan H. Clemens, E. Shapiro, F. Thoemmes","doi":"10.1037/A0025173","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/A0025173","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigated the accuracy of several early literacy measures that have been used in research and practice for first grade reading screening. A set of measures, Word Identification Fluency (WIF), Letter Naming Fluency, Phoneme Segmentation Fluency, and Nonsense Word Fluency, were administered as screening measures with 138 first grade students in the fall of the school year. Reading skills were assessed at the end of first grade using Oral Reading Fluency, the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (Sight Word Efficiency and Phonemic Decoding Efficiency), reading Maze, and a latent variable composite. Analyses compared the accuracy at which each screening measure, as well as combinations of screening measures, classified students according to successful or unsuccessful reading outcomes at the end of the year. Receiver operating characteristic curve analyses indicated that when compared with other single screening measures, WIF tended to demonstrate the strongest overall classification accuracy. With levels of sensitivity held constant at .90, combining screening measures with WIF using predicted probabilities from logistic regression analyses resulted in modest improvements in accuracy by reducing the number of false positives. More measures were not always better, however, as models of two or three measures were as accurate as models consisting of all measures. Overall results provided support for WIF as a starting point for first grade reading screening, whereby additional steps might be taken to reduce false positives.","PeriodicalId":48005,"journal":{"name":"SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY","volume":"26 1","pages":"231-244"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/A0025173","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"57370454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 59
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信