世界大多数国家婴幼儿发展的定性研究

IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL
Catherine E. Draper
{"title":"世界大多数国家婴幼儿发展的定性研究","authors":"Catherine E. Draper","doi":"10.1002/icd.70039","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Publication bias has become increasingly acknowledged in the field of child development (Draper et al. <span>2022</span>). The lack of diversity in child development research has been highlighted (Moriguchi <span>2022</span>; Nielsen et al. <span>2017</span>), including for specific fields such as cognitive development research (Alves et al. <span>2022</span>; Miller-Cotto et al. <span>2022</span>; Rowley and Camacho <span>2015</span>), caregiving (Benito-Gomez et al. <span>2020</span>; Raval and Walker <span>2019</span>), physical punishment and child development (Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor <span>2016</span>; Heilmann et al. <span>2021</span>) and early childhood development interventions (Draper et al. <span>2023</span>; Ferreira et al. <span>2020</span>). Furthermore, recent reviews have noted biases in infant development research (Singh et al. <span>2023</span>).</p><p>While criticisms initially highlighted the narrow focus of psychology on American (Arnett <span>2008</span>) and ‘WEIRD’ (Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich and Democratic) populations, drawing attention to the lack of representation in our understanding of human psychology and behaviour (Henrich et al. <span>2010a</span>, <span>2010b</span>). More recently, the terms ‘Majority’ and ‘Minority World’ (Alam <span>2019</span>) are used rather than terms such as WEIRD, Global South, developing or low- and middle-income countries to not bolster notions of superiority and false hierarchies (Khan et al. <span>2022</span>). Instead, Majority World refers collectively to the countries that make up the majority of the world's population, while Western Countries actually comprise the minority (Khan et al. <span>2022</span>).</p><p>Apart from the publication bias already mentioned, qualitative research in child development is scarce, relative to quantitative research. There are two main factors that arguably contribute to this. First, this relative scarcity may be due to long-held beliefs by some child development researchers from quantitative research backgrounds that qualitative research does not measure up to the same rigorous standards as quantitative research—a view not unique to child development research. Frameworks for evaluating the rigour of qualitative research can run the risk of applying a quantitative paradigm, and reviewers more entrenched in this quantitative paradigm can ask (and sometimes insist) that qualitative researchers apply quantitative concepts such as inter-rater reliability and generalizability, and consider small sample sizes a limitation. This can show a lack of insight into the underpinning tenets of qualitative research, thereby failing to properly recognise its unique contributions.</p><p>Second, many journals in the field of child development do not easily accommodate qualitative research articles. These journals have word limits that are unrealistic for qualitative research articles and would require authors to severely compromise on their ability to highlight the nuances and richness of qualitative data and their interpretation, thereby downplaying the significance of this type of research. The potential impact of this is that qualitative research in child development gets submitted to journals in adjacent fields, such as public and global health. This means that the target audience of child development researchers tend not to see this work published in the journals they follow and lose out on the opportunity to see its importance for the field. <i>Infant and Child Development</i> is one of the few journals in the field that makes accommodation in their word limits for qualitative research (unrestricted for methods and results sections), and more journals should follow their example.</p><p>Apart from addressing these factors, it needs to be emphasised that qualitative research can make a valuable contribution to addressing the need for greater contextual awareness, especially across diverse global settings (Draper et al. <span>2022</span>). Quantitative data is essential to characterise various contextual factors, such as socioeconomic status, household composition, presence or absence of resources in the home and community and the quality of learning environments. Qualitative data can be highly complementary to these data, by investigating in more detail and depth how these factors interact and intersect, particularly when they do not necessarily operate in the same way across diverse global settings. Furthermore, qualitative research can explore the complexities of the perceptions, lived experiences, attitudes, beliefs, behaviours and motivations of children and adolescents, as well as actors within the home, school and community environments.</p><p>From an implementation science perspective, qualitative research can play an integral role in community participatory methods for the co-creation of child development interventions, especially to ensure the contextual relevance of such interventions. Qualitative research is extremely useful for answering questions relating to implementation, such as feasibility and acceptability of interventions, and developing a better understanding of <i>how</i> child development interventions work, and why they work better for some people or settings than others. All these insights can feed into meaningful strategies for the translation of research findings into policy and practice, and for guiding future research.</p><p>This special issue therefore aims to address not only the bias against publications from Majority World countries, but also the dearth of published qualitative research from these countries in the field of child development. While publishing special issues featuring this type of research is not the full solution to address these issues, it is an important step in the right direction. By platforming qualitative research in child development from around the world, and by highlighting the challenges faced in publishing this research, we hope to encourage a greater openness to publishing this work, and to acknowledging its value for the field of child development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"34 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8000,"publicationDate":"2025-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.70039","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Qualitative Research on Infant and Child Development in Majority World Countries\",\"authors\":\"Catherine E. Draper\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/icd.70039\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Publication bias has become increasingly acknowledged in the field of child development (Draper et al. <span>2022</span>). The lack of diversity in child development research has been highlighted (Moriguchi <span>2022</span>; Nielsen et al. <span>2017</span>), including for specific fields such as cognitive development research (Alves et al. <span>2022</span>; Miller-Cotto et al. <span>2022</span>; Rowley and Camacho <span>2015</span>), caregiving (Benito-Gomez et al. <span>2020</span>; Raval and Walker <span>2019</span>), physical punishment and child development (Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor <span>2016</span>; Heilmann et al. <span>2021</span>) and early childhood development interventions (Draper et al. <span>2023</span>; Ferreira et al. <span>2020</span>). Furthermore, recent reviews have noted biases in infant development research (Singh et al. <span>2023</span>).</p><p>While criticisms initially highlighted the narrow focus of psychology on American (Arnett <span>2008</span>) and ‘WEIRD’ (Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich and Democratic) populations, drawing attention to the lack of representation in our understanding of human psychology and behaviour (Henrich et al. <span>2010a</span>, <span>2010b</span>). More recently, the terms ‘Majority’ and ‘Minority World’ (Alam <span>2019</span>) are used rather than terms such as WEIRD, Global South, developing or low- and middle-income countries to not bolster notions of superiority and false hierarchies (Khan et al. <span>2022</span>). Instead, Majority World refers collectively to the countries that make up the majority of the world's population, while Western Countries actually comprise the minority (Khan et al. <span>2022</span>).</p><p>Apart from the publication bias already mentioned, qualitative research in child development is scarce, relative to quantitative research. There are two main factors that arguably contribute to this. First, this relative scarcity may be due to long-held beliefs by some child development researchers from quantitative research backgrounds that qualitative research does not measure up to the same rigorous standards as quantitative research—a view not unique to child development research. Frameworks for evaluating the rigour of qualitative research can run the risk of applying a quantitative paradigm, and reviewers more entrenched in this quantitative paradigm can ask (and sometimes insist) that qualitative researchers apply quantitative concepts such as inter-rater reliability and generalizability, and consider small sample sizes a limitation. This can show a lack of insight into the underpinning tenets of qualitative research, thereby failing to properly recognise its unique contributions.</p><p>Second, many journals in the field of child development do not easily accommodate qualitative research articles. These journals have word limits that are unrealistic for qualitative research articles and would require authors to severely compromise on their ability to highlight the nuances and richness of qualitative data and their interpretation, thereby downplaying the significance of this type of research. The potential impact of this is that qualitative research in child development gets submitted to journals in adjacent fields, such as public and global health. This means that the target audience of child development researchers tend not to see this work published in the journals they follow and lose out on the opportunity to see its importance for the field. <i>Infant and Child Development</i> is one of the few journals in the field that makes accommodation in their word limits for qualitative research (unrestricted for methods and results sections), and more journals should follow their example.</p><p>Apart from addressing these factors, it needs to be emphasised that qualitative research can make a valuable contribution to addressing the need for greater contextual awareness, especially across diverse global settings (Draper et al. <span>2022</span>). Quantitative data is essential to characterise various contextual factors, such as socioeconomic status, household composition, presence or absence of resources in the home and community and the quality of learning environments. Qualitative data can be highly complementary to these data, by investigating in more detail and depth how these factors interact and intersect, particularly when they do not necessarily operate in the same way across diverse global settings. Furthermore, qualitative research can explore the complexities of the perceptions, lived experiences, attitudes, beliefs, behaviours and motivations of children and adolescents, as well as actors within the home, school and community environments.</p><p>From an implementation science perspective, qualitative research can play an integral role in community participatory methods for the co-creation of child development interventions, especially to ensure the contextual relevance of such interventions. Qualitative research is extremely useful for answering questions relating to implementation, such as feasibility and acceptability of interventions, and developing a better understanding of <i>how</i> child development interventions work, and why they work better for some people or settings than others. All these insights can feed into meaningful strategies for the translation of research findings into policy and practice, and for guiding future research.</p><p>This special issue therefore aims to address not only the bias against publications from Majority World countries, but also the dearth of published qualitative research from these countries in the field of child development. While publishing special issues featuring this type of research is not the full solution to address these issues, it is an important step in the right direction. By platforming qualitative research in child development from around the world, and by highlighting the challenges faced in publishing this research, we hope to encourage a greater openness to publishing this work, and to acknowledging its value for the field of child development.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":47820,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Infant and Child Development\",\"volume\":\"34 4\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-07-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.70039\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Infant and Child Development\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/icd.70039\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Infant and Child Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/icd.70039","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

在儿童发展领域,发表偏倚已得到越来越多的承认(Draper et al. 2022)。儿童发展研究缺乏多样性已经得到强调(Moriguchi 2022;Nielsen et al. 2017),包括认知发展研究等特定领域(Alves et al. 2022;Miller-Cotto et al. 2022;Rowley and Camacho 2015),看护(Benito-Gomez et al. 2020;拉瓦尔和沃克2019),体罚和儿童发展(Gershoff和Grogan-Kaylor 2016;Heilmann et al. 2021)和儿童早期发展干预(Draper et al. 2023;Ferreira et al. 2020)。此外,最近的评论指出了婴儿发育研究中的偏见(Singh et al. 2023)。虽然最初的批评强调了心理学对美国人(Arnett 2008)和“WEIRD”(西方,受过教育,工业,富裕和民主)人群的狭隘关注,但人们注意到我们对人类心理和行为的理解缺乏代表性(Henrich et al. 2010a, 2010b)。最近,术语“多数”和“少数世界”(Alam 2019)被使用,而不是像WEIRD、全球南方、发展中国家或低收入和中等收入国家这样的术语,以避免强化优越感和错误的等级观念(Khan et al. 2022)。相反,多数世界是指占世界人口大多数的国家,而西方国家实际上只占少数(Khan et al. 2022)。除了前面提到的发表偏倚,相对于定量研究而言,儿童发展方面的定性研究很少。有两个主要因素可以说是造成这种情况的原因。首先,这种相对稀缺可能是由于一些来自定量研究背景的儿童发展研究人员长期持有的信念,即定性研究无法达到与定量研究相同的严格标准——这并非儿童发展研究独有的观点。评估定性研究严谨性的框架可能会有应用定量范式的风险,而更根深蒂固于这种定量范式的审稿人可能会要求(有时会坚持)定性研究人员应用定量概念,如评分者之间的可靠性和普遍性,并考虑小样本量是一个限制。这可能表明对定性研究的基本原则缺乏洞察力,从而未能正确认识到其独特的贡献。其次,儿童发展领域的许多期刊不容易容纳定性研究文章。这些期刊的字数限制对于定性研究文章来说是不现实的,并且要求作者在强调定性数据的细微差别和丰富性及其解释的能力上严重妥协,从而淡化了这类研究的重要性。这样做的潜在影响是,儿童发展方面的定性研究可以提交给相关领域的期刊,比如公共卫生和全球卫生。这意味着儿童发展研究人员的目标受众往往不会在他们关注的期刊上看到这项工作,从而失去了看到它对该领域重要性的机会。《婴幼儿发展》是该领域为数不多的对定性研究(方法和结果部分不受限制)的字数限制的期刊之一,更多的期刊应该效仿他们的做法。除了解决这些因素外,需要强调的是,定性研究可以为解决更大的上下文意识需求做出宝贵贡献,特别是在不同的全球环境中(Draper et al. 2022)。定量数据对于描述各种背景因素至关重要,例如社会经济地位、家庭组成、家庭和社区资源的有无以及学习环境的质量。通过更详细和深入地调查这些因素如何相互作用和交叉,特别是当它们在不同的全球环境中不一定以相同的方式运作时,定性数据可以对这些数据进行高度补充。此外,定性研究可以探索儿童和青少年以及家庭、学校和社区环境中的行为者的认知、生活经历、态度、信仰、行为和动机的复杂性。从实施科学的角度来看,定性研究可以在社区参与方法中发挥不可或缺的作用,共同创造儿童发展干预措施,特别是确保这些干预措施的背景相关性。定性研究对于回答与实施有关的问题非常有用,例如干预措施的可行性和可接受性,以及更好地了解儿童发展干预措施如何起作用,以及为什么它们对某些人或环境比其他环境更有效。 所有这些见解都可以形成有意义的策略,将研究成果转化为政策和实践,并指导未来的研究。因此,本期特刊的目的不仅在于解决对多数世界国家出版物的偏见,而且在于解决这些国家在儿童发展领域发表的定性研究的缺乏问题。虽然出版以这类研究为特色的特刊并不能完全解决这些问题,但这是朝着正确方向迈出的重要一步。通过将来自世界各地的儿童发展定性研究作为平台,并强调发表这项研究所面临的挑战,我们希望鼓励更大的开放性来发表这项工作,并承认其对儿童发展领域的价值。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Qualitative Research on Infant and Child Development in Majority World Countries

Publication bias has become increasingly acknowledged in the field of child development (Draper et al. 2022). The lack of diversity in child development research has been highlighted (Moriguchi 2022; Nielsen et al. 2017), including for specific fields such as cognitive development research (Alves et al. 2022; Miller-Cotto et al. 2022; Rowley and Camacho 2015), caregiving (Benito-Gomez et al. 2020; Raval and Walker 2019), physical punishment and child development (Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor 2016; Heilmann et al. 2021) and early childhood development interventions (Draper et al. 2023; Ferreira et al. 2020). Furthermore, recent reviews have noted biases in infant development research (Singh et al. 2023).

While criticisms initially highlighted the narrow focus of psychology on American (Arnett 2008) and ‘WEIRD’ (Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich and Democratic) populations, drawing attention to the lack of representation in our understanding of human psychology and behaviour (Henrich et al. 2010a, 2010b). More recently, the terms ‘Majority’ and ‘Minority World’ (Alam 2019) are used rather than terms such as WEIRD, Global South, developing or low- and middle-income countries to not bolster notions of superiority and false hierarchies (Khan et al. 2022). Instead, Majority World refers collectively to the countries that make up the majority of the world's population, while Western Countries actually comprise the minority (Khan et al. 2022).

Apart from the publication bias already mentioned, qualitative research in child development is scarce, relative to quantitative research. There are two main factors that arguably contribute to this. First, this relative scarcity may be due to long-held beliefs by some child development researchers from quantitative research backgrounds that qualitative research does not measure up to the same rigorous standards as quantitative research—a view not unique to child development research. Frameworks for evaluating the rigour of qualitative research can run the risk of applying a quantitative paradigm, and reviewers more entrenched in this quantitative paradigm can ask (and sometimes insist) that qualitative researchers apply quantitative concepts such as inter-rater reliability and generalizability, and consider small sample sizes a limitation. This can show a lack of insight into the underpinning tenets of qualitative research, thereby failing to properly recognise its unique contributions.

Second, many journals in the field of child development do not easily accommodate qualitative research articles. These journals have word limits that are unrealistic for qualitative research articles and would require authors to severely compromise on their ability to highlight the nuances and richness of qualitative data and their interpretation, thereby downplaying the significance of this type of research. The potential impact of this is that qualitative research in child development gets submitted to journals in adjacent fields, such as public and global health. This means that the target audience of child development researchers tend not to see this work published in the journals they follow and lose out on the opportunity to see its importance for the field. Infant and Child Development is one of the few journals in the field that makes accommodation in their word limits for qualitative research (unrestricted for methods and results sections), and more journals should follow their example.

Apart from addressing these factors, it needs to be emphasised that qualitative research can make a valuable contribution to addressing the need for greater contextual awareness, especially across diverse global settings (Draper et al. 2022). Quantitative data is essential to characterise various contextual factors, such as socioeconomic status, household composition, presence or absence of resources in the home and community and the quality of learning environments. Qualitative data can be highly complementary to these data, by investigating in more detail and depth how these factors interact and intersect, particularly when they do not necessarily operate in the same way across diverse global settings. Furthermore, qualitative research can explore the complexities of the perceptions, lived experiences, attitudes, beliefs, behaviours and motivations of children and adolescents, as well as actors within the home, school and community environments.

From an implementation science perspective, qualitative research can play an integral role in community participatory methods for the co-creation of child development interventions, especially to ensure the contextual relevance of such interventions. Qualitative research is extremely useful for answering questions relating to implementation, such as feasibility and acceptability of interventions, and developing a better understanding of how child development interventions work, and why they work better for some people or settings than others. All these insights can feed into meaningful strategies for the translation of research findings into policy and practice, and for guiding future research.

This special issue therefore aims to address not only the bias against publications from Majority World countries, but also the dearth of published qualitative research from these countries in the field of child development. While publishing special issues featuring this type of research is not the full solution to address these issues, it is an important step in the right direction. By platforming qualitative research in child development from around the world, and by highlighting the challenges faced in publishing this research, we hope to encourage a greater openness to publishing this work, and to acknowledging its value for the field of child development.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Infant and Child Development
Infant and Child Development PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL-
CiteScore
2.90
自引率
9.10%
发文量
93
期刊介绍: Infant and Child Development publishes high quality empirical, theoretical and methodological papers addressing psychological development from the antenatal period through to adolescence. The journal brings together research on: - social and emotional development - perceptual and motor development - cognitive development - language development atypical development (including conduct problems, anxiety and depressive conditions, language impairments, autistic spectrum disorders, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders)
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信