Paul Okyere Omane, Adebola A. Isaiah, Reginald Akuoko Duah, Thierry Nazzi
{"title":"非洲持续语言习得研究述评Scaff等人(2025)","authors":"Paul Okyere Omane, Adebola A. Isaiah, Reginald Akuoko Duah, Thierry Nazzi","doi":"10.1111/desc.70063","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The study by Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) provides additional evidence for the biases toward WEIRD populations in child language acquisition research. While their findings are unsurprising, they emphasize the urgent need to diversify research samples in terms of speaker population, geography, language, and culture (see also Aravena-Bravo et al. <span>2023</span>; Cristia et al. <span>2023</span>; Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>; Singh et al. <span>2023</span>). Scaff and colleagues convincingly argue for greater diversity and generalizability in the field (see also Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>; Singh et al. <span>2023</span>), making a strong case for studying child language acquisition in more varied countries, more socioeconomically diverse populations (in terms of socioeconomic status [SES], education, and occupation), underrepresented rural communities, different family structures, and lesser-studied languages from more diverse linguistic families and including more bilingual and multilingual infants with more varied language combinations.</p><p>Some of the issues raised by Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) can be addressed by researchers in WEIRD countries, for example, by including or targeting infants from lower SES families, and infants growing in rural areas of industrialized countries (e.g., Gonzalez-Gomez et al. <span>2021</span>), which will not be discussed here. Rather, in this commentary, we argue that studying language acquisition in rural and urban communities in Africa is essential for diversifying research on child language acquisition and addressing several of the other issues raised in the target article (i.e., language diversity; bi/multilingualism; sociocultural diversity), taking Ghana and Nigeria as examples. We also offer some suggestions to help create developmental corpora from underrepresented languages and speaker populations and, more generally, foster more inclusive language acquisition research.</p><p>Regarding language diversity, Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) report a dominance of Indo-European languages, with English being the most prevalent among them. Although Africa is the most linguistically diverse continent, with approximately 2582 languages (Lodhi <span>1993</span>), it is striking that not a single indigenous African language—particularly from the Niger-Congo family, which is the largest with 1554 languages (Eberhard et al. <span>2025</span>)—is represented in the languages or language combinations (for bilinguals and multilinguals) analyzed by Scaff and colleagues. This shows the extent of linguistic, geographical, and cultural biases in the data underlying most language acquisition theories. Corpus and experimental data from Ghana and Nigeria, where about 73 and 520 languages are spoken, respectively (Eberhard et al. <span>2025</span>), could contribute to expanding diversity and our understanding of language acquisition processes and to fostering more generalizable theories. While efforts to build some corpora of endangered languages are currently being conducted (e.g., the Pangloss collection, which includes recordings of 258 corpora of underrepresented languages and dialects from 46 countries, https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/?lang = en; Adamou et al. <span>2025</span>; Michailovsky et al. <span>2014</span>), such efforts should be extended to language acquisition corpora and experimental research. Note that one difficulty in doing so is that for some of the world's languages spoken in small communities of less than a few thousand speakers, adults have stopped transmitting these languages to their children. Examples include the 14 Ghana-Togo Mountain languages, namely Adele, Animere, Akebu, Ikposo, Lelemi, Igo, Tuwuli, Siwu, Sɛlɛɛ, Sɛkpɛle, Ikpana, Siya(sɛ), Nyagbo, and Tafi (Dakubu and Ford <span>1988</span>; Agyekum <span>2010</span>; Ameka <span>2017</span>), spoken in southeastern Ghana and parts of Togo; and Kokomba, Tokoshi, Dindi, and Lukawa, all spoken in Kwara North, Nigeria (Lawal et al. <span>2016</span>).</p><p>Second, Scaff and colleagues also report an overrepresentation of monolinguals in the CHILDES corpora. Again, studying language acquisition in Africa would help address this point, as children in many African contexts are raised in bilingual/multilingual homes and communities and are regularly exposed to multiple languages. This is true in both rural and urban areas, where multilingualism is a common aspect of daily life (e.g., Ghana: Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>; Nigeria: Isaiah <span>2023</span>). For instance, in Accra (Ghana), infants between 3 and 12 months hear between 2 and 6 different languages in their environment, most of which are indigenous Ghanaian languages (Omane et al. <span>2025</span>), while children aged 6 to 10 years and adults speak between 2 and 6 and 2 and 8 languages, respectively (Bodomo et al. <span>2010</span>), suggesting the early and sustained multilingual experiences of children in Ghana. Similarly, in Nigeria, children often acquire two to three languages because of the environment in which they are raised (Bamgbose <span>1991</span>). Moreover, the total number of language combinations (<i>n</i> = 22) in the bilingual corpora analyzed by Scaff and colleagues is fewer than the unique language combinations (<i>n</i> = 32) found in the multilingual sample of Omane et al. (<span>2025</span>), who studied Ghanaian infants in Accra. If we are serious about including more bilingual/multilingual children in our research samples, we must look beyond WEIRD contexts to underrepresented African settings.</p><p>Third, studying rural and urban African communities such as those found in Ghana and Nigeria could contribute to efforts to diversify the language acquisition field along cultural dimensions (Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>), given the sociocultural environments in which infants grow up in these countries. In Ghana and Nigeria, caregiving practices and family structures expose children to a wide range of experiences, as they interact with multiple caregivers, including members of the nuclear family (mother, father, siblings), extended relatives (e.g., grandparents, aunts, uncles), and non-relatives (e.g., co-tenants, neighbors) (Igboanusi and Peter <span>2005</span>; Isaiah <span>2023</span>; Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>; Omane et al. <span>2025</span>). These multi-caregiver arrangements are closely tied to the types of housing in which children are raised. For example, in a compound house in Ghana—a structure that accommodates multiple households within a single compound—residents often include several nuclear and extended families who share common spaces such as playgrounds and cooking areas (Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>). In these communities, the socioeconomic status (SES) range is also varied. In many Ghanaian and Nigerian communities, especially in rural areas, language socialization is viewed as a communal responsibility, involving not just parents and older siblings but also extended relatives and community members. Children in these settings are cared for by various members of the compound, and our understanding of language acquisition would benefit from documenting this multi-caregiver household structure, investigating how it may provide children with a form of socialization that may differ from what is experienced by children in populations that dominate current language acquisition research, and how it may impact language acquisition trajectories.</p><p>Developing a language acquisition science that is not Western-centered and includes underrepresented communities and speaker populations is undoubtedly possible. We provide some steps that can help achieve this goal.</p><p>First, funding agencies must invest in this enterprise. Conducting language acquisition research involves a significant amount of work and also requires financial commitment. Given that researchers or local people in underrepresented communities in Africa may not have the financial capacity to undertake this important activity, we encourage funding agencies to support data collection and language acquisition research in these areas.</p><p>Second, language acquisition researchers must collaborate with experts in other disciplines, such as language documentation, ethnography, and sociolinguistics. This kind of cross-disciplinary work can help foster experimental work and corpora creation in understudied communities. In addition, we also encourage collaboration between researchers from WEIRD and non-Western contexts, as well as the local people. This could extend beyond an authorship role to include providing training to one another, which may involve data collection approaches in local communities and guidance on how to engage with community participants (e.g., a few cultural dos and don'ts; see Omane et al. <span>2023</span>).</p><p>Third, we encourage researchers to provide international mentorship opportunities to students from underrepresented communities (see Aravena-Bravo et al. <span>2023</span>) who have the desire and passion to conduct research in language acquisition but lack access to experts in these fields at their local universities.</p><p>Fourth, encourage fieldwork. Most of what we know so far has primarily come from studies conducted in mainstream lab settings, predominantly in the WEIRD countries. Our efforts at diversification in naturalistic language research can benefit from undertaking fieldwork or expanding our research to underrepresented rural and urban communities.</p><p>Finally, based on our field-based experimental research in Ghana and Nigeria, we have found that most community members have little to no knowledge of language development research. This lack of awareness significantly hinders participant recruitment and parents’ willingness to participate in our research. In our effort to promote inclusivity across cultures, populations, and languages, researchers working in underrepresented communities must provide meaningful feedback about their work. This is crucial, as the nature and impact of our research are often not immediately apparent to the communities we engage with.</p><p>In conclusion, as a field, our efforts to include language and population diversity will gain from focusing on children and languages in non-WEIRD countries. Based on the examples of Ghana and Nigeria, with their rich linguistic diversity and in which bilingualism and multilingualism are the norm (typically not driven by immigration but rather being a lived, everyday reality), we argued that including underrepresented populations from Africa can help reduce biases, promote cross-linguistic research, and support the creation of more diverse research samples, thereby fostering the generalizability of our research findings.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"28 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2000,"publicationDate":"2025-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.70063","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Sustaining Language Acquisition Research in Africa: A Commentary on Scaff et al. (2025)\",\"authors\":\"Paul Okyere Omane, Adebola A. Isaiah, Reginald Akuoko Duah, Thierry Nazzi\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/desc.70063\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>The study by Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) provides additional evidence for the biases toward WEIRD populations in child language acquisition research. While their findings are unsurprising, they emphasize the urgent need to diversify research samples in terms of speaker population, geography, language, and culture (see also Aravena-Bravo et al. <span>2023</span>; Cristia et al. <span>2023</span>; Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>; Singh et al. <span>2023</span>). Scaff and colleagues convincingly argue for greater diversity and generalizability in the field (see also Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>; Singh et al. <span>2023</span>), making a strong case for studying child language acquisition in more varied countries, more socioeconomically diverse populations (in terms of socioeconomic status [SES], education, and occupation), underrepresented rural communities, different family structures, and lesser-studied languages from more diverse linguistic families and including more bilingual and multilingual infants with more varied language combinations.</p><p>Some of the issues raised by Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) can be addressed by researchers in WEIRD countries, for example, by including or targeting infants from lower SES families, and infants growing in rural areas of industrialized countries (e.g., Gonzalez-Gomez et al. <span>2021</span>), which will not be discussed here. Rather, in this commentary, we argue that studying language acquisition in rural and urban communities in Africa is essential for diversifying research on child language acquisition and addressing several of the other issues raised in the target article (i.e., language diversity; bi/multilingualism; sociocultural diversity), taking Ghana and Nigeria as examples. We also offer some suggestions to help create developmental corpora from underrepresented languages and speaker populations and, more generally, foster more inclusive language acquisition research.</p><p>Regarding language diversity, Scaff et al. (<span>2025</span>) report a dominance of Indo-European languages, with English being the most prevalent among them. Although Africa is the most linguistically diverse continent, with approximately 2582 languages (Lodhi <span>1993</span>), it is striking that not a single indigenous African language—particularly from the Niger-Congo family, which is the largest with 1554 languages (Eberhard et al. <span>2025</span>)—is represented in the languages or language combinations (for bilinguals and multilinguals) analyzed by Scaff and colleagues. This shows the extent of linguistic, geographical, and cultural biases in the data underlying most language acquisition theories. Corpus and experimental data from Ghana and Nigeria, where about 73 and 520 languages are spoken, respectively (Eberhard et al. <span>2025</span>), could contribute to expanding diversity and our understanding of language acquisition processes and to fostering more generalizable theories. While efforts to build some corpora of endangered languages are currently being conducted (e.g., the Pangloss collection, which includes recordings of 258 corpora of underrepresented languages and dialects from 46 countries, https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/?lang = en; Adamou et al. <span>2025</span>; Michailovsky et al. <span>2014</span>), such efforts should be extended to language acquisition corpora and experimental research. Note that one difficulty in doing so is that for some of the world's languages spoken in small communities of less than a few thousand speakers, adults have stopped transmitting these languages to their children. Examples include the 14 Ghana-Togo Mountain languages, namely Adele, Animere, Akebu, Ikposo, Lelemi, Igo, Tuwuli, Siwu, Sɛlɛɛ, Sɛkpɛle, Ikpana, Siya(sɛ), Nyagbo, and Tafi (Dakubu and Ford <span>1988</span>; Agyekum <span>2010</span>; Ameka <span>2017</span>), spoken in southeastern Ghana and parts of Togo; and Kokomba, Tokoshi, Dindi, and Lukawa, all spoken in Kwara North, Nigeria (Lawal et al. <span>2016</span>).</p><p>Second, Scaff and colleagues also report an overrepresentation of monolinguals in the CHILDES corpora. Again, studying language acquisition in Africa would help address this point, as children in many African contexts are raised in bilingual/multilingual homes and communities and are regularly exposed to multiple languages. This is true in both rural and urban areas, where multilingualism is a common aspect of daily life (e.g., Ghana: Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>; Nigeria: Isaiah <span>2023</span>). For instance, in Accra (Ghana), infants between 3 and 12 months hear between 2 and 6 different languages in their environment, most of which are indigenous Ghanaian languages (Omane et al. <span>2025</span>), while children aged 6 to 10 years and adults speak between 2 and 6 and 2 and 8 languages, respectively (Bodomo et al. <span>2010</span>), suggesting the early and sustained multilingual experiences of children in Ghana. Similarly, in Nigeria, children often acquire two to three languages because of the environment in which they are raised (Bamgbose <span>1991</span>). Moreover, the total number of language combinations (<i>n</i> = 22) in the bilingual corpora analyzed by Scaff and colleagues is fewer than the unique language combinations (<i>n</i> = 32) found in the multilingual sample of Omane et al. (<span>2025</span>), who studied Ghanaian infants in Accra. If we are serious about including more bilingual/multilingual children in our research samples, we must look beyond WEIRD contexts to underrepresented African settings.</p><p>Third, studying rural and urban African communities such as those found in Ghana and Nigeria could contribute to efforts to diversify the language acquisition field along cultural dimensions (Kidd and Garcia <span>2022</span>), given the sociocultural environments in which infants grow up in these countries. In Ghana and Nigeria, caregiving practices and family structures expose children to a wide range of experiences, as they interact with multiple caregivers, including members of the nuclear family (mother, father, siblings), extended relatives (e.g., grandparents, aunts, uncles), and non-relatives (e.g., co-tenants, neighbors) (Igboanusi and Peter <span>2005</span>; Isaiah <span>2023</span>; Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>; Omane et al. <span>2025</span>). These multi-caregiver arrangements are closely tied to the types of housing in which children are raised. For example, in a compound house in Ghana—a structure that accommodates multiple households within a single compound—residents often include several nuclear and extended families who share common spaces such as playgrounds and cooking areas (Nutakor and Amfo <span>2018</span>). In these communities, the socioeconomic status (SES) range is also varied. In many Ghanaian and Nigerian communities, especially in rural areas, language socialization is viewed as a communal responsibility, involving not just parents and older siblings but also extended relatives and community members. Children in these settings are cared for by various members of the compound, and our understanding of language acquisition would benefit from documenting this multi-caregiver household structure, investigating how it may provide children with a form of socialization that may differ from what is experienced by children in populations that dominate current language acquisition research, and how it may impact language acquisition trajectories.</p><p>Developing a language acquisition science that is not Western-centered and includes underrepresented communities and speaker populations is undoubtedly possible. We provide some steps that can help achieve this goal.</p><p>First, funding agencies must invest in this enterprise. Conducting language acquisition research involves a significant amount of work and also requires financial commitment. Given that researchers or local people in underrepresented communities in Africa may not have the financial capacity to undertake this important activity, we encourage funding agencies to support data collection and language acquisition research in these areas.</p><p>Second, language acquisition researchers must collaborate with experts in other disciplines, such as language documentation, ethnography, and sociolinguistics. This kind of cross-disciplinary work can help foster experimental work and corpora creation in understudied communities. In addition, we also encourage collaboration between researchers from WEIRD and non-Western contexts, as well as the local people. This could extend beyond an authorship role to include providing training to one another, which may involve data collection approaches in local communities and guidance on how to engage with community participants (e.g., a few cultural dos and don'ts; see Omane et al. <span>2023</span>).</p><p>Third, we encourage researchers to provide international mentorship opportunities to students from underrepresented communities (see Aravena-Bravo et al. <span>2023</span>) who have the desire and passion to conduct research in language acquisition but lack access to experts in these fields at their local universities.</p><p>Fourth, encourage fieldwork. Most of what we know so far has primarily come from studies conducted in mainstream lab settings, predominantly in the WEIRD countries. Our efforts at diversification in naturalistic language research can benefit from undertaking fieldwork or expanding our research to underrepresented rural and urban communities.</p><p>Finally, based on our field-based experimental research in Ghana and Nigeria, we have found that most community members have little to no knowledge of language development research. This lack of awareness significantly hinders participant recruitment and parents’ willingness to participate in our research. In our effort to promote inclusivity across cultures, populations, and languages, researchers working in underrepresented communities must provide meaningful feedback about their work. This is crucial, as the nature and impact of our research are often not immediately apparent to the communities we engage with.</p><p>In conclusion, as a field, our efforts to include language and population diversity will gain from focusing on children and languages in non-WEIRD countries. Based on the examples of Ghana and Nigeria, with their rich linguistic diversity and in which bilingualism and multilingualism are the norm (typically not driven by immigration but rather being a lived, everyday reality), we argued that including underrepresented populations from Africa can help reduce biases, promote cross-linguistic research, and support the creation of more diverse research samples, thereby fostering the generalizability of our research findings.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":48392,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Developmental Science\",\"volume\":\"28 5\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.2000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-08-18\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.70063\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Developmental Science\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/desc.70063\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Developmental Science","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/desc.70063","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
摘要
Scaff et al.(2025)的研究为儿童语言习得研究中对WEIRD人群的偏见提供了额外的证据。虽然他们的发现并不令人惊讶,但他们强调迫切需要在说话者人口、地理、语言和文化方面使研究样本多样化(另见Aravena-Bravo等人,2023;Cristia等人,2023;Kidd和Garcia, 2022; Singh等人,2023)。Scaff及其同事令人信服地论证了该领域更大的多样性和普遍性(另见Kidd和Garcia 2022;Singh et al. 2023),为在更多样化的国家、社会经济更多样化的人口(在社会经济地位[SES]、教育和职业方面)、代表性不足的农村社区、不同的家庭结构、来自更多样化的语言家庭的较少研究的语言以及包括更多双语和多语婴儿的语言组合更多样化的情况下研究儿童语言习得提供了强有力的理由。Scaff et al.(2025)提出的一些问题可以由WEIRD国家的研究人员解决,例如,通过包括或针对来自较低社会经济地位家庭的婴儿,以及在工业化国家农村地区长大的婴儿(例如Gonzalez-Gomez et al. 2021),这里不再讨论。相反,在这篇评论中,我们认为研究非洲农村和城市社区的语言习得对于多样化儿童语言习得研究和解决目标文章中提出的其他几个问题(即语言多样性、双/多语言、社会文化多样性)至关重要,并以加纳和尼日利亚为例。我们还提供了一些建议,以帮助从代表性不足的语言和说话人群中创建发展性语料库,更广泛地说,促进更具包容性的语言习得研究。关于语言多样性,Scaff等人(2025)报告了印欧语言的主导地位,英语是其中最普遍的。尽管非洲是语言最多样化的大陆,大约有2582种语言(Lodhi 1993),但令人惊讶的是,在Scaff及其同事分析的语言或语言组合(双语和多语)中,没有一种非洲本土语言——特别是来自尼日尔-刚果语系的语言,它是最大的,有1554种语言(Eberhard et al. 2025)。这显示了大多数语言习得理论背后的数据中存在的语言、地理和文化偏见的程度。来自加纳和尼日利亚的语料库和实验数据,这两个国家分别使用大约73种和520种语言(Eberhard et al. 2025),可以有助于扩大多样性和我们对语言习得过程的理解,并促进更普遍的理论。虽然目前正在努力建立一些濒危语言的语料库(例如Pangloss收集,其中包括来自46个国家的258个代表性不足的语言和方言语料库的记录,https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/?lang = en; Adamou et al. 2025; Michailovsky et al. 2014),但这种努力应该扩展到语言学习语料库和实验研究。请注意,这样做的一个困难是,对于世界上一些在不到几千人的小社区中使用的语言,成年人已经停止向他们的孩子传播这些语言。例子包括14种加纳-多哥山地语言,即阿黛尔、阿尼米尔、阿克布、伊克波索、莱莱米、伊戈、图乌里、西乌、S æ l æ le、S æ kp æ le、伊克帕纳、西亚(S æ)、尼亚博和塔菲(Dakubu和Ford 1988; Agyekum 2010; Ameka 2017),在加纳东南部和多哥部分地区使用;以及Kokomba, Tokoshi, Dindi和Lukawa,都是尼日利亚Kwara北部的语言(Lawal et al. 2016)。其次,Scaff和他的同事还报告了单语者在CHILDES语料库中的过度代表。同样,在非洲研究语言习得将有助于解决这一问题,因为非洲的许多儿童都是在双语/多语家庭和社区中长大的,并且经常接触多种语言。在农村和城市地区都是如此,使用多种语言是日常生活的一个普遍方面(例如,加纳:Nutakor and Amfo 2018;尼日利亚:以赛亚书2023)。例如,在加纳的阿克拉,3 - 12个月大的婴儿在他们的环境中会听到2 - 6种不同的语言,其中大多数是加纳本土语言(Omane et al. 2025),而6 - 10岁的儿童和成年人分别会说2 - 6种语言和2 - 8种语言(Bodomo et al. 2010),这表明加纳儿童早期和持续的多语言经历。同样,在尼日利亚,由于儿童成长的环境,儿童往往掌握两到三种语言(Bamgbose 1991)。 总之,作为一个领域,我们在语言和人口多样性方面的努力将从关注非怪异国家的儿童和语言中获益。基于加纳和尼日利亚的例子,他们拥有丰富的语言多样性,双语和多语是常态(通常不是由移民驱动的,而是生活的,日常的现实),我们认为,包括来自非洲的代表性不足的人口可以帮助减少偏见,促进跨语言研究,并支持创建更多样化的研究样本,从而促进我们的研究结果的普遍性。作者声明无利益冲突。
Sustaining Language Acquisition Research in Africa: A Commentary on Scaff et al. (2025)
The study by Scaff et al. (2025) provides additional evidence for the biases toward WEIRD populations in child language acquisition research. While their findings are unsurprising, they emphasize the urgent need to diversify research samples in terms of speaker population, geography, language, and culture (see also Aravena-Bravo et al. 2023; Cristia et al. 2023; Kidd and Garcia 2022; Singh et al. 2023). Scaff and colleagues convincingly argue for greater diversity and generalizability in the field (see also Kidd and Garcia 2022; Singh et al. 2023), making a strong case for studying child language acquisition in more varied countries, more socioeconomically diverse populations (in terms of socioeconomic status [SES], education, and occupation), underrepresented rural communities, different family structures, and lesser-studied languages from more diverse linguistic families and including more bilingual and multilingual infants with more varied language combinations.
Some of the issues raised by Scaff et al. (2025) can be addressed by researchers in WEIRD countries, for example, by including or targeting infants from lower SES families, and infants growing in rural areas of industrialized countries (e.g., Gonzalez-Gomez et al. 2021), which will not be discussed here. Rather, in this commentary, we argue that studying language acquisition in rural and urban communities in Africa is essential for diversifying research on child language acquisition and addressing several of the other issues raised in the target article (i.e., language diversity; bi/multilingualism; sociocultural diversity), taking Ghana and Nigeria as examples. We also offer some suggestions to help create developmental corpora from underrepresented languages and speaker populations and, more generally, foster more inclusive language acquisition research.
Regarding language diversity, Scaff et al. (2025) report a dominance of Indo-European languages, with English being the most prevalent among them. Although Africa is the most linguistically diverse continent, with approximately 2582 languages (Lodhi 1993), it is striking that not a single indigenous African language—particularly from the Niger-Congo family, which is the largest with 1554 languages (Eberhard et al. 2025)—is represented in the languages or language combinations (for bilinguals and multilinguals) analyzed by Scaff and colleagues. This shows the extent of linguistic, geographical, and cultural biases in the data underlying most language acquisition theories. Corpus and experimental data from Ghana and Nigeria, where about 73 and 520 languages are spoken, respectively (Eberhard et al. 2025), could contribute to expanding diversity and our understanding of language acquisition processes and to fostering more generalizable theories. While efforts to build some corpora of endangered languages are currently being conducted (e.g., the Pangloss collection, which includes recordings of 258 corpora of underrepresented languages and dialects from 46 countries, https://pangloss.cnrs.fr/?lang = en; Adamou et al. 2025; Michailovsky et al. 2014), such efforts should be extended to language acquisition corpora and experimental research. Note that one difficulty in doing so is that for some of the world's languages spoken in small communities of less than a few thousand speakers, adults have stopped transmitting these languages to their children. Examples include the 14 Ghana-Togo Mountain languages, namely Adele, Animere, Akebu, Ikposo, Lelemi, Igo, Tuwuli, Siwu, Sɛlɛɛ, Sɛkpɛle, Ikpana, Siya(sɛ), Nyagbo, and Tafi (Dakubu and Ford 1988; Agyekum 2010; Ameka 2017), spoken in southeastern Ghana and parts of Togo; and Kokomba, Tokoshi, Dindi, and Lukawa, all spoken in Kwara North, Nigeria (Lawal et al. 2016).
Second, Scaff and colleagues also report an overrepresentation of monolinguals in the CHILDES corpora. Again, studying language acquisition in Africa would help address this point, as children in many African contexts are raised in bilingual/multilingual homes and communities and are regularly exposed to multiple languages. This is true in both rural and urban areas, where multilingualism is a common aspect of daily life (e.g., Ghana: Nutakor and Amfo 2018; Nigeria: Isaiah 2023). For instance, in Accra (Ghana), infants between 3 and 12 months hear between 2 and 6 different languages in their environment, most of which are indigenous Ghanaian languages (Omane et al. 2025), while children aged 6 to 10 years and adults speak between 2 and 6 and 2 and 8 languages, respectively (Bodomo et al. 2010), suggesting the early and sustained multilingual experiences of children in Ghana. Similarly, in Nigeria, children often acquire two to three languages because of the environment in which they are raised (Bamgbose 1991). Moreover, the total number of language combinations (n = 22) in the bilingual corpora analyzed by Scaff and colleagues is fewer than the unique language combinations (n = 32) found in the multilingual sample of Omane et al. (2025), who studied Ghanaian infants in Accra. If we are serious about including more bilingual/multilingual children in our research samples, we must look beyond WEIRD contexts to underrepresented African settings.
Third, studying rural and urban African communities such as those found in Ghana and Nigeria could contribute to efforts to diversify the language acquisition field along cultural dimensions (Kidd and Garcia 2022), given the sociocultural environments in which infants grow up in these countries. In Ghana and Nigeria, caregiving practices and family structures expose children to a wide range of experiences, as they interact with multiple caregivers, including members of the nuclear family (mother, father, siblings), extended relatives (e.g., grandparents, aunts, uncles), and non-relatives (e.g., co-tenants, neighbors) (Igboanusi and Peter 2005; Isaiah 2023; Nutakor and Amfo 2018; Omane et al. 2025). These multi-caregiver arrangements are closely tied to the types of housing in which children are raised. For example, in a compound house in Ghana—a structure that accommodates multiple households within a single compound—residents often include several nuclear and extended families who share common spaces such as playgrounds and cooking areas (Nutakor and Amfo 2018). In these communities, the socioeconomic status (SES) range is also varied. In many Ghanaian and Nigerian communities, especially in rural areas, language socialization is viewed as a communal responsibility, involving not just parents and older siblings but also extended relatives and community members. Children in these settings are cared for by various members of the compound, and our understanding of language acquisition would benefit from documenting this multi-caregiver household structure, investigating how it may provide children with a form of socialization that may differ from what is experienced by children in populations that dominate current language acquisition research, and how it may impact language acquisition trajectories.
Developing a language acquisition science that is not Western-centered and includes underrepresented communities and speaker populations is undoubtedly possible. We provide some steps that can help achieve this goal.
First, funding agencies must invest in this enterprise. Conducting language acquisition research involves a significant amount of work and also requires financial commitment. Given that researchers or local people in underrepresented communities in Africa may not have the financial capacity to undertake this important activity, we encourage funding agencies to support data collection and language acquisition research in these areas.
Second, language acquisition researchers must collaborate with experts in other disciplines, such as language documentation, ethnography, and sociolinguistics. This kind of cross-disciplinary work can help foster experimental work and corpora creation in understudied communities. In addition, we also encourage collaboration between researchers from WEIRD and non-Western contexts, as well as the local people. This could extend beyond an authorship role to include providing training to one another, which may involve data collection approaches in local communities and guidance on how to engage with community participants (e.g., a few cultural dos and don'ts; see Omane et al. 2023).
Third, we encourage researchers to provide international mentorship opportunities to students from underrepresented communities (see Aravena-Bravo et al. 2023) who have the desire and passion to conduct research in language acquisition but lack access to experts in these fields at their local universities.
Fourth, encourage fieldwork. Most of what we know so far has primarily come from studies conducted in mainstream lab settings, predominantly in the WEIRD countries. Our efforts at diversification in naturalistic language research can benefit from undertaking fieldwork or expanding our research to underrepresented rural and urban communities.
Finally, based on our field-based experimental research in Ghana and Nigeria, we have found that most community members have little to no knowledge of language development research. This lack of awareness significantly hinders participant recruitment and parents’ willingness to participate in our research. In our effort to promote inclusivity across cultures, populations, and languages, researchers working in underrepresented communities must provide meaningful feedback about their work. This is crucial, as the nature and impact of our research are often not immediately apparent to the communities we engage with.
In conclusion, as a field, our efforts to include language and population diversity will gain from focusing on children and languages in non-WEIRD countries. Based on the examples of Ghana and Nigeria, with their rich linguistic diversity and in which bilingualism and multilingualism are the norm (typically not driven by immigration but rather being a lived, everyday reality), we argued that including underrepresented populations from Africa can help reduce biases, promote cross-linguistic research, and support the creation of more diverse research samples, thereby fostering the generalizability of our research findings.
期刊介绍:
Developmental Science publishes cutting-edge theory and up-to-the-minute research on scientific developmental psychology from leading thinkers in the field. It is currently the only journal that specifically focuses on human developmental cognitive neuroscience. Coverage includes: - Clinical, computational and comparative approaches to development - Key advances in cognitive and social development - Developmental cognitive neuroscience - Functional neuroimaging of the developing brain