Michael R. Minicozzi , Shannon Hansen , Michael Akland , Ana Braga , Owen Kyle , C. Loren Buck , Miriam Ashley-Ross , Alice C. Gibb
{"title":"翻转还是翻转?鱼对搁浅的行为反应代表六目硬骨鱼","authors":"Michael R. Minicozzi , Shannon Hansen , Michael Akland , Ana Braga , Owen Kyle , C. Loren Buck , Miriam Ashley-Ross , Alice C. Gibb","doi":"10.1016/j.zool.2025.126294","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Aquatic and amphibious fishes produce a variety of behaviors when stranded on land, including a ballistic movement called a tail-flip jump. Although recent studies have examined jump performance (distance) in several cyprinodontiform species, less is known about the diversity of behaviors produced by teleosts when stranded on land. Here, seven species from four major teleost lineages (individual n = 4–12) were manually stranded in a terrestrial arena for two-minute trials. From videos of the trials, the number of jumps, flops (movements <1 body length), displacement (total and mean), latency (time to onset of movement), and percent time moving were quantified for each individual. A principal components analysis of these variables was used to define four “behavioral spaces:” species that produced effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (<em>Danio</em>); species that produced less effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (<em>Pseudomugil</em>); species that produced less effective displacement and spent little time moving (<em>Umbra</em>); and species that produced effective displacement, but spent little time moving (<em>Kryptolebias</em>). Based on the literature and metabolic data collected for this study, it appears that the ability to extract oxygen from air (vs. water) predicts the amount of time spent moving when stranded on land. Air-breathing fishes appear to adopt a “wait and see strategy,” while species that lack the ability to breathe air move immediately and frequently, likely an attempt to return to the water as quickly as possible.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":49330,"journal":{"name":"Zoology","volume":"172 ","pages":"Article 126294"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6000,"publicationDate":"2025-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Flip or flop? Behavioral response to stranding in fish representing six teleost orders\",\"authors\":\"Michael R. Minicozzi , Shannon Hansen , Michael Akland , Ana Braga , Owen Kyle , C. Loren Buck , Miriam Ashley-Ross , Alice C. Gibb\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.zool.2025.126294\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>Aquatic and amphibious fishes produce a variety of behaviors when stranded on land, including a ballistic movement called a tail-flip jump. Although recent studies have examined jump performance (distance) in several cyprinodontiform species, less is known about the diversity of behaviors produced by teleosts when stranded on land. Here, seven species from four major teleost lineages (individual n = 4–12) were manually stranded in a terrestrial arena for two-minute trials. From videos of the trials, the number of jumps, flops (movements <1 body length), displacement (total and mean), latency (time to onset of movement), and percent time moving were quantified for each individual. A principal components analysis of these variables was used to define four “behavioral spaces:” species that produced effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (<em>Danio</em>); species that produced less effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (<em>Pseudomugil</em>); species that produced less effective displacement and spent little time moving (<em>Umbra</em>); and species that produced effective displacement, but spent little time moving (<em>Kryptolebias</em>). Based on the literature and metabolic data collected for this study, it appears that the ability to extract oxygen from air (vs. water) predicts the amount of time spent moving when stranded on land. Air-breathing fishes appear to adopt a “wait and see strategy,” while species that lack the ability to breathe air move immediately and frequently, likely an attempt to return to the water as quickly as possible.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":49330,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Zoology\",\"volume\":\"172 \",\"pages\":\"Article 126294\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.6000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-09-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Zoology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"99\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0944200625000583\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"生物学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"ZOOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Zoology","FirstCategoryId":"99","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0944200625000583","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"ZOOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
Flip or flop? Behavioral response to stranding in fish representing six teleost orders
Aquatic and amphibious fishes produce a variety of behaviors when stranded on land, including a ballistic movement called a tail-flip jump. Although recent studies have examined jump performance (distance) in several cyprinodontiform species, less is known about the diversity of behaviors produced by teleosts when stranded on land. Here, seven species from four major teleost lineages (individual n = 4–12) were manually stranded in a terrestrial arena for two-minute trials. From videos of the trials, the number of jumps, flops (movements <1 body length), displacement (total and mean), latency (time to onset of movement), and percent time moving were quantified for each individual. A principal components analysis of these variables was used to define four “behavioral spaces:” species that produced effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (Danio); species that produced less effective displacement and spent the majority of the trial period moving (Pseudomugil); species that produced less effective displacement and spent little time moving (Umbra); and species that produced effective displacement, but spent little time moving (Kryptolebias). Based on the literature and metabolic data collected for this study, it appears that the ability to extract oxygen from air (vs. water) predicts the amount of time spent moving when stranded on land. Air-breathing fishes appear to adopt a “wait and see strategy,” while species that lack the ability to breathe air move immediately and frequently, likely an attempt to return to the water as quickly as possible.
期刊介绍:
Zoology is a journal devoted to experimental and comparative animal science. It presents a common forum for all scientists who take an explicitly organism oriented and integrative approach to the study of animal form, function, development and evolution.
The journal invites papers that take a comparative or experimental approach to behavior and neurobiology, functional morphology, evolution and development, ecological physiology, and cell biology. Due to the increasing realization that animals exist only within a partnership with symbionts, Zoology encourages submissions of papers focused on the analysis of holobionts or metaorganisms as associations of the macroscopic host in synergistic interdependence with numerous microbial and eukaryotic species.
The editors and the editorial board are committed to presenting science at its best. The editorial team is regularly adjusting editorial practice to the ever changing field of animal biology.