气质对儿童嗅觉表现的影响:自我调节的作用

IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience
Lenka Martinec Nováková, Radka Vojtušová Mrzílková
{"title":"气质对儿童嗅觉表现的影响:自我调节的作用","authors":"Lenka Martinec Nováková,&nbsp;Radka Vojtušová Mrzílková","doi":"10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>A body of research predominantly in young adults has suggested a link between olfactory perception, especially sensitivity, and personality characteristics, particularly neuroticism. Nevertheless, it is not clear whether these associations are present outside this particular age range and whether they involve other olfactory abilities, such as odor identification and discrimination, and temperamental differences in reactivity and self-regulation. On the one hand, extrapolating from studies with adults to children, participants scoring high on negative affectivity, which broadly maps onto neuroticism, should outperform the low-scoring ones. On the other hand, well-developed self-regulatory processes, referred to as effortful control, which modulate the expression of such tendencies, might also contribute to better olfactory performance.</p><p>Thus, the aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of the temperamental factors on olfactory performance. Namely, we hypothesized that superior olfactory performance would be delivered by children scoring relatively higher on negative affectivity but also by those perceived as exhibiting greater self-regulation.</p><p>Odor identification and discrimination in 143 children (72 boys) aged 6 to 8 years were assessed with the Sniffin‘ Sticks, controlling for verbal fluency. Parents provided reports of their children‘s temperament by means of a short form of the Children‘s Behavior Questionnaire. The potential influence of parental responsiveness and demands on temperamental attributions was controlled for with hypothetical vignettes representing parenting styles.</p><p>There was an effect of effortful control (but not negative affectivity) on the total identification (but not discrimination) scores. Namely, children who were perceived as more capable of self-regulation exhibited higher odor identification scores. Girls did not outperform boys on either of the olfactory tests, but they were deemed by their parents to show greater self-regulation.</p><p>Our findings indirectly point to the effect of self-regulatory tendencies on odor identification in young children. However, they did not corroborate the idea that children varying in negative affectivity (underlying neuroticism) differ in terms of olfactory performance. Given the narrow age range of children recruited in the present study, further studies with preadolescent and adolescent participants are needed to gain more insight into the nature of these relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":"9 4","pages":"153 - 173"},"PeriodicalIF":1.0000,"publicationDate":"2016-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0","citationCount":"4","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Temperamental Influences on Children’s Olfactory Performance: the Role of Self-Regulation\",\"authors\":\"Lenka Martinec Nováková,&nbsp;Radka Vojtušová Mrzílková\",\"doi\":\"10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>A body of research predominantly in young adults has suggested a link between olfactory perception, especially sensitivity, and personality characteristics, particularly neuroticism. Nevertheless, it is not clear whether these associations are present outside this particular age range and whether they involve other olfactory abilities, such as odor identification and discrimination, and temperamental differences in reactivity and self-regulation. On the one hand, extrapolating from studies with adults to children, participants scoring high on negative affectivity, which broadly maps onto neuroticism, should outperform the low-scoring ones. On the other hand, well-developed self-regulatory processes, referred to as effortful control, which modulate the expression of such tendencies, might also contribute to better olfactory performance.</p><p>Thus, the aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of the temperamental factors on olfactory performance. Namely, we hypothesized that superior olfactory performance would be delivered by children scoring relatively higher on negative affectivity but also by those perceived as exhibiting greater self-regulation.</p><p>Odor identification and discrimination in 143 children (72 boys) aged 6 to 8 years were assessed with the Sniffin‘ Sticks, controlling for verbal fluency. Parents provided reports of their children‘s temperament by means of a short form of the Children‘s Behavior Questionnaire. The potential influence of parental responsiveness and demands on temperamental attributions was controlled for with hypothetical vignettes representing parenting styles.</p><p>There was an effect of effortful control (but not negative affectivity) on the total identification (but not discrimination) scores. Namely, children who were perceived as more capable of self-regulation exhibited higher odor identification scores. Girls did not outperform boys on either of the olfactory tests, but they were deemed by their parents to show greater self-regulation.</p><p>Our findings indirectly point to the effect of self-regulatory tendencies on odor identification in young children. However, they did not corroborate the idea that children varying in negative affectivity (underlying neuroticism) differ in terms of olfactory performance. Given the narrow age range of children recruited in the present study, further studies with preadolescent and adolescent participants are needed to gain more insight into the nature of these relationships.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":516,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Chemosensory Perception\",\"volume\":\"9 4\",\"pages\":\"153 - 173\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2016-10-04\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0\",\"citationCount\":\"4\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Chemosensory Perception\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q4\",\"JCRName\":\"Neuroscience\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Chemosensory Perception","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"Neuroscience","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4

摘要

一项主要针对年轻人的研究表明,嗅觉感知(尤其是敏感度)和个性特征(尤其是神经质)之间存在联系。然而,目前尚不清楚这些关联是否存在于这个特定年龄范围之外,以及它们是否涉及其他嗅觉能力,如气味识别和辨别,以及反应性和自我调节的气质差异。一方面,从成人到儿童的研究推断,负面情绪得分高的参与者(大致与神经质相关)应该比得分低的参与者表现更好。另一方面,发达的自我调节过程,即努力控制,调节这种倾向的表达,也可能有助于更好的嗅觉表现。因此,本研究的目的是探讨气质因素对嗅觉表现的影响。也就是说,我们假设,优秀的嗅觉表现不仅来自那些在消极情感上得分相对较高的孩子,也来自那些被认为表现出更强自我调节能力的孩子。用嗅探棒对143名6 ~ 8岁儿童(72名男孩)的气味识别和辨别能力进行了评估,并控制了语言流畅性。家长们通过儿童行为问卷的简短形式提供了他们孩子性情的报告。父母的反应和要求对气质归因的潜在影响被控制与假想的小插曲代表父母的方式。努力控制(但不影响消极情感)对总识别(但不影响歧视)得分有影响。也就是说,那些被认为更有自我调节能力的孩子表现出更高的气味识别分数。女孩在两项嗅觉测试中的表现都没有超过男孩,但他们的父母认为女孩表现出更强的自我调节能力。我们的研究结果间接指出了自我调节倾向对幼儿气味识别的影响。然而,他们并没有证实消极情绪(潜在的神经质)不同的儿童在嗅觉表现方面存在差异的观点。鉴于本研究招募的儿童年龄范围狭窄,需要对青春期前和青春期参与者进行进一步的研究,以更深入地了解这些关系的本质。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。

Temperamental Influences on Children’s Olfactory Performance: the Role of Self-Regulation

Temperamental Influences on Children’s Olfactory Performance: the Role of Self-Regulation

A body of research predominantly in young adults has suggested a link between olfactory perception, especially sensitivity, and personality characteristics, particularly neuroticism. Nevertheless, it is not clear whether these associations are present outside this particular age range and whether they involve other olfactory abilities, such as odor identification and discrimination, and temperamental differences in reactivity and self-regulation. On the one hand, extrapolating from studies with adults to children, participants scoring high on negative affectivity, which broadly maps onto neuroticism, should outperform the low-scoring ones. On the other hand, well-developed self-regulatory processes, referred to as effortful control, which modulate the expression of such tendencies, might also contribute to better olfactory performance.

Thus, the aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of the temperamental factors on olfactory performance. Namely, we hypothesized that superior olfactory performance would be delivered by children scoring relatively higher on negative affectivity but also by those perceived as exhibiting greater self-regulation.

Odor identification and discrimination in 143 children (72 boys) aged 6 to 8 years were assessed with the Sniffin‘ Sticks, controlling for verbal fluency. Parents provided reports of their children‘s temperament by means of a short form of the Children‘s Behavior Questionnaire. The potential influence of parental responsiveness and demands on temperamental attributions was controlled for with hypothetical vignettes representing parenting styles.

There was an effect of effortful control (but not negative affectivity) on the total identification (but not discrimination) scores. Namely, children who were perceived as more capable of self-regulation exhibited higher odor identification scores. Girls did not outperform boys on either of the olfactory tests, but they were deemed by their parents to show greater self-regulation.

Our findings indirectly point to the effect of self-regulatory tendencies on odor identification in young children. However, they did not corroborate the idea that children varying in negative affectivity (underlying neuroticism) differ in terms of olfactory performance. Given the narrow age range of children recruited in the present study, further studies with preadolescent and adolescent participants are needed to gain more insight into the nature of these relationships.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Chemosensory Perception
Chemosensory Perception 农林科学-神经科学
CiteScore
2.50
自引率
0.00%
发文量
8
审稿时长
>36 weeks
期刊介绍: Coverage in Chemosensory Perception includes animal work with implications for human phenomena and explores the following areas: Identification of chemicals producing sensory response; Identification of sensory response associated with chemicals; Human in vivo response to chemical stimuli; Human in vitro response to chemical stimuli; Neuroimaging of chemosensory function; Neurological processing of chemoreception; Chemoreception mechanisms; Psychophysics of chemoperception; Trigeminal function; Multisensory perception; Contextual effect on chemoperception; Behavioral response to chemical stimuli; Physiological factors affecting and contributing to chemoperception; Flavor and hedonics; Memory and chemoperception.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信