将双语作为健康和受神经系统影响的说话者一生中的动态现象建模:双语语言学习的计算建模:当前模型与未来方向

IF 3.5 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH
Claudia Peñaloza, Uli Grasemann, Risto Miikkulainen, Swathi Kiran
{"title":"将双语作为健康和受神经系统影响的说话者一生中的动态现象建模:双语语言学习的计算建模:当前模型与未来方向","authors":"Claudia Peñaloza,&nbsp;Uli Grasemann,&nbsp;Risto Miikkulainen,&nbsp;Swathi Kiran","doi":"10.1111/lang.12566","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>In their review article, Li and Xu offered an insightful overview of the contributions and limitations of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to our current understanding of the bilingual mind. They further proposed joining cross-disciplinary efforts toward building a computational account that links cognitive theory and neurobiological accounts of bilingualism as part of their suggested future research agenda. We agree with Li and Xu's suggestions and further propose that (a) the scope of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing should be expanded to include other perspectives: language learning context, maintenance, and decay of linguistic competence and bilingual language breakdown and that (b) existing modeling efforts already work toward addressing these areas, answering the proposed desiderata for good computational models.</p><p>As reviewed by Li and Xu, developmental computational models have helped researchers understand how language representation emerges as a function of a speaker's bilingual experience. However, language learning context must be better accounted for. Specifically, bilingual language learning poses additional challenges for behavioral research when studies seek to address more naturalistic learning contexts such as second language (L2) acquisition via immersion in a foreign language context versus L2 acquisition in the classroom, and the involvement of implicit and explicit learning mechanisms constitutes an important axis of differentiation in this regard. Thus, while computational models include data-driven learning mechanisms to discover and organize linguistic representations as indicated by Li and Xu, future models should incorporate testable theory-driven implicit and explicit mechanisms for language learning (Peñaloza et al., <span>2022</span>). Existing computational models of bilingual lexical access (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>) could incorporate such mechanisms to help test the contributions of these mechanisms to bilingual language learning.</p><p>In addition, in modeling bilingual learning, both maintenance of the acquired linguistic knowledge and the reverse decay process are equally important in the lifespan timeline. For example, the extant literature makes it clear that contextual changes may reduce bilingual exposure and use that affect young and older bilinguals, yet bilingual processing decay in older adults can be further confounded with age-related language and cognitive decline (Goral et al., <span>2008</span>). In supporting Li and Xu's proposal for cross-disciplinary work, we argue that computational modeling could implement and test assumptions from closely related fields including memory theory (Mickan et al., <span>2019</span>) to gain understanding on how speakers’ language processing abilities in their first language (L1) and their L2 change with contextual experience and over the lifespan.</p><p>Cognitive control is a domain-general ability worth incorporating in an expanded scope for computational models of bilingualism. Although it is well known that different brain regions contribute to language control in bilinguals, their unique contributions to helping bilinguals overcome cross-language interference across different learning environments and language processing contexts with high versus low cognitive control demands remain an open question. We propose that the study of bilingual language breakdown following damage to critical brain regions offers a unique window into the modeling of lexical access and cognitive control. At the broadest level, lesion studies and patient behavioral data can inform computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to achieve the cognitive and neurobiological plausibility proposed by Li and Xu. At the most specific level, the computational simulation of control, learning, and processing mechanisms in bilingual patients with brain damage could help specify causal links between specific brain regions and bilingual behavior while controlling for relevant variables difficult to manipulate in behavioral research. For instance, using the BiLex computational model (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>), we demonstrated that applying damage to the semantic and L1 and L2 phonetic components of individual prestroke models reproduced L1 and L2 lexical retrieval deficits in bilingual aphasia patients (Grasemann et al., <span>2021</span>). In turn, applying semantic but not other lesion types could best reproduce the pattern of language decline in semantic dementia patients (Fidelman et al., <span>2022</span>). These simulation findings align with theories of lexical access deficits versus semantic storage deficits put forth for these patient groups, respectively (Mirman &amp; Britt, <span>2014</span>). Notably, BiLex could offer prognostic value in predicting rates of language decline in bilingual dementia and in identifying the language to target in treatment to achieve optimal recovery across the two languages in bilingual aphasia, an approach currently being tested in a clinical trial (Peñaloza et al., <span>2020</span>).</p><p>Models that expand the scope outlined above go a long way toward the desiderata for good computational models proposed by Li and Xu. One example is the BiLex model (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>): While based on the same principles of self-organizing maps and Hebbian learning reviewed by Li and Xu, it focuses on modeling individual proficiency, impairment, and treatment-induced recovery in stroke and dementia patients. The model is valid in that its initial training parameters map to actual L2 age of acquisition and language exposure of bilingual speakers, its performance is measured via clinical tests used with patients, and its rehabilitation training parameters map to actual treatment items, intensity, and duration. It makes good contact with real language in that the semantic representations encode the semantic dimensions identified through crowdsourcing (Mechanical Turk) experiments, and its phonetic representations encode the International Phonetic Alphabet principles of the languages modeled. BiLex is interpretable in that the maps and connectivity patterns explain the behavioral patterns, and specific damage leads to specific impairments; it is also predictive because it can be used to identify the treatment options leading to the best possible recovery.</p><p>In summary, we agree with Li and Xu in that an integrative computational neuroscience of bilingualism is needed, but its scope needs to be expanded to include language learning context, maintenance and decay of linguistic competence, and bilingual language breakdown to account for a larger range of phenomena in bilingual language learning and processing. Building upon work addressing the desiderata for good models proposed by Li and Xu, computational modeling can achieve cognitive and neurobiological plausibility by incorporating evidence from lesion studies and behavioral patient data to provide a vehicle for contrasting bilingualism theories and a bridge between computational science and real-world clinical needs.</p>","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5000,"publicationDate":"2023-03-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12566","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Modeling Bilingualism as a Dynamic Phenomenon in Healthy and Neurologically Affected Speakers Across the Lifespan: A Commentary on “Computational Modeling of Bilingual Language Learning: Current Models and Future Directions”\",\"authors\":\"Claudia Peñaloza,&nbsp;Uli Grasemann,&nbsp;Risto Miikkulainen,&nbsp;Swathi Kiran\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/lang.12566\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>In their review article, Li and Xu offered an insightful overview of the contributions and limitations of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to our current understanding of the bilingual mind. They further proposed joining cross-disciplinary efforts toward building a computational account that links cognitive theory and neurobiological accounts of bilingualism as part of their suggested future research agenda. We agree with Li and Xu's suggestions and further propose that (a) the scope of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing should be expanded to include other perspectives: language learning context, maintenance, and decay of linguistic competence and bilingual language breakdown and that (b) existing modeling efforts already work toward addressing these areas, answering the proposed desiderata for good computational models.</p><p>As reviewed by Li and Xu, developmental computational models have helped researchers understand how language representation emerges as a function of a speaker's bilingual experience. However, language learning context must be better accounted for. Specifically, bilingual language learning poses additional challenges for behavioral research when studies seek to address more naturalistic learning contexts such as second language (L2) acquisition via immersion in a foreign language context versus L2 acquisition in the classroom, and the involvement of implicit and explicit learning mechanisms constitutes an important axis of differentiation in this regard. Thus, while computational models include data-driven learning mechanisms to discover and organize linguistic representations as indicated by Li and Xu, future models should incorporate testable theory-driven implicit and explicit mechanisms for language learning (Peñaloza et al., <span>2022</span>). Existing computational models of bilingual lexical access (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>) could incorporate such mechanisms to help test the contributions of these mechanisms to bilingual language learning.</p><p>In addition, in modeling bilingual learning, both maintenance of the acquired linguistic knowledge and the reverse decay process are equally important in the lifespan timeline. For example, the extant literature makes it clear that contextual changes may reduce bilingual exposure and use that affect young and older bilinguals, yet bilingual processing decay in older adults can be further confounded with age-related language and cognitive decline (Goral et al., <span>2008</span>). In supporting Li and Xu's proposal for cross-disciplinary work, we argue that computational modeling could implement and test assumptions from closely related fields including memory theory (Mickan et al., <span>2019</span>) to gain understanding on how speakers’ language processing abilities in their first language (L1) and their L2 change with contextual experience and over the lifespan.</p><p>Cognitive control is a domain-general ability worth incorporating in an expanded scope for computational models of bilingualism. Although it is well known that different brain regions contribute to language control in bilinguals, their unique contributions to helping bilinguals overcome cross-language interference across different learning environments and language processing contexts with high versus low cognitive control demands remain an open question. We propose that the study of bilingual language breakdown following damage to critical brain regions offers a unique window into the modeling of lexical access and cognitive control. At the broadest level, lesion studies and patient behavioral data can inform computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to achieve the cognitive and neurobiological plausibility proposed by Li and Xu. At the most specific level, the computational simulation of control, learning, and processing mechanisms in bilingual patients with brain damage could help specify causal links between specific brain regions and bilingual behavior while controlling for relevant variables difficult to manipulate in behavioral research. For instance, using the BiLex computational model (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>), we demonstrated that applying damage to the semantic and L1 and L2 phonetic components of individual prestroke models reproduced L1 and L2 lexical retrieval deficits in bilingual aphasia patients (Grasemann et al., <span>2021</span>). In turn, applying semantic but not other lesion types could best reproduce the pattern of language decline in semantic dementia patients (Fidelman et al., <span>2022</span>). These simulation findings align with theories of lexical access deficits versus semantic storage deficits put forth for these patient groups, respectively (Mirman &amp; Britt, <span>2014</span>). Notably, BiLex could offer prognostic value in predicting rates of language decline in bilingual dementia and in identifying the language to target in treatment to achieve optimal recovery across the two languages in bilingual aphasia, an approach currently being tested in a clinical trial (Peñaloza et al., <span>2020</span>).</p><p>Models that expand the scope outlined above go a long way toward the desiderata for good computational models proposed by Li and Xu. One example is the BiLex model (Peñaloza et al., <span>2019</span>): While based on the same principles of self-organizing maps and Hebbian learning reviewed by Li and Xu, it focuses on modeling individual proficiency, impairment, and treatment-induced recovery in stroke and dementia patients. The model is valid in that its initial training parameters map to actual L2 age of acquisition and language exposure of bilingual speakers, its performance is measured via clinical tests used with patients, and its rehabilitation training parameters map to actual treatment items, intensity, and duration. It makes good contact with real language in that the semantic representations encode the semantic dimensions identified through crowdsourcing (Mechanical Turk) experiments, and its phonetic representations encode the International Phonetic Alphabet principles of the languages modeled. BiLex is interpretable in that the maps and connectivity patterns explain the behavioral patterns, and specific damage leads to specific impairments; it is also predictive because it can be used to identify the treatment options leading to the best possible recovery.</p><p>In summary, we agree with Li and Xu in that an integrative computational neuroscience of bilingualism is needed, but its scope needs to be expanded to include language learning context, maintenance and decay of linguistic competence, and bilingual language breakdown to account for a larger range of phenomena in bilingual language learning and processing. Building upon work addressing the desiderata for good models proposed by Li and Xu, computational modeling can achieve cognitive and neurobiological plausibility by incorporating evidence from lesion studies and behavioral patient data to provide a vehicle for contrasting bilingualism theories and a bridge between computational science and real-world clinical needs.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":51371,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Language Learning\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.5000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-03-16\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12566\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Language Learning\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"98\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lang.12566\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"文学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Language Learning","FirstCategoryId":"98","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lang.12566","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

摘要

值得注意的是,BiLex可以在预测双语痴呆症的语言下降率和确定治疗目标语言方面提供预后价值,以便在双语失语症中实现两种语言的最佳恢复,这一方法目前正在临床试验中进行测试(Peñaloza et al., 2020)。扩展上述范围的模型对Li和Xu提出的良好计算模型的需求有很长的路要走。一个例子是BiLex模型(Peñaloza等人,2019):虽然基于Li和Xu回顾的自组织地图和Hebbian学习的相同原理,但它侧重于对中风和痴呆患者的个人熟练程度、损伤和治疗诱导的恢复进行建模。该模型的初始训练参数映射到双语者的实际L2习得年龄和语言暴露,其性能通过患者临床测试来衡量,其康复训练参数映射到实际治疗项目、强度和持续时间是有效的。它的语义表示编码了通过众包(Mechanical Turk)实验识别的语义维度,其语音表示编码了建模语言的国际音标原则,与真实语言有很好的联系。BiLex是可解释的,因为地图和连接模式解释了行为模式,特定的损害导致特定的损害;它还具有预测性,因为它可以用来确定治疗方案,从而实现最佳的康复。总之,我们同意Li和Xu的观点,即需要一个综合的双语计算神经科学,但其范围需要扩大到包括语言学习背景,语言能力的维持和衰退,以及双语语言分解,以解释双语语言学习和加工中的更大范围现象。在Li和Xu提出的解决良好模型需求的工作基础上,计算建模可以通过结合病变研究和患者行为数据的证据来实现认知和神经生物学的可行性,从而为对比双语理论提供了一种工具,并在计算科学和现实世界的临床需求之间架起了一座桥梁。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Modeling Bilingualism as a Dynamic Phenomenon in Healthy and Neurologically Affected Speakers Across the Lifespan: A Commentary on “Computational Modeling of Bilingual Language Learning: Current Models and Future Directions”

In their review article, Li and Xu offered an insightful overview of the contributions and limitations of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to our current understanding of the bilingual mind. They further proposed joining cross-disciplinary efforts toward building a computational account that links cognitive theory and neurobiological accounts of bilingualism as part of their suggested future research agenda. We agree with Li and Xu's suggestions and further propose that (a) the scope of computational models of bilingual language learning and processing should be expanded to include other perspectives: language learning context, maintenance, and decay of linguistic competence and bilingual language breakdown and that (b) existing modeling efforts already work toward addressing these areas, answering the proposed desiderata for good computational models.

As reviewed by Li and Xu, developmental computational models have helped researchers understand how language representation emerges as a function of a speaker's bilingual experience. However, language learning context must be better accounted for. Specifically, bilingual language learning poses additional challenges for behavioral research when studies seek to address more naturalistic learning contexts such as second language (L2) acquisition via immersion in a foreign language context versus L2 acquisition in the classroom, and the involvement of implicit and explicit learning mechanisms constitutes an important axis of differentiation in this regard. Thus, while computational models include data-driven learning mechanisms to discover and organize linguistic representations as indicated by Li and Xu, future models should incorporate testable theory-driven implicit and explicit mechanisms for language learning (Peñaloza et al., 2022). Existing computational models of bilingual lexical access (Peñaloza et al., 2019) could incorporate such mechanisms to help test the contributions of these mechanisms to bilingual language learning.

In addition, in modeling bilingual learning, both maintenance of the acquired linguistic knowledge and the reverse decay process are equally important in the lifespan timeline. For example, the extant literature makes it clear that contextual changes may reduce bilingual exposure and use that affect young and older bilinguals, yet bilingual processing decay in older adults can be further confounded with age-related language and cognitive decline (Goral et al., 2008). In supporting Li and Xu's proposal for cross-disciplinary work, we argue that computational modeling could implement and test assumptions from closely related fields including memory theory (Mickan et al., 2019) to gain understanding on how speakers’ language processing abilities in their first language (L1) and their L2 change with contextual experience and over the lifespan.

Cognitive control is a domain-general ability worth incorporating in an expanded scope for computational models of bilingualism. Although it is well known that different brain regions contribute to language control in bilinguals, their unique contributions to helping bilinguals overcome cross-language interference across different learning environments and language processing contexts with high versus low cognitive control demands remain an open question. We propose that the study of bilingual language breakdown following damage to critical brain regions offers a unique window into the modeling of lexical access and cognitive control. At the broadest level, lesion studies and patient behavioral data can inform computational models of bilingual language learning and processing to achieve the cognitive and neurobiological plausibility proposed by Li and Xu. At the most specific level, the computational simulation of control, learning, and processing mechanisms in bilingual patients with brain damage could help specify causal links between specific brain regions and bilingual behavior while controlling for relevant variables difficult to manipulate in behavioral research. For instance, using the BiLex computational model (Peñaloza et al., 2019), we demonstrated that applying damage to the semantic and L1 and L2 phonetic components of individual prestroke models reproduced L1 and L2 lexical retrieval deficits in bilingual aphasia patients (Grasemann et al., 2021). In turn, applying semantic but not other lesion types could best reproduce the pattern of language decline in semantic dementia patients (Fidelman et al., 2022). These simulation findings align with theories of lexical access deficits versus semantic storage deficits put forth for these patient groups, respectively (Mirman & Britt, 2014). Notably, BiLex could offer prognostic value in predicting rates of language decline in bilingual dementia and in identifying the language to target in treatment to achieve optimal recovery across the two languages in bilingual aphasia, an approach currently being tested in a clinical trial (Peñaloza et al., 2020).

Models that expand the scope outlined above go a long way toward the desiderata for good computational models proposed by Li and Xu. One example is the BiLex model (Peñaloza et al., 2019): While based on the same principles of self-organizing maps and Hebbian learning reviewed by Li and Xu, it focuses on modeling individual proficiency, impairment, and treatment-induced recovery in stroke and dementia patients. The model is valid in that its initial training parameters map to actual L2 age of acquisition and language exposure of bilingual speakers, its performance is measured via clinical tests used with patients, and its rehabilitation training parameters map to actual treatment items, intensity, and duration. It makes good contact with real language in that the semantic representations encode the semantic dimensions identified through crowdsourcing (Mechanical Turk) experiments, and its phonetic representations encode the International Phonetic Alphabet principles of the languages modeled. BiLex is interpretable in that the maps and connectivity patterns explain the behavioral patterns, and specific damage leads to specific impairments; it is also predictive because it can be used to identify the treatment options leading to the best possible recovery.

In summary, we agree with Li and Xu in that an integrative computational neuroscience of bilingualism is needed, but its scope needs to be expanded to include language learning context, maintenance and decay of linguistic competence, and bilingual language breakdown to account for a larger range of phenomena in bilingual language learning and processing. Building upon work addressing the desiderata for good models proposed by Li and Xu, computational modeling can achieve cognitive and neurobiological plausibility by incorporating evidence from lesion studies and behavioral patient data to provide a vehicle for contrasting bilingualism theories and a bridge between computational science and real-world clinical needs.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Language Learning
Language Learning Multiple-
CiteScore
9.10
自引率
15.90%
发文量
65
期刊介绍: Language Learning is a scientific journal dedicated to the understanding of language learning broadly defined. It publishes research articles that systematically apply methods of inquiry from disciplines including psychology, linguistics, cognitive science, educational inquiry, neuroscience, ethnography, sociolinguistics, sociology, and anthropology. It is concerned with fundamental theoretical issues in language learning such as child, second, and foreign language acquisition, language education, bilingualism, literacy, language representation in mind and brain, culture, cognition, pragmatics, and intergroup relations. A subscription includes one or two annual supplements, alternating among a volume from the Language Learning Cognitive Neuroscience Series, the Currents in Language Learning Series or the Language Learning Special Issue Series.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信