{"title":"Black archives, white philanthropists: Pan-African worldmaking in the interwar United States","authors":"Jake Hodder","doi":"10.1016/j.jhg.2025.03.003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhg.2025.03.003","url":null,"abstract":"This paper examines the development of key Pan-African collections in the United States during the interwar period, a time of heightened Pan-African consciousness among African Americans. The emergence of professionally trained Black curators and the growth of major US philanthropic organisations fostered a concerted effort to archive and document the global Black experience. These collections, housed at institutions such as the New York Public Library and historically Black colleges and universities (HBCUs), became significant sites of ‘worldmaking’, establishing Pan-Africanism as a distinct and transmittable field of study. In this context, Black activism intersected with white philanthropic ambition. While activists saw archives as tools for social change, philanthropists viewed them as sufficiently apolitical to meet their preference for ‘uplift’ rather than activism. The paper underscores the capacity of archives to hold multiple, conflicting interpretations and shows how a geographical approach can help avoid narrow, singular understandings of collections in the present.","PeriodicalId":47094,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Historical Geography","volume":"49 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143827721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"历史学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"How are far-right online communities using X/Twitter Spaces? Discourse, communication, sharing","authors":"Kurt Sengul","doi":"10.1016/j.dcm.2025.100884","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.dcm.2025.100884","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This paper presents the first scholarly study of the use of <em>X/Twitter Spaces</em> by far-right online communities. <em>Spaces</em> — <em>X/Twitter’s</em> live audio-based platform — has become an increasingly prominent tool in the far-right’s digital communication ecosystem in recent years. The popularity of <em>Spaces</em> with far-right users has increased in the wake of Elon Musk’s acquisition of <em>Twitter</em> in 2022 which signalled a marked rightward shift in the platform’s governance, particularly its techno-libertarian, ‘free speech absolutist’ approach to content moderation. Through a netnography and critical discourse analysis of (n = 41) <em>Spaces</em> sessions from January to July 2024, this paper critically examines how online far-right communities are using the voice-mediated affordance. In particular, this research explores the discursive practices and sharing strategies employed by individuals to propagate extreme and radical ideas, as well as to cultivate group membership, collective identity, and intersubjectivity. The findings demonstrate that <em>Spaces</em> are being used by a diverse range of far-right online communities and subcultures to promulgate conspiracy theories and radical and extreme ideological content. However, the findings also revealed a high degree of apolitical, non-ideological, and more everyday sharing practices. This paper broadens our empirical understanding of how <em>Spaces</em> are being instrumentalised by reactionary communities, and the role of voice-mediated affordances in the amplification, socialisation, recruitment, and radicalisation of far-right ideas globally.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":46649,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Context & Media","volume":"65 ","pages":"Article 100884"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143815174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
LinguaPub Date : 2025-04-11DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2025.103936
Haiquan Huang , Yixiong Chen , Xuewu Qin , Xin Wang , Jie Xu
{"title":"Mandarin-speaking children’s acquisition of the additive particle ye ‘also’","authors":"Haiquan Huang , Yixiong Chen , Xuewu Qin , Xin Wang , Jie Xu","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2025.103936","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lingua.2025.103936","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Previous studies have reported conflicting findings on children’s interpretation of additive particles. One of the previous studies has reported that Mandarin-speaking children up to 7 have difficulty deriving the presupposition triggered by the additive particle <em>ye</em> ‘also’. By contrast, another body of research has found that 3-to-4-year-olds can correctly interpret additive particles across languages. Such conflicting findings might have resulted from the different research techniques. Moreover, no study has been conducted to probe whether children are able to identify subject- versus object-associated focus in sentences with an additive particle. Against this backdrop, the current study attempts to systematically re-evaluate how Mandarin-speaking children interpret sentences with <em>ye</em> ‘also’, using an adapted research method. Two experiments were conducted with 3-to-4-year-olds and a group of adult controls. Experiment 1 probed whether children were able to identify object-associated focus in sentences with <em>ye</em>. Experiment 2 evaluated whether another group of children were able to identify subject-associated focus in sentences with <em>ye</em>. It was found that 3-year-olds already performed quite well and 4-year-olds nearly had adult-like performance in both experiments. The findings indicate that Mandarin-speaking preschoolers are able to identify subject- versus object-associated focus in sentences with <em>ye</em>, and such a linguistic ability develops in an incremental fashion. We discuss the methodological and theoretical implications of the findings.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"320 ","pages":"Article 103936"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143816545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Modern ItalyPub Date : 2025-04-11DOI: 10.1017/mit.2025.9
Eden K. McLean
{"title":"Bishop Geisler and the 1934 ‘Torello-Ricci Affair’: fighting for moral authority in a Fascist borderland","authors":"Eden K. McLean","doi":"10.1017/mit.2025.9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/mit.2025.9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the autumn of 1934, Bishop Johannes Geisler of Brixen/Bressanone denied two Italian-speaking priests, Carlo Torello and Giuseppe Ricci, permission to teach within his predominantly German-speaking diocese. In response, Benito Mussolini threatened to expel all Church representatives from the state education system and, by extension, to unravel the recently signed Lateran Accords. Untangling the motivations behind Geisler’s decision, the escalating tensions it precipitated, and, ultimately, the discussions that led to its quiet resolution reveal much about Fascist and Church ambitions in the newly annexed territory of Trentino-South Tyrol. This ‘Torello-Ricci Affair’ provides a micro-historical lens with which to better understand the political and cultural infrastructures of power in interwar South Tyrol and their relationship to institutions in Rome. In particular, it illustrates the ongoing battle between civil and religious officials to assert moral authority within the region, most importantly as it regarded the education of its children.</p>","PeriodicalId":18688,"journal":{"name":"Modern Italy","volume":"60 2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143819215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"历史学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Ana Milosavljevic, Thomas Castelain, Nausicaa Pouscoulous, Diana Mazzarella
{"title":"The Developmental Puzzle of Irony Understanding: Is Epistemic Vigilance the Missing Piece?","authors":"Ana Milosavljevic, Thomas Castelain, Nausicaa Pouscoulous, Diana Mazzarella","doi":"10.1017/s0305000925000091","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000925000091","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The prolonged developmental window of irony understanding opens up the question of which socio-cognitive repertoire underlies this pragmatic capacity. In the present study, we investigated the relationship between epistemic vigilance and irony understanding in 5/6- and 6/7-year-old children using a picture selection task. We assessed children’s vigilance towards unreliable informants and manipulated the reliability of the irony target. Our findings confirm that irony comprehension is a late-emerging skill and highlight the need to differentiate its full-fledged understanding from mere sensitivity to contextual mismatches. While irony understanding was not affected by our reliability manipulation, our findings revealed that more vigilant children were better at irony understanding than less vigilant ones. This provides the first empirical evidence that epistemic vigilance is a good predictor of irony performance and lays the ground for future research on the intricate relationship between these two capacities.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"25 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143819255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
SystemPub Date : 2025-04-11DOI: 10.1016/j.system.2025.103678
Jinlu Liu , Ming Wu , Haitao Liu
{"title":"Investigating the dynamic relationship of lexical and syntactic complexity in L2 writing across proficiency levels: A CDST-inspired study","authors":"Jinlu Liu , Ming Wu , Haitao Liu","doi":"10.1016/j.system.2025.103678","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.system.2025.103678","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study examines the development of lexical and syntactic complexity in L2 writing across five proficiency levels (ST2-ST6) of Chinese EFL learners. Analyzing 3423 essays from the Chinese Learner English Corpus (2003), we employed the Moving-Average Type-Token Ratio and Mean Dependency Distance to measure lexical and syntactic complexity, respectively. Results revealed significant, though non-linear, increase in both measures across proficiency levels. Syntactic complexity showed consistent growth, while lexical complexity plateaued at intermediate levels. A dynamic relationship analysis indicated periods of compensatory growth between lexical and syntactic complexity, particularly at lower proficiency levels. A combined complexity measure demonstrated an overall linear trend with significant non-linear components. These findings, interpreted through the lens of Complex Dynamic Systems Theory, suggest that L2 writing development involves intricate, sometimes competitive interactions between different levels of language proficiency. The study's outcomes have implications for language pedagogy, particularly in informing stage-appropriate instructional strategies and multi-dimensional assessment practices.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48185,"journal":{"name":"System","volume":"131 ","pages":"Article 103678"},"PeriodicalIF":4.9,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143817314","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Is interpreter advantage a gift or an effect of training? Cognitive changes and interpreting acquisition at the early stage of training","authors":"Xueni Zhang, Binghan Zheng, Rui Wang, Haoshen He","doi":"10.1017/s136672892500032x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s136672892500032x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Simultaneous interpreting (SI) is an intensive multitasking activity that requires coordination of a variety of linguistic and cognitive control mechanisms. Research has shown that interpreters perform better in tasks that require domain-general executive functions (EF), but the question remains whether such cognitive alternation is a result of interpreting experience or it reflects a selection bias that only cognitively capable people are recruited and trained to be interpreters. We examined the cognitive changes experienced by beginner-level students engaged in an intensive, two-week interpreting training programme. Our findings show that: (a) only cognitive flexibility was enhanced by training, together with improvement in SI performance; (b) the three EF subcomponents in their pre-existing forms negatively correlated with training gains; and (c) only pre-existing cognitive flexibility was positively associated with improvement in SI performance. Findings were discussed regarding the relationship between cognitive abilities and the early-stage acquisition of interpreting.</p>","PeriodicalId":8758,"journal":{"name":"Bilingualism: Language and Cognition","volume":"217 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143819205","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"What is proficiency? Characterizing spoken language proficiency in older Spanish-English bilinguals","authors":"Dalia L. Garcia, Tamar H. Gollan","doi":"10.1017/s1366728925000343","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728925000343","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We conducted a detailed linguistic analysis of Oral Proficiency Interviews (OPIs) from older Spanish-English bilinguals (<span>n</span> = 28) to determine which cognitive, linguistic, and demographic factors predict proficiency. In the dominant language, older age was associated with lower proficiency scores, but aging effects were not significant after accounting for cognitive functioning scores. In the nondominant language, bilinguals with larger vocabulary scores, fewer speech errors, and higher education levels obtained higher proficiency scores. Multiple linguistic submeasures from the OPIs were highly correlated across languages (e.g., fast speakers spoke fast in both languages), but these same measures exhibited significant language dominance effects (e.g., bilinguals spoke faster in the dominant than in the nondominant language). These results suggest it is critical to control for cognitive functioning when examining aging effects on language production, reveal powerful individual differences that affect how people talk regardless of language, and validate the use of the OPI to measure bilingual proficiency.</p>","PeriodicalId":8758,"journal":{"name":"Bilingualism: Language and Cognition","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2025-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143819206","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Bolaji Balogun, Sarah Demart, Claire Eldridge, Chandra Frank, Camilla Hawthorne, Stefanie Michels, Erin Kathleen Rowe, Kimberly St. Julian-Varnon
{"title":"European History Quarterly Roundtable: Histories of Race in Europe and Questions of Knowledge Production","authors":"Bolaji Balogun, Sarah Demart, Claire Eldridge, Chandra Frank, Camilla Hawthorne, Stefanie Michels, Erin Kathleen Rowe, Kimberly St. Julian-Varnon","doi":"10.1177/02656914251328511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/02656914251328511","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44713,"journal":{"name":"European History Quarterly","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2025-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143819407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"历史学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Language TeachingPub Date : 2025-04-10DOI: 10.1017/s0261444825000011
Elena García-Guerrero, Cristóbal Lozano
{"title":"Is planning time beneficial for anaphora resolution? A corpus-based study of L1 Spanish–L2 English learners","authors":"Elena García-Guerrero, Cristóbal Lozano","doi":"10.1017/s0261444825000011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0261444825000011","url":null,"abstract":"<span></span><p>Previous research has investigated the effect of planning time (PT) on L2 learners’ production regarding fluency, complexity, and accuracy, but its influence at the discourse level has been overlooked. Thus, this study explores the influence of PT on learners’ written performance regarding anaphora resolution (AR) and their pragmatically (in)felicitous choices of referring expressions (REs) in discourse since PT may reduce learners’ cognitive load and facilitate the production of pragmatically felicitous REs.</p><span></span><p>Two film-retelling tasks were completed by intermediate L1 Spanish–L2 English learners and English natives, further divided into a planning and a non-planning subgroup. Their compositions were analysed focusing on the REs produced, taking into consideration the pragmatic context. Results showed a PT effect on learners’ RE choices, although not all pragmatic contexts were equally affected. Planning time exerted a positive influence on topic continuity contexts, where learners produced more economical forms, but no effect was observed in topic shift scenarios.</p>","PeriodicalId":47770,"journal":{"name":"Language Teaching","volume":"30 4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2025-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143813985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}