Child development research最新文献

筛选
英文 中文
Early vocabulary development of Australian Indigenous Children: Identifying strengths 澳洲原住民儿童早期词汇发展:优势识别
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1155/2014/942817
B. Farrant, Carrington C. J. Shepherd, R. Walker, Glenn Pearson
{"title":"Early vocabulary development of Australian Indigenous Children: Identifying strengths","authors":"B. Farrant, Carrington C. J. Shepherd, R. Walker, Glenn Pearson","doi":"10.1155/2014/942817","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/942817","url":null,"abstract":"The current study sought to increase our understanding of the factors involved in the early vocabulary development of Australian Indigenous children. Data from the Longitudinal Study of Indigenous Children were available for 573 Indigenous children (291 boys) who spoke English ( months, months, at wave 3). Data were also available for 86 children (51 boys) who spoke an Indigenous language ( months, months, at wave 3). As hypothesised, higher levels of parent-child book reading and having more children’s books in the home were associated with better English vocabulary development. Oral storytelling in Indigenous language was a significant predictor of the size of children’s Indigenous vocabulary.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-7"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/942817","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64755220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Taxonomies support preschoolers’ knowledge acquisition from storybooks 分类法支持学龄前儿童从故事书中获取知识
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-03-25 DOI: 10.1155/2014/386762
A. Pinkham, Tanya Kaefer, S. Neuman
{"title":"Taxonomies support preschoolers’ knowledge acquisition from storybooks","authors":"A. Pinkham, Tanya Kaefer, S. Neuman","doi":"10.1155/2014/386762","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/386762","url":null,"abstract":"For young children, storybooks may serve as especially valuable sources of new knowledge. While most research focuses on how extratextual comments influence knowledge acquisition, we propose that children’s learning may also be supported by the specific features of storybooks. More specifically, we propose that texts that invoke children’s knowledge of familiar taxonomic categories may support learning by providing a conceptual framework through which prior knowledge and new knowledge can be readily integrated. In this study, 60 5-year olds were read a storybook that either invoked their knowledge of a familiar taxonomic category (taxonomic storybook) or focused on a common thematic grouping (traditional storybook). Following the book-reading, children’s vocabulary acquisition, literal comprehension, and inferential comprehension were assessed. Children who were read the taxonomic storybook demonstrated greater acquisition of target vocabulary and comprehension of factual content than children who were read the traditional storybook. Inferential comprehension, however, did not differ across the two conditions. We argue for the importance of careful consideration of book features and storybook selection in order to provide children with every opportunity to gain the knowledge foundational for successful literacy development.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/386762","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64473806","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Abnormalities in Pattern of Lateralization in Relation to Visuospatial Short Term Memory in Children with Williams Syndrome 威廉斯综合征儿童与视觉空间短期记忆相关的侧化模式异常
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-03-20 DOI: 10.1155/2014/491458
Antonios Chasouris, P. Mayer, I. Stuart-Hamilton, M. Graff, L. Workman
{"title":"Abnormalities in Pattern of Lateralization in Relation to Visuospatial Short Term Memory in Children with Williams Syndrome","authors":"Antonios Chasouris, P. Mayer, I. Stuart-Hamilton, M. Graff, L. Workman","doi":"10.1155/2014/491458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/491458","url":null,"abstract":"Williams syndrome (WS) is a genetic disorder characterised by significant intellectual disability. Initial studies indicate that children with WS have a profound bias for information in the top left of visual arrays. Study 1, using a visuospatial memory test for items presented in a matrix, found a significant top left bias in WS children relative to controls. Study 2 used a probe-based memory test with arrays in which items appeared with equal probability in each position. Relative to controls, WS children showed a significant top and left bias. In Study 3, the same children engaged in a visual search task and again, a top and left bias was found in the WS group. It is concluded that children with WS display atypical laterality, which might be explained by abnormal saccadic movements, by abnormalities involving development of the dorsal stream or by uneven cortical development.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/491458","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64519183","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Smart Nonconserver: Preschoolers Detect Their Number Conservation Errors 聪明的不保守者:学龄前儿童发现他们的数字保守错误
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-02-12 DOI: 10.1155/2014/768186
W. Neys, A. Lubin, O. Houdé
{"title":"The Smart Nonconserver: Preschoolers Detect Their Number Conservation Errors","authors":"W. Neys, A. Lubin, O. Houdé","doi":"10.1155/2014/768186","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/768186","url":null,"abstract":"Classic developmental studies have established that children’s number conservation is often biased by misleading intuitions. However, the precise nature of these conservation errors is not clear. A key question is whether children detect that their erroneous conservation judgment is unwarranted. The present study focuses on this critical error sensitivity issue. Preschool children were given a classic version of a number conservation task in which an intuitively cued response conflicted with the correct conservation response and a control version in which this conflict was not present. After solving each version children were asked to indicate their response confidence. Results showed that in contrast with children who gave a correct conservation response, preschoolers who erred showed a sharp confidence decrease after solving the classic conflict problem. This suggests that nonconserving preschoolers detect that their response is questionable and are less ignorant about conservation than their well-documented errors might have previously suggested.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-7"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-02-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/768186","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64663803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 23
Comparing Adolescent Only Children with Those Who Have Siblings on Academic Related Outcomes and Psychosocial Adjustment 独生子女与有兄弟姐妹的青少年在学业相关结果和心理社会适应方面的比较
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-01-14 DOI: 10.1155/2014/578289
Zeng-yin Chen, Ruth X. Liu
{"title":"Comparing Adolescent Only Children with Those Who Have Siblings on Academic Related Outcomes and Psychosocial Adjustment","authors":"Zeng-yin Chen, Ruth X. Liu","doi":"10.1155/2014/578289","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/578289","url":null,"abstract":"This study uses a large and representative sample of adolescents to test the theoretically informed hypotheses comparing adolescent singletons with those who have siblings. The results found that, for academic related outcomes (educational expectations, time spent on homework, and self-reported grades), there are no differences between singletons and firstborns who have any number of younger siblings. Singletons are also not different from laterborns from two-child families. In contrast, singletons are more advantageous compared to laterborns who have two or more siblings on educational expectations and grades. Singletons also spend more time on homework than laterborns who have three or more siblings. For psychosocial outcomes (psychological distress, susceptibility to negative peer pressure, and problem behaviors), singletons are not different from both firstborns and laterborns with any number of siblings. The findings suggest that singletons are not at any disadvantage compared to their peers who have siblings and they enjoy some advantages over laterborns from medium to large families on academic related outcomes.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/578289","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64555547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 33
Cognitive Flexibility, Theory of Mind, and Hyperactivity/Inattention 认知灵活性,心理理论和多动/注意力不集中
Child development research Pub Date : 2014-01-12 DOI: 10.1155/2014/741543
B. Farrant, J. Fletcher, M. Maybery
{"title":"Cognitive Flexibility, Theory of Mind, and Hyperactivity/Inattention","authors":"B. Farrant, J. Fletcher, M. Maybery","doi":"10.1155/2014/741543","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/741543","url":null,"abstract":"The present study analyzed the concurrent and longitudinal relations among cognitive flexibility, theory of mind, and hyperactivity/inattention in a sample of 70 typically developing children ( age = 61.4 months, SD = 8.3 months). Mothers and teachers reported on children’s hyperactivity/inattention using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (Goodman, 1997), cognitive flexibility was measured using the dimension change card sort task (Zelazo, 2006), and theory of mind was assessed using a battery of tasks. Cognitive flexibility and theory of mind scores were found to be significantly negatively correlated with the level of hyperactivity/inattention at both time points. Furthermore, year 1 cognitive flexibility score was found to be a significant predictor of year 2 hyperactivity/inattention score after controlling for child age, gender, and year 1 hyperactivity/inattention score. Directions for future research include training studies which would further our understanding of these relationships and allow more effective interventions.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2014 1","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2014-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2014/741543","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64648860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 21
Children’s Trust Beliefs in Others and Trusting Behavior in Peer Interaction 儿童对他人的信任信念与同伴交往中的信任行为
Child development research Pub Date : 2013-12-25 DOI: 10.1155/2013/806597
K. Rotenberg, S. Petrocchi, F. Lecciso, A. Marchetti
{"title":"Children’s Trust Beliefs in Others and Trusting Behavior in Peer Interaction","authors":"K. Rotenberg, S. Petrocchi, F. Lecciso, A. Marchetti","doi":"10.1155/2013/806597","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/806597","url":null,"abstract":"The relation between children’s trust beliefs and trusting behavior in peer interaction was examined. One hundred and 5 Italian children (54 boys; mean age = 10 years-7 months) completed standardized scales of reliability (i.e., promise keeping) trust beliefs in parents and peers. The children participated in mixed-motive interactions with classmates which assessed behavior-dependent reliability trust on peers. The children’s reliability trustworthiness towards peers/classmates was assessed by peer reports. The SEM analyses supported the hypothesized model by showing: (1) a path between trust beliefs in parents and trust beliefs in peers; (2) paths between both types of trust beliefs and behavior-dependent trust on peers; (3) a path between behavior-dependent trust in peers and trustworthiness towards peers. Trust beliefs in peers were found to mediate the relation between trust beliefs in parents and behavior-dependent trust in peers. The findings yielded support for the basis, domain, and target trust framework and attachment theory.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2013 1","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-12-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2013/806597","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64261707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
A Bidirectional Relationship between Conceptual Organization and Word Learning 概念组织与词汇学习的双向关系
Child development research Pub Date : 2013-12-22 DOI: 10.1155/2013/298603
Tanya Kaefer, S. Neuman
{"title":"A Bidirectional Relationship between Conceptual Organization and Word Learning","authors":"Tanya Kaefer, S. Neuman","doi":"10.1155/2013/298603","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/298603","url":null,"abstract":"This study explores the relationship between word learning and conceptual organization for preschool-aged children. We proposed a bidirectional model in which increases in word learning lead to increases in taxonomic organization, which, in turn, leads to further increases in word learning. In order to examine this model, we recruited 104 4-year olds from Head Start classrooms; 52 children participated in a two-week training program, and 52 children were in a control group. Results indicated that children in the training program learned more words and were more likely to sort taxonomically than children in the control condition. Furthermore, the number of words learned over the training period predicted the extent to which children categorized taxonomically. Additionally, this ability to categorize taxonomically predicted the number of words learned outside the training program, over and above the number of words learned in the program. These results suggest a bi-directional relationship between conceptual organization and word learning.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2013 1","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2013/298603","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64403052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Associations of Motor Developmental Risks with the Socioeconomic Status of Preschool Children in North-Eastern Germany 德国东北部学龄前儿童运动发育风险与社会经济地位的关系
Child development research Pub Date : 2013-12-18 DOI: 10.1155/2013/790524
A. Gottschling-Lang, M. Franze, andWolfgang Hoffmann
{"title":"Associations of Motor Developmental Risks with the Socioeconomic Status of Preschool Children in North-Eastern Germany","authors":"A. Gottschling-Lang, M. Franze, andWolfgang Hoffmann","doi":"10.1155/2013/790524","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/790524","url":null,"abstract":"Aims. The study is part of the pilot project “children in preschools” and aims to detect developmental risks of preschool children in the context of their socioeconomic status (SES) as a base to initiate individual intervention strategies. Methods. The “Dortmund Developmental Screening for the Kindergarten” was used in 12 preschools in Mecklenburg-Western Pomerania (MWP) to detect early developmental risks in children aged 3 to 6 years (). Socioeconomic data from parents were collected by a standardised questionnaire. Results. Significant differences between the SES groups were identified especially in the field of fine motor skills (). In gross motor development differences were not statistically significant. Prevalence rate of fine motor developmental risks ranges from 1.7% to 20.9%; the rate of gross motor developmental risks tops out at 14.4%. The prevalence rates are associated with age and sex. Conclusions. Fine motor skills in 3–6 years old preschool children are significantly associated with the socioeconomic status. In gross motor skills an association could not be identified. In this study, motor development was more affected by sex than by SES.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2013 1","pages":"1-6"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2013/790524","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64254389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Children’s Perceptions of Their Play: Scale Development and Validation 儿童对游戏的感知:规模发展与验证
Child development research Pub Date : 2013-11-21 DOI: 10.1155/2013/284741
L. Barnett
{"title":"Children’s Perceptions of Their Play: Scale Development and Validation","authors":"L. Barnett","doi":"10.1155/2013/284741","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/284741","url":null,"abstract":"A wealth of research has been conducted on children’s play, yet the vast majority has relied on others’ accounts to define, describe, and characterize their play. This study presents a successful effort to generate a scale to measure third through fifth grade children’s perceptions of their play. Items were generated from children’s unrestricted accounts of how they viewed their play, and the reliability (internal consistency, stability), content, and construct (convergent and discriminant) validity of the Children’s Perceptions of Their Play (CPTP) scale were all high. Across two independent samples, children defined their play in terms of six factors: opportunities to choose what to do and with whom (Child’s Choice), play interactions with friends (Social Play), structured recreation activities in which they chose to participate (Planned Activities), their involvement and absorption in play (Engagement), how physically active they are (Active Play), and freedom from school obligations when they were able to play as they desired (Free Time). No differences between boys and girls, the three grade levels, or children attending public versus private school were found.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":"2013 1","pages":"1-18"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2013-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2013/284741","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64400865","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 22
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
相关产品
×
本文献相关产品
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信