家庭语言环境与儿童早期发展:来自中国农村和城市周边地区的LENA研究

IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL
Xinwu Zhang, Delei Liu, Lucy Pappas, Sarah-Eve Dill, Tianli Feng, Yunting Zhang, Jin Zhao, Scott Rozelle, Yue Ma
{"title":"家庭语言环境与儿童早期发展:来自中国农村和城市周边地区的LENA研究","authors":"Xinwu Zhang, Delei Liu, Lucy Pappas, Sarah-Eve Dill, Tianli Feng, Yunting Zhang, Jin Zhao, Scott Rozelle, Yue Ma","doi":"10.1080/10888691.2023.2267440","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"AbstractThe home language environment is a significant correlate of early childhood development outcomes; however, less is known about this mechanism in rural and peri-urban China where rates of developmental delay are as high as 52%. This study examines associations between the home language environment and child development in a sample of 158 children (58% boys) aged 18–24 months (Mage = 21.5) from rural and peri-urban households in Western China. Results show a significant association between adult-child conversation count and language development, suggesting the home language environment may be a mechanism for child development in rural and peri-urban China. 22.5% of the sample were at risk of language delay. Mother’s employment and child’s age were significant factors in the home language environment.RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTSThis research investigates the links between the home language environment and ECD in rural and peri-urban communities in China.This study shows significant positive associations between measures of the home language environment and language development outcomes, as well as with overall ECD outcomes.Adult-child conversations were more important for language development outcomes than was the number of adult words spoken.Female-initiated conversations were more important than child-initiated conversations for language development. AcknowledgmentsWe would like to thank the local collaborators and survey enumerators for their assistance with contacting local families and assisting with the survey and LENA data collection, and the children and families who participated in this study.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Data availability statementThe data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.Notes1 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.23%.2 Increase in AWC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.30%.3 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.49%.4 Increase in the number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.13%.5 Increase in number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.34%.6 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.10%.7 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.36%.8 Increase in the number of VABs initiated by a female adult by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.32%.9 On a typical day, the mother or grandmother of the household wakes up around 6:00 am and proceeds with household chores, such as cleaning and cooking breakfast. Subsequently, she looks after the children either at home or at a nearby neighbor's house until lunchtime. During the afternoon, the child might take a nap or engage in play with other children or by themselves, while their primary caregiver prepares dinner. After dinner, the caregiver and the child get ready for bed, with the caregiver often telling them stories or watching television together before bedtime.Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by private gifts from individual donors. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.","PeriodicalId":47792,"journal":{"name":"Applied Developmental Science","volume":"51 2","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7000,"publicationDate":"2023-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"The home language environment and early childhood development: a LENA study from rural and peri-urban China\",\"authors\":\"Xinwu Zhang, Delei Liu, Lucy Pappas, Sarah-Eve Dill, Tianli Feng, Yunting Zhang, Jin Zhao, Scott Rozelle, Yue Ma\",\"doi\":\"10.1080/10888691.2023.2267440\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"AbstractThe home language environment is a significant correlate of early childhood development outcomes; however, less is known about this mechanism in rural and peri-urban China where rates of developmental delay are as high as 52%. This study examines associations between the home language environment and child development in a sample of 158 children (58% boys) aged 18–24 months (Mage = 21.5) from rural and peri-urban households in Western China. Results show a significant association between adult-child conversation count and language development, suggesting the home language environment may be a mechanism for child development in rural and peri-urban China. 22.5% of the sample were at risk of language delay. Mother’s employment and child’s age were significant factors in the home language environment.RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTSThis research investigates the links between the home language environment and ECD in rural and peri-urban communities in China.This study shows significant positive associations between measures of the home language environment and language development outcomes, as well as with overall ECD outcomes.Adult-child conversations were more important for language development outcomes than was the number of adult words spoken.Female-initiated conversations were more important than child-initiated conversations for language development. AcknowledgmentsWe would like to thank the local collaborators and survey enumerators for their assistance with contacting local families and assisting with the survey and LENA data collection, and the children and families who participated in this study.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Data availability statementThe data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.Notes1 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.23%.2 Increase in AWC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.30%.3 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.49%.4 Increase in the number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.13%.5 Increase in number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.34%.6 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.10%.7 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.36%.8 Increase in the number of VABs initiated by a female adult by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.32%.9 On a typical day, the mother or grandmother of the household wakes up around 6:00 am and proceeds with household chores, such as cleaning and cooking breakfast. Subsequently, she looks after the children either at home or at a nearby neighbor's house until lunchtime. During the afternoon, the child might take a nap or engage in play with other children or by themselves, while their primary caregiver prepares dinner. After dinner, the caregiver and the child get ready for bed, with the caregiver often telling them stories or watching television together before bedtime.Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by private gifts from individual donors. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.\",\"PeriodicalId\":47792,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Applied Developmental Science\",\"volume\":\"51 2\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.7000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-10-24\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Applied Developmental Science\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2023.2267440\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Applied Developmental Science","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2023.2267440","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

【摘要】家庭语言环境对儿童早期发展有重要影响;然而,在中国农村和城郊地区,人们对这一机制知之甚少,那里的发育迟缓率高达52%。本研究调查了来自中国西部农村和城郊家庭的158名18-24个月的儿童(58%为男孩)(年龄= 21.5)的家庭语言环境与儿童发展之间的关系。结果显示,成人-儿童对话数量与语言发展之间存在显著关联,表明家庭语言环境可能是中国农村和城郊儿童发展的一个机制。母亲的职业和子女的年龄是影响家庭语言环境的重要因素。本研究调查了中国农村和城郊社区的母语环境与幼儿发展之间的联系。这项研究表明,家庭语言环境的测量与语言发展结果之间存在显著的正相关关系,以及与总体ECD结果之间的正相关关系。成人与儿童之间的对话对语言发展结果的影响比成人所说的话的数量更重要。在语言发展方面,女性发起的对话比儿童发起的对话更重要。我们要感谢当地合作者和调查统计员帮助我们联系当地家庭,协助调查和LENA数据收集,以及参与本研究的儿童和家庭。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。数据可得性声明支持本研究结果的数据可根据通讯作者的合理要求获得。由于隐私或道德限制,这些数据不会公开。注1 CTC增加1%对应于CREDI总体z分数增加1.23%AWC每增加1%,对应于CREDI语言z分数增加1.30%CTC增加1%对应于CREDI语言z分数增加1.49%病灶儿童发起的vab的CT次数增加1%,对应于CREDI总z分数增加1.13%由焦点儿童发起的vab中CT次数增加1%,对应于CREDI语言z-分数增加1.34%CT次数增加1%与CREDI总z分数增加1.10%相关CT次数增加1%与CREDI语言z-score增加1.36%相关8女性成人发起的vab数量增加1%,对应于CREDI语言z分数增加1.32%在一个典型的日子里,家里的母亲或祖母早上6点左右起床,然后开始做家务,比如打扫卫生和做早餐。随后,她在家里或附近邻居家照顾孩子,直到午餐时间。下午,孩子可能会打个盹,或与其他孩子或自己玩耍,而他们的主要照顾者则准备晚餐。晚饭后,照顾者和孩子准备睡觉,照顾者经常在睡觉前给他们讲故事或一起看电视。本研究由个人捐赠的私人礼物支持。资助者在研究设计、数据收集和分析、发表决定或手稿准备方面没有任何作用。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
The home language environment and early childhood development: a LENA study from rural and peri-urban China
AbstractThe home language environment is a significant correlate of early childhood development outcomes; however, less is known about this mechanism in rural and peri-urban China where rates of developmental delay are as high as 52%. This study examines associations between the home language environment and child development in a sample of 158 children (58% boys) aged 18–24 months (Mage = 21.5) from rural and peri-urban households in Western China. Results show a significant association between adult-child conversation count and language development, suggesting the home language environment may be a mechanism for child development in rural and peri-urban China. 22.5% of the sample were at risk of language delay. Mother’s employment and child’s age were significant factors in the home language environment.RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTSThis research investigates the links between the home language environment and ECD in rural and peri-urban communities in China.This study shows significant positive associations between measures of the home language environment and language development outcomes, as well as with overall ECD outcomes.Adult-child conversations were more important for language development outcomes than was the number of adult words spoken.Female-initiated conversations were more important than child-initiated conversations for language development. AcknowledgmentsWe would like to thank the local collaborators and survey enumerators for their assistance with contacting local families and assisting with the survey and LENA data collection, and the children and families who participated in this study.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Data availability statementThe data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.Notes1 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.23%.2 Increase in AWC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.30%.3 Increase in CTC by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.49%.4 Increase in the number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.13%.5 Increase in number of CT in VABs initiated by the focus child by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.34%.6 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI overall z-scores by 1.10%.7 Increase in the number of CT by 1% was associated with an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.36%.8 Increase in the number of VABs initiated by a female adult by 1% corresponded to an increase in CREDI language z-scores by 1.32%.9 On a typical day, the mother or grandmother of the household wakes up around 6:00 am and proceeds with household chores, such as cleaning and cooking breakfast. Subsequently, she looks after the children either at home or at a nearby neighbor's house until lunchtime. During the afternoon, the child might take a nap or engage in play with other children or by themselves, while their primary caregiver prepares dinner. After dinner, the caregiver and the child get ready for bed, with the caregiver often telling them stories or watching television together before bedtime.Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by private gifts from individual donors. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Applied Developmental Science
Applied Developmental Science PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL-
CiteScore
12.00
自引率
2.60%
发文量
23
期刊介绍: The focus of this multidisciplinary journal is the synthesis of research and application to promote positive development across the life span and across the globe. The journal publishes research that generates descriptive and explanatory knowledge about dynamic and reciprocal person-environment interactions essential to informed public dialogue, social policy, and preventive and development optimizing interventions. This includes research relevant to the development of individuals and social systems across the life span -- including the wide range of familial, biological, societal, cultural, physical, ecological, political and historical settings of human development.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信