{"title":"医师判断和临床实践指南","authors":"R. Hamm, Zsolt J. Nagykaldi","doi":"10.1177/1555343418782850","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care” by Paul R. Falzer in this special issue. Falzer (2018) and Klein, Woods, Klein, and Perry (2016) have called attention to the fact that external payers (insurance companies, government programs) may reward or penalize individual physicians or clinical groups depending on whether their behavior accords with the recommendation of an evidence-based clinical practice guideline (EB-CPG). (Klein et al. [2016] call this the “best practices regimen,” but to avoid confusion with a methodology focused on design of exemplary and successful approaches for accomplishing clinic tasks [Mold & Gregory, 2003], we will refer to “enforced conformance” to EB-CPG). Clinic administration may then institute a system to measure and reward individual physician performance to assure the group practice overall meets the stated standard. Falzer identifies flaws in the logic behind making reward contingent on meeting guideline-related standards and reviews the varied responses physicians have to enforced conformance. Although we concur with many aspects of the critique, we value the science and wisdom embodied in the guidelines and recognize that at times physicians may need external motivation to heed them. The focus is CPGs that in good faith address the health needs of patients and society. There are three frameworks for these. Guidelines can be based on the judgments of expert clinicians and other stakeholders (Crownover & Unwin, 2005), on studies providing justified evidence that clinical practices are likely to have beneficial effects (Alonso-Coello et al., 2016), or on broader analyses that consider the money needed to produce those beneficial effects so that society’s resources may be allocated to the most cost-effective practices (Mandelblatt, Fryback, Weinstein, Russell, & Gold, 1997; Pandya, 2018). Interestingly, the expert judgment recommendations are correlated with the cost-effectiveness analysis recommendations (Kuntz, Tsevat, Weinstein, & Goldman, 1999). CPGs from any of these frameworks can experience the problems Falzer (2018) identifies when recommendations become requirements and nonconformance with them is penalized. However, physicians cannot simply be ordered to behave in a way that maximizes rewards or optimizes outcomes. The way they manage patients is based on long-established habit (Hamm, 2009a). Hearing about, reading, or studying an EB-CPG endorsed by a respected authority does not make physicians immediately change behavior, even if they intend to. They have to learn the recommended alternative behavior, recognize when the guideline describes something different from what they usually do, and make the conscious choice to change behavior. This must be done consciously until it can become a new habit (Abernathy & Hamm, 1995). This situation is more complex when the current habitual practice involves multiple actors (Ackerman, Gonzales, Stahl, & Metlay, 2013; Gonzales, Steiner, Lum, & Barrett, 1999), not just the individual doctor, and when the current practice pays better (Hamm, 2009b). Hence an individual physician may need to be strongly motivated to conform to a guideline even when he or she intellectually accepts it as correct. 782850 EDMXXX10.1177/1555343418782850Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingJudgment and Guidelines 2018","PeriodicalId":46342,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","volume":"12 1","pages":"209 - 214"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2000,"publicationDate":"2018-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418782850","citationCount":"4","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Physician Judgment and Clinical Practice Guidelines\",\"authors\":\"R. Hamm, Zsolt J. Nagykaldi\",\"doi\":\"10.1177/1555343418782850\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care” by Paul R. Falzer in this special issue. Falzer (2018) and Klein, Woods, Klein, and Perry (2016) have called attention to the fact that external payers (insurance companies, government programs) may reward or penalize individual physicians or clinical groups depending on whether their behavior accords with the recommendation of an evidence-based clinical practice guideline (EB-CPG). (Klein et al. [2016] call this the “best practices regimen,” but to avoid confusion with a methodology focused on design of exemplary and successful approaches for accomplishing clinic tasks [Mold & Gregory, 2003], we will refer to “enforced conformance” to EB-CPG). Clinic administration may then institute a system to measure and reward individual physician performance to assure the group practice overall meets the stated standard. Falzer identifies flaws in the logic behind making reward contingent on meeting guideline-related standards and reviews the varied responses physicians have to enforced conformance. Although we concur with many aspects of the critique, we value the science and wisdom embodied in the guidelines and recognize that at times physicians may need external motivation to heed them. The focus is CPGs that in good faith address the health needs of patients and society. There are three frameworks for these. Guidelines can be based on the judgments of expert clinicians and other stakeholders (Crownover & Unwin, 2005), on studies providing justified evidence that clinical practices are likely to have beneficial effects (Alonso-Coello et al., 2016), or on broader analyses that consider the money needed to produce those beneficial effects so that society’s resources may be allocated to the most cost-effective practices (Mandelblatt, Fryback, Weinstein, Russell, & Gold, 1997; Pandya, 2018). Interestingly, the expert judgment recommendations are correlated with the cost-effectiveness analysis recommendations (Kuntz, Tsevat, Weinstein, & Goldman, 1999). CPGs from any of these frameworks can experience the problems Falzer (2018) identifies when recommendations become requirements and nonconformance with them is penalized. However, physicians cannot simply be ordered to behave in a way that maximizes rewards or optimizes outcomes. The way they manage patients is based on long-established habit (Hamm, 2009a). Hearing about, reading, or studying an EB-CPG endorsed by a respected authority does not make physicians immediately change behavior, even if they intend to. They have to learn the recommended alternative behavior, recognize when the guideline describes something different from what they usually do, and make the conscious choice to change behavior. This must be done consciously until it can become a new habit (Abernathy & Hamm, 1995). This situation is more complex when the current habitual practice involves multiple actors (Ackerman, Gonzales, Stahl, & Metlay, 2013; Gonzales, Steiner, Lum, & Barrett, 1999), not just the individual doctor, and when the current practice pays better (Hamm, 2009b). Hence an individual physician may need to be strongly motivated to conform to a guideline even when he or she intellectually accepts it as correct. 782850 EDMXXX10.1177/1555343418782850Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingJudgment and Guidelines 2018\",\"PeriodicalId\":46342,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making\",\"volume\":\"12 1\",\"pages\":\"209 - 214\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.2000,\"publicationDate\":\"2018-08-16\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1555343418782850\",\"citationCount\":\"4\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418782850\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1555343418782850","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"ENGINEERING, INDUSTRIAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
摘要
Paul R.Falzer在本期特刊中的《决策与医疗实践》。Falzer(2018)和Klein、Woods、Klein和Perry(2016)呼吁注意这样一个事实,即外部付款人(保险公司、政府项目)可能会根据个人医生或临床团体的行为是否符合循证临床实践指南(EB-CPG)的建议来奖励或惩罚他们。(Klein等人[2016]称之为“最佳实践方案”,但为了避免与专注于设计完成临床任务的示范性和成功方法的方法相混淆[Mold&Gregory,2003],我们将提及EB-CPG的“强制合规性”)。然后,诊所管理部门可以建立一个系统来衡量和奖励个别医生的表现,以确保团队实践总体符合规定的标准。Falzer发现了将奖励视为符合指南相关标准的逻辑中的缺陷,并审查了医生对强制遵守的各种反应。尽管我们同意批评的许多方面,但我们重视指导方针中体现的科学和智慧,并认识到有时医生可能需要外部动机来关注它们。重点是本着诚意满足患者和社会健康需求的CPG。有三个框架。指南可以基于临床医生专家和其他利益相关者的判断(Crownover&Unwin,2005),基于提供合理证据证明临床实践可能具有有益效果的研究(Alonso-Coello等人,2016),或者基于更广泛的分析,考虑产生这些有益效果所需的资金,以便将社会资源分配给最具成本效益的做法(Mandelblatt,Fryback,Weinstein,Russell,&Gold,1997;Pandya,2018)。有趣的是,专家判断建议与成本效益分析建议相关(Kuntz,Tsevat,Weinstein,&Goldman,1999)。当建议成为要求时,来自任何这些框架的CPG都可能遇到Falzer(2018)发现的问题,不符合这些建议的情况将受到惩罚。然而,不能简单地命令医生以最大化奖励或优化结果的方式行事。他们管理病人的方式是基于长期养成的习惯(Hamm,2009a)。听到、阅读或研究由受人尊敬的权威机构认可的EB-CPG并不能让医生立即改变行为,即使他们打算这样做。他们必须学习推荐的替代行为,识别指南何时描述了与他们通常做的不同的事情,并有意识地选择改变行为。这必须有意识地进行,直到它成为一种新习惯(Abernathy&Ham,1995)。当当前的习惯性做法涉及多个参与者时,这种情况会更加复杂(Ackerman,Gonzales,Stahl,&Metlay,2013;Gonzales、Steiner、Lum和Barrett,1999),而不仅仅是单个医生,当当前的做法效果更好时(Hamm,2009b)。因此,个体医生可能需要强烈的动机来遵守指导方针,即使他或她在理智上接受它是正确的。782850 EDMXX10.1177/1555334341878250Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingJudge and Guidelines 2018
Physician Judgment and Clinical Practice Guidelines
Decision Making and the Practice of Health Care” by Paul R. Falzer in this special issue. Falzer (2018) and Klein, Woods, Klein, and Perry (2016) have called attention to the fact that external payers (insurance companies, government programs) may reward or penalize individual physicians or clinical groups depending on whether their behavior accords with the recommendation of an evidence-based clinical practice guideline (EB-CPG). (Klein et al. [2016] call this the “best practices regimen,” but to avoid confusion with a methodology focused on design of exemplary and successful approaches for accomplishing clinic tasks [Mold & Gregory, 2003], we will refer to “enforced conformance” to EB-CPG). Clinic administration may then institute a system to measure and reward individual physician performance to assure the group practice overall meets the stated standard. Falzer identifies flaws in the logic behind making reward contingent on meeting guideline-related standards and reviews the varied responses physicians have to enforced conformance. Although we concur with many aspects of the critique, we value the science and wisdom embodied in the guidelines and recognize that at times physicians may need external motivation to heed them. The focus is CPGs that in good faith address the health needs of patients and society. There are three frameworks for these. Guidelines can be based on the judgments of expert clinicians and other stakeholders (Crownover & Unwin, 2005), on studies providing justified evidence that clinical practices are likely to have beneficial effects (Alonso-Coello et al., 2016), or on broader analyses that consider the money needed to produce those beneficial effects so that society’s resources may be allocated to the most cost-effective practices (Mandelblatt, Fryback, Weinstein, Russell, & Gold, 1997; Pandya, 2018). Interestingly, the expert judgment recommendations are correlated with the cost-effectiveness analysis recommendations (Kuntz, Tsevat, Weinstein, & Goldman, 1999). CPGs from any of these frameworks can experience the problems Falzer (2018) identifies when recommendations become requirements and nonconformance with them is penalized. However, physicians cannot simply be ordered to behave in a way that maximizes rewards or optimizes outcomes. The way they manage patients is based on long-established habit (Hamm, 2009a). Hearing about, reading, or studying an EB-CPG endorsed by a respected authority does not make physicians immediately change behavior, even if they intend to. They have to learn the recommended alternative behavior, recognize when the guideline describes something different from what they usually do, and make the conscious choice to change behavior. This must be done consciously until it can become a new habit (Abernathy & Hamm, 1995). This situation is more complex when the current habitual practice involves multiple actors (Ackerman, Gonzales, Stahl, & Metlay, 2013; Gonzales, Steiner, Lum, & Barrett, 1999), not just the individual doctor, and when the current practice pays better (Hamm, 2009b). Hence an individual physician may need to be strongly motivated to conform to a guideline even when he or she intellectually accepts it as correct. 782850 EDMXXX10.1177/1555343418782850Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision MakingJudgment and Guidelines 2018