教育研究中因果关系的方法、理解和表达

IF 2.3 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH
K. Morrison, G. P. van der Werf
{"title":"教育研究中因果关系的方法、理解和表达","authors":"K. Morrison, G. P. van der Werf","doi":"10.1080/13803611.2021.1991643","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The opening pages of Pearl and Mackenzie’s volume The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect (2018) herald their captivating romp through causality by referring to a “ladder of causation” (p. 28) that starts from association (by seeing and observing), moves up to intervention (by doing and intervening), and thence to counterfactuals (by imagining, retrospection, and understanding). Each rung of the ladder establishes causality more certainly. Humans think causally. Causality can be studied by many methods. Here, Pearl and Mackenzie (2018) state that statistical analysis does not simply concern data and their methods of analysis; rather, there is a need for an “understanding of the process that produces the data” (p. 85). Such “understanding” comes from introducing causality, as causality yields something additional to the original data. “Methods” of data analysis are informed by an “understanding” of causality, as this Editorial shows. Pearl and Mackenzie write that if we remove the understanding of causation from statistical analysis, all that we are left with is data reduction, which does not tell us much. The papers in this issue move forward from “methods” to “understanding” data with regard to causality. Further, the Editorial indicates how easily it is to find expressions of causality in articles; this should caution researchers to take care in the wording that they use. The Editorial below draws attention to wording in deliberately italicising causal words in quoting from the articles in this issue. For example, is causality really being demonstrated, or, like Pearl and MacKenzie’s lowest rung of the ladder, is there merely association? Causality, be it post hoc or ante hoc, is self-evidently important in education. However, how we adduce causality is far from straightforward, and the papers in this issue yield insights into, and cautions concerning, claims for, and demonstrations of, causality. The papers here indicate methods, challenges, outcomes, and benefits of studying causality. The challenges of “methods” and “understanding” when investigating causality are legion. Witness, for example, in the perennial search for causality, its differences from association, prediction, explanation, inference, influence, correlation, accounting for, correspondence to, purposiveness, and a whole armoury of other words. Look at the dangers of working with mediating, confounding, and moderating variables, transitivity, or controlling out almost everything such that what remains is very little. Wrestle with underdetermination, overdetermination, supervenience, and the difficulties of mereology. Consider the challenges of probabilistic causation and Bayesian approaches, leavened by multilevel causal modelling. Add to these the context-dense, variable-rich, causally complex world of education, and the attraction of Pearl and MacKenzie’s (2018) “childlike simplicity” (p. 39) of a causal diagram evaporates in front of our eyes. Little wonder it is, then, that authors studying causality in the field of education choose their words very carefully. This helps them to avoid charges of assumed or","PeriodicalId":47025,"journal":{"name":"Educational Research and Evaluation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3000,"publicationDate":"2020-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Methods, understandings, and expressions of causality in educational research\",\"authors\":\"K. Morrison, G. P. van der Werf\",\"doi\":\"10.1080/13803611.2021.1991643\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"The opening pages of Pearl and Mackenzie’s volume The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect (2018) herald their captivating romp through causality by referring to a “ladder of causation” (p. 28) that starts from association (by seeing and observing), moves up to intervention (by doing and intervening), and thence to counterfactuals (by imagining, retrospection, and understanding). Each rung of the ladder establishes causality more certainly. Humans think causally. Causality can be studied by many methods. Here, Pearl and Mackenzie (2018) state that statistical analysis does not simply concern data and their methods of analysis; rather, there is a need for an “understanding of the process that produces the data” (p. 85). Such “understanding” comes from introducing causality, as causality yields something additional to the original data. “Methods” of data analysis are informed by an “understanding” of causality, as this Editorial shows. Pearl and Mackenzie write that if we remove the understanding of causation from statistical analysis, all that we are left with is data reduction, which does not tell us much. The papers in this issue move forward from “methods” to “understanding” data with regard to causality. Further, the Editorial indicates how easily it is to find expressions of causality in articles; this should caution researchers to take care in the wording that they use. The Editorial below draws attention to wording in deliberately italicising causal words in quoting from the articles in this issue. For example, is causality really being demonstrated, or, like Pearl and MacKenzie’s lowest rung of the ladder, is there merely association? Causality, be it post hoc or ante hoc, is self-evidently important in education. However, how we adduce causality is far from straightforward, and the papers in this issue yield insights into, and cautions concerning, claims for, and demonstrations of, causality. The papers here indicate methods, challenges, outcomes, and benefits of studying causality. The challenges of “methods” and “understanding” when investigating causality are legion. Witness, for example, in the perennial search for causality, its differences from association, prediction, explanation, inference, influence, correlation, accounting for, correspondence to, purposiveness, and a whole armoury of other words. Look at the dangers of working with mediating, confounding, and moderating variables, transitivity, or controlling out almost everything such that what remains is very little. Wrestle with underdetermination, overdetermination, supervenience, and the difficulties of mereology. Consider the challenges of probabilistic causation and Bayesian approaches, leavened by multilevel causal modelling. Add to these the context-dense, variable-rich, causally complex world of education, and the attraction of Pearl and MacKenzie’s (2018) “childlike simplicity” (p. 39) of a causal diagram evaporates in front of our eyes. Little wonder it is, then, that authors studying causality in the field of education choose their words very carefully. This helps them to avoid charges of assumed or\",\"PeriodicalId\":47025,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Educational Research and Evaluation\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.3000,\"publicationDate\":\"2020-11-16\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Educational Research and Evaluation\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1080/13803611.2021.1991643\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Educational Research and Evaluation","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13803611.2021.1991643","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

珀尔和麦肯齐的《为什么之书:因果关系的新科学》(2018)一书的开头几页通过提到“因果关系的阶梯”(第28页),预示着他们在因果关系中的迷人嬉戏,“因果关系阶梯”从联想(通过看和观察)开始,向上移动到干预(通过做和干预),然后是反事实(通过想象、回顾和理解)。阶梯的每一级都更确定地建立了因果关系。人类的思考是因果的。因果关系可以通过多种方法来研究。在这里,Pearl和Mackenzie(2018)指出,统计分析不仅仅涉及数据及其分析方法;相反,需要“理解产生数据的过程”(第85页)。这种“理解”来自于引入因果关系,因为因果关系在原始数据的基础上产生了额外的东西。正如这篇社论所示,数据分析的“方法”是由对因果关系的“理解”决定的。珀尔和麦肯齐写道,如果我们从统计分析中去除对因果关系的理解,我们只剩下数据减少,这并不能告诉我们什么。本期的论文从“方法”转向了“理解”关于因果关系的数据。此外,社论指出,在文章中找到因果关系的表达是多么容易;这应该提醒研究人员注意他们使用的措辞。下面的社论提请注意在引用本期文章时故意用斜体表示因果词的措辞。例如,因果关系真的得到了证明吗?或者,就像珀尔和麦肯齐的最底层一样,仅仅存在关联吗?因果关系,无论是事后的还是事前的,在教育中都是不言而喻的重要。然而,我们如何援引因果关系远非易事,本期的论文对因果关系的主张和证明提出了见解和警告。本文指出了研究因果关系的方法、挑战、结果和好处。在调查因果关系时,“方法”和“理解”的挑战是巨大的。例如,在长期寻找因果关系的过程中,见证了因果关系与联想、预测、解释、推理、影响、相关性、解释、对应性、目的性以及其他词汇的差异。看看使用中介、混淆和调节变量、传递性或控制几乎所有东西的危险,这样剩下的就很少了。与不确定、过度确定、偶然性和语言学的困难作斗争。考虑概率因果关系和贝叶斯方法的挑战,这些方法受到多层次因果模型的影响。再加上上下文密集、变量丰富、因果关系复杂的教育世界,珀尔和麦肯齐(2018)的因果图“童真”(第39页)的吸引力在我们眼前消失了。因此,难怪在教育领域研究因果关系的作者措辞非常谨慎。这有助于他们避免承担或
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Methods, understandings, and expressions of causality in educational research
The opening pages of Pearl and Mackenzie’s volume The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect (2018) herald their captivating romp through causality by referring to a “ladder of causation” (p. 28) that starts from association (by seeing and observing), moves up to intervention (by doing and intervening), and thence to counterfactuals (by imagining, retrospection, and understanding). Each rung of the ladder establishes causality more certainly. Humans think causally. Causality can be studied by many methods. Here, Pearl and Mackenzie (2018) state that statistical analysis does not simply concern data and their methods of analysis; rather, there is a need for an “understanding of the process that produces the data” (p. 85). Such “understanding” comes from introducing causality, as causality yields something additional to the original data. “Methods” of data analysis are informed by an “understanding” of causality, as this Editorial shows. Pearl and Mackenzie write that if we remove the understanding of causation from statistical analysis, all that we are left with is data reduction, which does not tell us much. The papers in this issue move forward from “methods” to “understanding” data with regard to causality. Further, the Editorial indicates how easily it is to find expressions of causality in articles; this should caution researchers to take care in the wording that they use. The Editorial below draws attention to wording in deliberately italicising causal words in quoting from the articles in this issue. For example, is causality really being demonstrated, or, like Pearl and MacKenzie’s lowest rung of the ladder, is there merely association? Causality, be it post hoc or ante hoc, is self-evidently important in education. However, how we adduce causality is far from straightforward, and the papers in this issue yield insights into, and cautions concerning, claims for, and demonstrations of, causality. The papers here indicate methods, challenges, outcomes, and benefits of studying causality. The challenges of “methods” and “understanding” when investigating causality are legion. Witness, for example, in the perennial search for causality, its differences from association, prediction, explanation, inference, influence, correlation, accounting for, correspondence to, purposiveness, and a whole armoury of other words. Look at the dangers of working with mediating, confounding, and moderating variables, transitivity, or controlling out almost everything such that what remains is very little. Wrestle with underdetermination, overdetermination, supervenience, and the difficulties of mereology. Consider the challenges of probabilistic causation and Bayesian approaches, leavened by multilevel causal modelling. Add to these the context-dense, variable-rich, causally complex world of education, and the attraction of Pearl and MacKenzie’s (2018) “childlike simplicity” (p. 39) of a causal diagram evaporates in front of our eyes. Little wonder it is, then, that authors studying causality in the field of education choose their words very carefully. This helps them to avoid charges of assumed or
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Educational Research and Evaluation
Educational Research and Evaluation EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH-
CiteScore
3.00
自引率
0.00%
发文量
25
期刊介绍: International, comparative and multidisciplinary in scope, Educational Research and Evaluation (ERE) publishes original, peer-reviewed academic articles dealing with research on issues of worldwide relevance in educational practice. The aim of the journal is to increase understanding of learning in pre-primary, primary, high school, college, university and adult education, and to contribute to the improvement of educational processes and outcomes. The journal seeks to promote cross-national and international comparative educational research by publishing findings relevant to the scholarly community, as well as to practitioners and others interested in education. The scope of the journal is deliberately broad in terms of both topics covered and disciplinary perspective.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信