关于情景规划实验研究的边界:对德比郡等人的评论(2022)

Ahti Salo
{"title":"关于情景规划实验研究的边界:对德比郡等人的评论(2022)","authors":"Ahti Salo","doi":"10.1002/ffo2.152","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The authors of this thoughtfully crafted article argue cogently that increased efforts should be taken to strengthen the role of experiments in building an accumulated body of knowledge of scenario planning. While such efforts can foster the emergence of promising research results, it is pertinent to remain cognizant of the realities which put limits on what experiments can contribute to the advancement such knowledge. Many of these realities ensue from the distinctive characteristics of scenario planning as an intervention (or, using the terminology of experimental design, the “treatment” as the independent variable). Such interventions can be carried out in alternative ways to promote desired outcomes (e.g., inducing changes in the participants' mental models). Apart from the intervention, these outcomes also depend on contextual factors of which some may not be under the experimenter's control. For instance, because scenario planning is typically a group activity, the outcomes depend not only the selected scenario method but also on how well the participants are able to communicate with each other, which in turn depends on their linguistic skills, cognitive abilities, and educational background, including familiarity with futures studies.</p><p>For starters, one can posit that the variables in terms of which the interventions, outcomes, and contextual factors are characterized should be similar enough to those encountered in the practice of scenario planning. This would be a prerequisite for interpreting experiments from the viewpoint of practice and for inferring tentative generalizations. Without such a correspondence, there is a potential danger that the experimental research would evolve as a semi-independent activity which—despite fostering the emergence of a continuing stream of empirical experiments as such—would have limited impact in informing the work of practitioners who would continue to rely on their accumulated body of expertise and the insights that they have gained from the many sources of information at their disposal, including anecdotal evidence in reported case studies.</p><p>There is an inherent challenge in that if the interventions (e.g., variants of scenario processes), their outcomes (e.g., impacts on mental models), and contextual factors (e.g., participants’ level of trust in each other) are specified with a higher level of granularity, it becomes exceedingly laborious to carry out sufficiently many experimental runs to arrive at validated—perhaps statistically significant—conclusions about the likely outcomes of a given scenario approach in a specific planning context.</p><p>To illustrate this point, consider a setting in which there are five participants in each scenario group and four alternative interventions to scenario development based on two variables, (i) the number of scenarios (small vs. large) and (ii) the approach to the characterization of uncertainties (quantitative vs. qualitative). Furthermore, assume that the contextual factors are associated with two variables, (iii) level of expertise (students vs. experienced managers) and (iv) the degree to which the participants know each other before the scenario process (no prior collaboration vs. close colleagues).</p><p>In this experimental setup, one would need 5 × 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 = 80 participants to obtain a <i>single</i> observation for each of the 16 possible combinations of these four variables. To arrive at statistically significant results, one would need several observations for each of these combinations. Thus, if the aim is to study many methods and multiple contexts subject to the demands of controlled experiments, this combinatorial growth means that the total number of participants to be recruited would quickly become larger than what can be typically accommodated. This difficulty is compounded by the fact that the participants would not be able to take part in more than a single experimental run (i.e., if the participants have already completed a scenario process using one method, this earlier exposure would affect their behaviour when working with another method).</p><p>As a result, if controlled experiments are carried out with the aim of producing statistically significant conclusions, the number of variables and their values in specifying the experimental design (e.g., the 16 combinations above) would have to be limited to very few and the level of granularity in characterizing these variables would have to remain relatively coarse. Due to this coarseness, experimental results may be more adequate for uncovering “general patterns”, formulated as statements about what can <i>usually</i> be expected to occur when scenario planning is carried out in such-and-such a way. In contrast, there would be limited possibilities for making reliable predictions about what outcomes will be attained across the full range of the many ways in which the contextual factors do manifest themselves in practice. It may be particularly challenging to study outcomes caused by interactions between two or more variables. By necessity, all the variables that are involved in such interactions would have to be retained to study the impacts that come about due to the joint occurrence of specific combinations of values for two more variables in the experimental design.</p><p>On another note, one needs to bear in mind that statistically significant results are just that–statistical. Their predictive power stems from statistical inferences about what can be expected to happen, on average, when a <i>similar</i> intervention is repeated in an essentially <i>similar</i> context. But unless the results are confirmed with an exceptionally high level of significance (in which case they may be so obvious that no experiments are needed to ascertain them), the results do not dictate what the outcome will be for the <i>next</i> observation that is of acute interest. This may diminish the relevance of statistical significance in providing normative guidance for the design for real-life scenario processes which come in many shapes and sizes. There may also be contextual factors that have not been covered in the experimental literature, making it hard to interpret to what extent the findings of related earlier experiments would hold nevertheless.</p><p>Challenges in setting the adequate scope of experiments are likely to surface when seeking to give a precise meaning to what is meant by the “outcomes” of scenario planning as well. Technically, it is not straightforward to measure acclaimed outcomes such as “changing the participants' mental models”. Furthermore, in the larger scheme of things, scenario planning is not an end in itself: rather, it is one of the structured approaches that can contribute to the shaping and implementation of more informed strategies, thereby helping organizations prosper in a world in which resources are in short supply and which is either more or less turbulent (see, e.g., Amer et al., <span>2013</span>; Bunn &amp; Salo, <span>1993</span>). Crucially, the contribution that scenario planning can make to support organizational survival depends not only on scenario planning but also on the extent to which organizations are faced with such turbulence or can exert influence on it (cf. Vilkkumaa et al., <span>2018</span>).</p><p>One could even hypothesize that in stable and predictable planning contexts, processes of onerous scenario planning could–despite their measurable impacts on the participants' mental models–lead to excessive administrative overheads. Thus, the relative merits of scenario planning cannot be fully evaluated in isolation of the planning contexts it is enacted. For instance, if the oil crisis of the 1970s had not occurred, the Shell scenarios (Wack, <span>1985</span>) would probably not have become so celebrated. For comprehensiveness, then, the emerging agenda on experimental research should seek to ascertain in what kinds of planning contexts, differentiated by their degree of turbulence for example, scenario planning may be most effective.</p><p>The above points on coarseness have parallels to the selection and characterization of uncertainty factors in scenario development. Often, scenarios are built from a few uncertainty factors (e.g., GDP growth) whose possible realizations (e.g., more than 3%) are described using a few verbal descriptions (or intervals, if numerical measurement scales can be associated with uncertainty factors; see Salo et al., <span>2022</span>; Tosoni et al., <span>2019</span>). Because in most scenario processes only rather few scenarios are elaborated, there will be a very large number of possible futures that are not explicitly addressed even within the “closed” boundaries set by the uncertainty factors and their realizations. These boundaries can be expanded through deliberate attempts to accommodate extraordinary phenomena, whereby the scenarios and the processual outcomes of scenario planning may become less predictable and less repeatable. One may therefore hypothesize that the more formal approaches to scenario planning may be more amenable for experimental studies, premised on the assumption that they exhibit more regularities than less structured approaches which rely on intuitionist interactions.</p><p>The above remarks notwithstanding, I believe that more experimental research on scenario planning is called for. In view of the large variety of methods of scenario planning and the many kinds of contexts in which they are deployed, it may be hard to arrive at general results that hold conclusively, always and everywhere. Still, even modest experimental findings may be highly useful in advising the shaping of scenario processes and the emergence of better “boilerplate designs” that can be instantiated repeatedly across comparable planning contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":100567,"journal":{"name":"FUTURES & FORESIGHT SCIENCE","volume":"5 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ffo2.152","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"On the boundaries of experimental research on scenario planning: A commentary on Derbyshire et al. (2022)\",\"authors\":\"Ahti Salo\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/ffo2.152\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>The authors of this thoughtfully crafted article argue cogently that increased efforts should be taken to strengthen the role of experiments in building an accumulated body of knowledge of scenario planning. While such efforts can foster the emergence of promising research results, it is pertinent to remain cognizant of the realities which put limits on what experiments can contribute to the advancement such knowledge. Many of these realities ensue from the distinctive characteristics of scenario planning as an intervention (or, using the terminology of experimental design, the “treatment” as the independent variable). Such interventions can be carried out in alternative ways to promote desired outcomes (e.g., inducing changes in the participants' mental models). Apart from the intervention, these outcomes also depend on contextual factors of which some may not be under the experimenter's control. For instance, because scenario planning is typically a group activity, the outcomes depend not only the selected scenario method but also on how well the participants are able to communicate with each other, which in turn depends on their linguistic skills, cognitive abilities, and educational background, including familiarity with futures studies.</p><p>For starters, one can posit that the variables in terms of which the interventions, outcomes, and contextual factors are characterized should be similar enough to those encountered in the practice of scenario planning. This would be a prerequisite for interpreting experiments from the viewpoint of practice and for inferring tentative generalizations. Without such a correspondence, there is a potential danger that the experimental research would evolve as a semi-independent activity which—despite fostering the emergence of a continuing stream of empirical experiments as such—would have limited impact in informing the work of practitioners who would continue to rely on their accumulated body of expertise and the insights that they have gained from the many sources of information at their disposal, including anecdotal evidence in reported case studies.</p><p>There is an inherent challenge in that if the interventions (e.g., variants of scenario processes), their outcomes (e.g., impacts on mental models), and contextual factors (e.g., participants’ level of trust in each other) are specified with a higher level of granularity, it becomes exceedingly laborious to carry out sufficiently many experimental runs to arrive at validated—perhaps statistically significant—conclusions about the likely outcomes of a given scenario approach in a specific planning context.</p><p>To illustrate this point, consider a setting in which there are five participants in each scenario group and four alternative interventions to scenario development based on two variables, (i) the number of scenarios (small vs. large) and (ii) the approach to the characterization of uncertainties (quantitative vs. qualitative). Furthermore, assume that the contextual factors are associated with two variables, (iii) level of expertise (students vs. experienced managers) and (iv) the degree to which the participants know each other before the scenario process (no prior collaboration vs. close colleagues).</p><p>In this experimental setup, one would need 5 × 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 = 80 participants to obtain a <i>single</i> observation for each of the 16 possible combinations of these four variables. To arrive at statistically significant results, one would need several observations for each of these combinations. Thus, if the aim is to study many methods and multiple contexts subject to the demands of controlled experiments, this combinatorial growth means that the total number of participants to be recruited would quickly become larger than what can be typically accommodated. This difficulty is compounded by the fact that the participants would not be able to take part in more than a single experimental run (i.e., if the participants have already completed a scenario process using one method, this earlier exposure would affect their behaviour when working with another method).</p><p>As a result, if controlled experiments are carried out with the aim of producing statistically significant conclusions, the number of variables and their values in specifying the experimental design (e.g., the 16 combinations above) would have to be limited to very few and the level of granularity in characterizing these variables would have to remain relatively coarse. Due to this coarseness, experimental results may be more adequate for uncovering “general patterns”, formulated as statements about what can <i>usually</i> be expected to occur when scenario planning is carried out in such-and-such a way. In contrast, there would be limited possibilities for making reliable predictions about what outcomes will be attained across the full range of the many ways in which the contextual factors do manifest themselves in practice. It may be particularly challenging to study outcomes caused by interactions between two or more variables. By necessity, all the variables that are involved in such interactions would have to be retained to study the impacts that come about due to the joint occurrence of specific combinations of values for two more variables in the experimental design.</p><p>On another note, one needs to bear in mind that statistically significant results are just that–statistical. Their predictive power stems from statistical inferences about what can be expected to happen, on average, when a <i>similar</i> intervention is repeated in an essentially <i>similar</i> context. But unless the results are confirmed with an exceptionally high level of significance (in which case they may be so obvious that no experiments are needed to ascertain them), the results do not dictate what the outcome will be for the <i>next</i> observation that is of acute interest. This may diminish the relevance of statistical significance in providing normative guidance for the design for real-life scenario processes which come in many shapes and sizes. There may also be contextual factors that have not been covered in the experimental literature, making it hard to interpret to what extent the findings of related earlier experiments would hold nevertheless.</p><p>Challenges in setting the adequate scope of experiments are likely to surface when seeking to give a precise meaning to what is meant by the “outcomes” of scenario planning as well. Technically, it is not straightforward to measure acclaimed outcomes such as “changing the participants' mental models”. Furthermore, in the larger scheme of things, scenario planning is not an end in itself: rather, it is one of the structured approaches that can contribute to the shaping and implementation of more informed strategies, thereby helping organizations prosper in a world in which resources are in short supply and which is either more or less turbulent (see, e.g., Amer et al., <span>2013</span>; Bunn &amp; Salo, <span>1993</span>). Crucially, the contribution that scenario planning can make to support organizational survival depends not only on scenario planning but also on the extent to which organizations are faced with such turbulence or can exert influence on it (cf. Vilkkumaa et al., <span>2018</span>).</p><p>One could even hypothesize that in stable and predictable planning contexts, processes of onerous scenario planning could–despite their measurable impacts on the participants' mental models–lead to excessive administrative overheads. Thus, the relative merits of scenario planning cannot be fully evaluated in isolation of the planning contexts it is enacted. For instance, if the oil crisis of the 1970s had not occurred, the Shell scenarios (Wack, <span>1985</span>) would probably not have become so celebrated. For comprehensiveness, then, the emerging agenda on experimental research should seek to ascertain in what kinds of planning contexts, differentiated by their degree of turbulence for example, scenario planning may be most effective.</p><p>The above points on coarseness have parallels to the selection and characterization of uncertainty factors in scenario development. Often, scenarios are built from a few uncertainty factors (e.g., GDP growth) whose possible realizations (e.g., more than 3%) are described using a few verbal descriptions (or intervals, if numerical measurement scales can be associated with uncertainty factors; see Salo et al., <span>2022</span>; Tosoni et al., <span>2019</span>). Because in most scenario processes only rather few scenarios are elaborated, there will be a very large number of possible futures that are not explicitly addressed even within the “closed” boundaries set by the uncertainty factors and their realizations. These boundaries can be expanded through deliberate attempts to accommodate extraordinary phenomena, whereby the scenarios and the processual outcomes of scenario planning may become less predictable and less repeatable. One may therefore hypothesize that the more formal approaches to scenario planning may be more amenable for experimental studies, premised on the assumption that they exhibit more regularities than less structured approaches which rely on intuitionist interactions.</p><p>The above remarks notwithstanding, I believe that more experimental research on scenario planning is called for. In view of the large variety of methods of scenario planning and the many kinds of contexts in which they are deployed, it may be hard to arrive at general results that hold conclusively, always and everywhere. Still, even modest experimental findings may be highly useful in advising the shaping of scenario processes and the emergence of better “boilerplate designs” that can be instantiated repeatedly across comparable planning contexts.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":100567,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"FUTURES & FORESIGHT SCIENCE\",\"volume\":\"5 2\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-03-06\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ffo2.152\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"FUTURES & FORESIGHT SCIENCE\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ffo2.152\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"FUTURES & FORESIGHT SCIENCE","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ffo2.152","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

摘要

这篇精心撰写的文章的作者令人信服地认为,应该加大力度,加强实验在积累场景规划知识方面的作用。虽然这些努力可以促进有希望的研究结果的出现,但重要的是要认识到现实,这些现实限制了哪些实验可以促进这些知识的进步。这些现实中的许多都源于情景规划作为干预的独特特征(或者,使用实验设计的术语,将“治疗”作为自变量)。这种干预可以以其他方式进行,以促进预期的结果(例如,诱导参与者的心理模型发生变化)。除了干预之外,这些结果还取决于情境因素,其中一些因素可能不在实验者的控制之下。例如,由于情景规划通常是一项集体活动,结果不仅取决于所选的情景方法,还取决于参与者相互沟通的能力,这反过来又取决于他们的语言技能、认知能力和教育背景,包括对未来研究的熟悉程度。首先,可以假设干预措施、结果和背景因素所用的变量应该与情景规划实践中遇到的变量足够相似。这将是从实践的角度解释实验和推断初步概括的先决条件。在没有这种对应关系的情况下,有一种潜在的危险是,实验研究将演变为一种半独立的活动,尽管这种活动促进了持续不断的实证实验的出现,但对从业者的工作影响有限,他们将继续依赖他们积累的专业知识和从许多来源获得的见解他们掌握的信息,包括报告的案例研究中的轶事证据。存在一个固有的挑战,即如果干预措施(例如,情景过程的变体)、其结果(例如,对心理模型的影响)和情境因素(例如,参与者对彼此的信任程度)以更高的粒度指定,要进行足够多的实验运行,以得出关于特定规划环境中给定场景方法的可能结果的经过验证的结论,这可能具有统计学意义,这变得非常困难。为了说明这一点,考虑一个场景,在该场景中,每个场景组有五名参与者,并基于两个变量,即(i)场景的数量(小与大)和(ii)不确定性表征方法(定量与定性),对场景开发进行四种替代干预。此外,假设情境因素与两个变量有关,(iii)专业水平(学生与经验丰富的管理者)和(iv)参与者在场景过程之前相互了解的程度(之前没有合作与亲密同事) × 2. × 2. × 2. × 2. = 80名参与者,以获得这四个变量的16个可能组合中的每一个的单个观察结果。为了得出具有统计学意义的结果,需要对这些组合中的每一个进行多次观察。因此,如果目标是根据受控实验的要求研究许多方法和多种背景,这种组合增长意味着要招募的参与者总数将很快变得比通常能容纳的人数更多。参与者不能参加一次以上的实验(即,如果参与者已经使用一种方法完成了一个场景过程,那么这种早期的暴露会影响他们使用另一种方法时的行为),这一事实加剧了这一困难,如果对照实验的目的是得出具有统计学意义的结论,那么在指定实验设计时(例如,上面的16个组合),变量的数量及其值必须限制在非常少的范围内,并且表征这些变量的粒度水平必须保持相对粗糙。由于这种粗糙性,实验结果可能更适合于揭示“一般模式”,即当以这样或那样的方式进行场景规划时,通常可以预期会发生什么。相比之下,在背景因素在实践中表现出来的多种方式中,对将取得的结果做出可靠预测的可能性有限。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
On the boundaries of experimental research on scenario planning: A commentary on Derbyshire et al. (2022)

The authors of this thoughtfully crafted article argue cogently that increased efforts should be taken to strengthen the role of experiments in building an accumulated body of knowledge of scenario planning. While such efforts can foster the emergence of promising research results, it is pertinent to remain cognizant of the realities which put limits on what experiments can contribute to the advancement such knowledge. Many of these realities ensue from the distinctive characteristics of scenario planning as an intervention (or, using the terminology of experimental design, the “treatment” as the independent variable). Such interventions can be carried out in alternative ways to promote desired outcomes (e.g., inducing changes in the participants' mental models). Apart from the intervention, these outcomes also depend on contextual factors of which some may not be under the experimenter's control. For instance, because scenario planning is typically a group activity, the outcomes depend not only the selected scenario method but also on how well the participants are able to communicate with each other, which in turn depends on their linguistic skills, cognitive abilities, and educational background, including familiarity with futures studies.

For starters, one can posit that the variables in terms of which the interventions, outcomes, and contextual factors are characterized should be similar enough to those encountered in the practice of scenario planning. This would be a prerequisite for interpreting experiments from the viewpoint of practice and for inferring tentative generalizations. Without such a correspondence, there is a potential danger that the experimental research would evolve as a semi-independent activity which—despite fostering the emergence of a continuing stream of empirical experiments as such—would have limited impact in informing the work of practitioners who would continue to rely on their accumulated body of expertise and the insights that they have gained from the many sources of information at their disposal, including anecdotal evidence in reported case studies.

There is an inherent challenge in that if the interventions (e.g., variants of scenario processes), their outcomes (e.g., impacts on mental models), and contextual factors (e.g., participants’ level of trust in each other) are specified with a higher level of granularity, it becomes exceedingly laborious to carry out sufficiently many experimental runs to arrive at validated—perhaps statistically significant—conclusions about the likely outcomes of a given scenario approach in a specific planning context.

To illustrate this point, consider a setting in which there are five participants in each scenario group and four alternative interventions to scenario development based on two variables, (i) the number of scenarios (small vs. large) and (ii) the approach to the characterization of uncertainties (quantitative vs. qualitative). Furthermore, assume that the contextual factors are associated with two variables, (iii) level of expertise (students vs. experienced managers) and (iv) the degree to which the participants know each other before the scenario process (no prior collaboration vs. close colleagues).

In this experimental setup, one would need 5 × 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 = 80 participants to obtain a single observation for each of the 16 possible combinations of these four variables. To arrive at statistically significant results, one would need several observations for each of these combinations. Thus, if the aim is to study many methods and multiple contexts subject to the demands of controlled experiments, this combinatorial growth means that the total number of participants to be recruited would quickly become larger than what can be typically accommodated. This difficulty is compounded by the fact that the participants would not be able to take part in more than a single experimental run (i.e., if the participants have already completed a scenario process using one method, this earlier exposure would affect their behaviour when working with another method).

As a result, if controlled experiments are carried out with the aim of producing statistically significant conclusions, the number of variables and their values in specifying the experimental design (e.g., the 16 combinations above) would have to be limited to very few and the level of granularity in characterizing these variables would have to remain relatively coarse. Due to this coarseness, experimental results may be more adequate for uncovering “general patterns”, formulated as statements about what can usually be expected to occur when scenario planning is carried out in such-and-such a way. In contrast, there would be limited possibilities for making reliable predictions about what outcomes will be attained across the full range of the many ways in which the contextual factors do manifest themselves in practice. It may be particularly challenging to study outcomes caused by interactions between two or more variables. By necessity, all the variables that are involved in such interactions would have to be retained to study the impacts that come about due to the joint occurrence of specific combinations of values for two more variables in the experimental design.

On another note, one needs to bear in mind that statistically significant results are just that–statistical. Their predictive power stems from statistical inferences about what can be expected to happen, on average, when a similar intervention is repeated in an essentially similar context. But unless the results are confirmed with an exceptionally high level of significance (in which case they may be so obvious that no experiments are needed to ascertain them), the results do not dictate what the outcome will be for the next observation that is of acute interest. This may diminish the relevance of statistical significance in providing normative guidance for the design for real-life scenario processes which come in many shapes and sizes. There may also be contextual factors that have not been covered in the experimental literature, making it hard to interpret to what extent the findings of related earlier experiments would hold nevertheless.

Challenges in setting the adequate scope of experiments are likely to surface when seeking to give a precise meaning to what is meant by the “outcomes” of scenario planning as well. Technically, it is not straightforward to measure acclaimed outcomes such as “changing the participants' mental models”. Furthermore, in the larger scheme of things, scenario planning is not an end in itself: rather, it is one of the structured approaches that can contribute to the shaping and implementation of more informed strategies, thereby helping organizations prosper in a world in which resources are in short supply and which is either more or less turbulent (see, e.g., Amer et al., 2013; Bunn & Salo, 1993). Crucially, the contribution that scenario planning can make to support organizational survival depends not only on scenario planning but also on the extent to which organizations are faced with such turbulence or can exert influence on it (cf. Vilkkumaa et al., 2018).

One could even hypothesize that in stable and predictable planning contexts, processes of onerous scenario planning could–despite their measurable impacts on the participants' mental models–lead to excessive administrative overheads. Thus, the relative merits of scenario planning cannot be fully evaluated in isolation of the planning contexts it is enacted. For instance, if the oil crisis of the 1970s had not occurred, the Shell scenarios (Wack, 1985) would probably not have become so celebrated. For comprehensiveness, then, the emerging agenda on experimental research should seek to ascertain in what kinds of planning contexts, differentiated by their degree of turbulence for example, scenario planning may be most effective.

The above points on coarseness have parallels to the selection and characterization of uncertainty factors in scenario development. Often, scenarios are built from a few uncertainty factors (e.g., GDP growth) whose possible realizations (e.g., more than 3%) are described using a few verbal descriptions (or intervals, if numerical measurement scales can be associated with uncertainty factors; see Salo et al., 2022; Tosoni et al., 2019). Because in most scenario processes only rather few scenarios are elaborated, there will be a very large number of possible futures that are not explicitly addressed even within the “closed” boundaries set by the uncertainty factors and their realizations. These boundaries can be expanded through deliberate attempts to accommodate extraordinary phenomena, whereby the scenarios and the processual outcomes of scenario planning may become less predictable and less repeatable. One may therefore hypothesize that the more formal approaches to scenario planning may be more amenable for experimental studies, premised on the assumption that they exhibit more regularities than less structured approaches which rely on intuitionist interactions.

The above remarks notwithstanding, I believe that more experimental research on scenario planning is called for. In view of the large variety of methods of scenario planning and the many kinds of contexts in which they are deployed, it may be hard to arrive at general results that hold conclusively, always and everywhere. Still, even modest experimental findings may be highly useful in advising the shaping of scenario processes and the emergence of better “boilerplate designs” that can be instantiated repeatedly across comparable planning contexts.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
7.00
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信