{"title":"A tale of one letter: Morphological processing in early Arabic spelling","authors":"Elinor Saiegh-Haddad","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.857586","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.857586","url":null,"abstract":"The study examined spelling of the letter ت in Arabic among first-, second-, third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade native Arabic-speaking children (N = 150). The letter is among the most frequent letters in Arabic and it participates in the encoding of three productive morphological entities: root, word-pattern and affix. The letter is also homographic and may represent the default voiceless dental-alveolar stop phoneme /t/ as well as its emphatic allophonic variant [ṭ] coinciding, hence, with the phoneme typically represented by the letter ط. The study tested whether children use morphological cues in spelling the letter in Arabic, and whether morphological processing is different for different morphemes and in different grades. The results indicate that morphological processing is functional very early on in Arabic spelling among children. Yet, morphological processing appears to depend on the specific morpheme targeted, with some morphemes lending themselves more strongly to morphological processing than others. The results are discussed within the framework of the morphological and morpho-orthographic structure of Arabic.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"169 - 188"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.857586","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60437230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Maternal mediation of writing and children's early spelling and reading: The Semitic abjad versus the European alphabet","authors":"I. Levin, Dorit Aram, L. Tolchinsky, C. McBride","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.797335","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.797335","url":null,"abstract":"Maternal writing mediation and children's literacy were analysed in two writing systems; the Semitic abjad and the European alphabet. Forty Israeli Hebrew-speaking and 43 Spanish-speaking mother-child dyads participated in this study. The children, aged: M=68.58 months, had not yet been exposed to formal reading instruction. Israeli kindergartners embark on their initial steps in reading and spelling in an abjad—a consonantal writing system that deficiently and inconsistently marks vowels by letters. Spanish kindergartners are introduced to a shallow alphabetic writing system that consistently marks consonants and vowels. This study assessed: (1) children's code-based skills (letter knowledge and phonological awareness), spelling, and reading, and (2) mothers' word writing mediation. The groups were basically similar in code-based skills, but the reading and spelling of the Israeli children were substantially lower than those of their Spanish counterparts. Maternal writing mediation was lower among Israeli than Spanish mothers with respect to vowels. Regression analyses showed that children's spelling in Hebrew and in Spanish were predicted by children's code-based skills and by maternal writing mediation. Children's reading in Hebrew was uniquely predicted by code-based skills and in Spanish by maternal writing mediation. This study sheds light on the importance of writing mediation and its relation to writing systems.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"134 - 155"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.797335","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60436235","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Diacritics and the Perso-Arabic script","authors":"D. Kurzon","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.799451","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.799451","url":null,"abstract":"In this paper, I will examine diacritics in order to show that the graphemics of a language is in principle dependent on its phonological structure. I distinguish three types of diacritics: (1) Those that represent distinctive features, i.e., that have a consistent function in the specific writing system. Examples include the dieresis in German indicating vowel fronting, and the “caron” or háček in Czech indicating palatalisation; (2) those that are used to distinguish phonemes, usually consonants, but in an inconsistent manner; this is a very flexible type of diacritic used in an inconsistent manner such as in the adoption of a “foreign” writing system and its adaptation in order to represent the phonemes of the adopting language, e.g., not only dots added to Arabic rasms to distinguish consonants in Arabic, but also those added to represent non-Arabic consonants that occur in languages such as Persian, Urdu, and Sindhi; (3) Those that indicate vowels in abjad writing systems such as Arabic and Hebrew.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"234 - 243"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.799451","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60436248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"The effects of orthographic complexity and diglossia on letter naming in Arabic: A developmental study","authors":"Hanan Asaad, Z. Eviatar","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.862163","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.862163","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of the study was to examine the influence of orthographic complexity and diglossia on letter naming and automaticity in Arabic. Two experiments were carried out by 31 first graders, 30 third graders, 34 fifth graders and 20 university students. In the first experiment we took advantage of the Arabic orthographic variation in letter shape, and compared the Stroop effect for correctly written and orthographically distorted words. All participants revealed a Stroop effect with both types of words, but only first graders showed the same degree of interference with distorted and correctly written words. We interpret these results to reflect the development of automaticity in reading. In the second experiment, six letter-naming tests were performed. The results showed that retrieval time of naming letters or the sounds that these letters represent decreased inversely with age. A different pattern was found between the school-age children and the university students. In children, the relationships between types of tests of retrieval speed remained constant: retrieval of letter names or sounds which do not have visual or phonological neighbours was the fastest, and of letter names representing sounds that do not exist in spoken Arabic was the slowest. There was no effect of changing letter shape. However, among the university students only changing letter shape affected the speed of responses. We interpret these results to reflect different representations of letter categories in adults and children. The findings have implications for models of reading development in Arabic.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"156 - 168"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.862163","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60437334","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Orthographic constraints on phonological awareness in biliteracy development","authors":"P. Reddy, K. Koda","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2012.748639","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2012.748639","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This study examined how orthography-specific demands affect decoding development in 10–14-year-old multilingual students learning to read Kannada and English in India. Given that there is dual-level representation of the size of phonological information (syllable and phoneme) in alphasyllabic Kannada and single-level (phoneme) representation in alphabetic English, our study posed three questions: (1) Are there distinct phonological awareness (PA) levels corresponding to decoding development in Kannada as a first language of literacy vs. English as a second language of literacy?; (2) What is the relationship between phonological awareness in Kannada and that in English?; and (3) How does phonological awareness in Kannada contribute to decoding development in English? The results provided evidence for differences in the level at which phonological information is graphically encoded in each language. The findings further suggested that there are orthography-specific constraints on transfer of phonological awareness from alphasyllabic Kannada to alphabetic English.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"110 - 130"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2012.748639","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60435528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Community-based orthography development in four Western Zambian languages","authors":"C. Bow","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2012.747427","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2012.747427","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Community-based orthography development engages the native speakers as custodians of the language in decisions about how it should be written. While there are various guidelines on how to go about such an activity, examples of the implementation and resulting challenges are underrepresented in the literature. This paper describes a workshop which brought together native speakers from four Bantu languages of Western Zambia to establish writing systems for their languages (Fwe, Mashi, Makoma and Kwangwa) and considers some of the linguistic and non-linguistic issues involved in initial development of writing systems.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"50 1","pages":"73 - 87"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2012.747427","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60435464","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Eye dialect and casual speech spelling: Orthographic variation in OT","authors":"Antonio Baroni","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.808155","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.808155","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Nonstandard spelling of certain words in English, French and other languages is quite a widespread phenomenon, commonly referred to as Eye Dialect. Typical examples are instead of in English and instead of in French. Eye Dialect, despite using nonstandard spelling, maintains grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences that reflect standard pronunciation, unlike Casual Speech Spelling, which aims to transcribe substandard forms (e.g., ). In this paper I attempt to account for both phenomena in a framework couched in Optimality Theory, partly drawing on a set of constraints already proposed in existing literature (Song & Wiese, 2010), at the same time as proposing new ones justified on phonetic, cognitive or system-internal grounds. It is shown how Eye Dialect and Casual Speech Spelling, instead of creating new sound-to-letter relationships, promote the more general ones, at the expense of very specific or idiosyncratic phoneme-to-grapheme mappings. There are several other factors that seem to interact: the preference for 1:1 relationships (e.g., unambiguous graphemes are preferred to ambiguous ones), the acoustic salience of certain segments or features (e.g., stridency or nasality are more likely to require a graphic representation than glides or schwa), the visual salience of certain letters (e.g., letters whose shape extends to upper and/or lower spaces seem to contribute more to word recognition and are thus more likely to be retained). It is interesting to note that logography and rebus writing are also employed, along with Eye Dialect and Casual Speech Spelling. The common intent seems to be simplicity and, possibly, rebellion.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"24 - 53"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.808155","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60436255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"The graphematic foot in English and German","authors":"Martin Evertz, Beatrice Primus","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.765356","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.765356","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In traditional graphematics words are represented as a linear sequence of letters. We will present a non-linear graphematic approach which supplements linearity with a hierarchical graphematic structure. This hierarchy of graphematic units comprises letter features, letters, graphemes, syllables, feet and words. We will present structural and experimental evidence for this hierarchical organisation of graphematic units. Our focus lies on the graphematic foot and on the graphematics of English and German. We have found an asymmetry between a canonical and a non-canonical structural organisation of graphematic words. The regularities found in these types of structure can be captured with reference to hierarchical graphematic structures and to foot structures in particular. In order to elucidate whether graphematic foot structure is a relevant unit in graphematics that may influence the analysis of the corresponding phonological foot structure we have conducted a production experiment with German pseudowords.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"1 - 23"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.765356","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60436168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"On a “kinetic”-like sequence in rongorongo tablet “Mamari”","authors":"Tomi S. Melka","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2012.742005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2012.742005","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The paper addresses the question of “kinetic”-like attributes observed on a choice of glyphs in rongorongo tablet “Mamari” (lines 9–10, side b, text C). This tablet, a medium-sized rongorongo text, as near as I can tell, has been aptly referred to by researchers and commentators as containing mixed genres. “Mamari” also has a unique place in the corpus of Easter Island inscriptions due to its “lunar calendar” fragment. The sequence in Cb9–10 elicits interest since it may shed light on the disputed nature of rongorongo script. A suggested textual parallel on side a of “Mamari” gives ground to cautious discussions on the degree of variation, paraphrasing and economy of expression, as well as to the plausible sound-mapping in the rongorongo orthography. Similarly, it is shown by comparison of earlier tracings of Thomas Barthel and Steven R. Fischer with the photographs of the actual artefact that the accuracy of the published drawn corpus still requires some amendments, and hence all further studies are supposed to pay more attention to the use of photographic material or investigation of the original. These issues are especially crucial for palaeographic and calligraphic research.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"54 - 72"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2012.742005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60435784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Parent-child joint writing in Chinese kindergarteners: Explicit instruction in radical knowledge and stroke writing skills","authors":"S. Lam, C. McBride-Chang","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2013.812532","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2013.812532","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The present study aimed to investigate the effectiveness of two types of learning, stroke order writing and radical knowledge training, for enhancing children's Chinese literacy skills in the context of parent-child joint Chinese writing. Eighty Hong Kong kindergarteners were pretested on nonverbal IQ, word reading, vocabulary knowledge, semantic-radical awareness tasks, writing dictation and single character reading. Then they were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: stroke training, radical training or control. After eight weeks of training, the radical condition yielded greater improvement compared with the control condition in dictation and semantic-radical awareness tasks. The findings of the study highlight the potential positive effects of parent-child joint writing on children's Chinese language and literacy skills. Findings also demonstrate that parents typically use multiple strategies in parent-child joint writing, and it is likely that different approaches to writing might facilitate literacy knowledge in different ways with development.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"5 1","pages":"109 - 88"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2013.812532","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60436360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}