Berenice Johnston, Brooke Ryan, Megan Hatfield, Samuel D. Calder, Mary Claessen
{"title":"写作前干预的有效性:一项先导随机对照试验","authors":"Berenice Johnston, Brooke Ryan, Megan Hatfield, Samuel D. Calder, Mary Claessen","doi":"10.1111/1440-1630.70043","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div>\n \n \n <section>\n \n <h3> Introduction</h3>\n \n <p>Successful handwriting is dependent on accurate and efficient letter formation, which is dependent on drawing sub-strokes of letters and prewriting patterns. Currently, there is no prewriting intervention programmes with established efficacy, and little is known about children's perceptions of engaging in these programmes. This study aimed to determine the efficacy and feasibility of a prewriting intervention.</p>\n </section>\n \n <section>\n \n <h3> Methods</h3>\n \n <p>A pilot randomised control trial was conducted with embedded aspects of fidelity and acceptability. Participants included 18 typically developing 4- to 5-year-old children, attending a Western Australian kindergarten (year before first formal schooling year), randomly allocated to an intervention or waitlist control group. Baseline and post intervention data were collected using the Developmental Test of Visual Perception (Third Edition) and the Prewriting Assessment (PWA). Participants received six Peggy Lego intervention sessions, and a fidelity checklist was completed following each session. Immediately following completion of the intervention, participants provided acceptability feedback using a modified Likert scale.</p>\n </section>\n \n <section>\n \n <h3> Consumer and Community Involvement</h3>\n \n <p>Teachers and occupational therapists working with 4- to 5-year-old children provided feedback on the intervention.</p>\n </section>\n \n <section>\n \n <h3> Results</h3>\n \n <p>There was a statistically significant main effect of time on the PWA score (<i>p</i> = 0.003); however, the main effect of group and the interaction of group and time were non-significant (<i>p</i> = 0.070 and <i>p</i> = 0.46). The intervention was implemented with high levels of fidelity with 19 sessions (<i>n</i> = 34) achieving 100% fidelity. Eligibility was deemed feasible with 60% of those enrolled for the study meeting eligibility criteria. Most participants (<i>n</i> = 17) completed six intervention sessions. Most participants found the intervention acceptable (<i>n</i> = 13).</p>\n </section>\n \n <section>\n \n <h3> Conclusions</h3>\n \n <p>Exploratory analysis showed all children significantly improved their prewriting ability; however, it is likely that this effect is not attributed to intervention alone. This pilot randomised control trial is deemed feasible in terms of recruitment, retention of participants, and data collection. Further research on the efficacy of this intervention is justified.</p>\n </section>\n \n <section>\n \n <h3> PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY</h3>\n \n <p>For writing to be easy to read, students need to form letters the right way and with good control. Before they can do this, they need to learn basic pencil strokes used to make letters. Handwriting programmes are used in schools and with children who find writing hard. We know some of these programmes help, but we do not know which prewriting programmes work best. We wanted to find out if a programme called Peggy Lego helps and if it is easy to use. We did six Peggy Lego sessions with 18 kindergarten children in Western Australia. All of the children got better at their prewriting skills, but we could not tell if Peggy Lego helped more than routine teaching, or if the children improved over time. We asked the students how they felt about the programme. Most said they liked it and thought their drawing got better, even though it was a bit hard. Future research could look at whether feeling more confident helps children when they start learning to write letters.</p>\n </section>\n </div>","PeriodicalId":55418,"journal":{"name":"Australian Occupational Therapy Journal","volume":"72 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2025-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/1440-1630.70043","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Efficacy of a prewriting intervention: A pilot randomised control trial\",\"authors\":\"Berenice Johnston, Brooke Ryan, Megan Hatfield, Samuel D. Calder, Mary Claessen\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/1440-1630.70043\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div>\\n \\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> Introduction</h3>\\n \\n <p>Successful handwriting is dependent on accurate and efficient letter formation, which is dependent on drawing sub-strokes of letters and prewriting patterns. Currently, there is no prewriting intervention programmes with established efficacy, and little is known about children's perceptions of engaging in these programmes. This study aimed to determine the efficacy and feasibility of a prewriting intervention.</p>\\n </section>\\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> Methods</h3>\\n \\n <p>A pilot randomised control trial was conducted with embedded aspects of fidelity and acceptability. Participants included 18 typically developing 4- to 5-year-old children, attending a Western Australian kindergarten (year before first formal schooling year), randomly allocated to an intervention or waitlist control group. Baseline and post intervention data were collected using the Developmental Test of Visual Perception (Third Edition) and the Prewriting Assessment (PWA). Participants received six Peggy Lego intervention sessions, and a fidelity checklist was completed following each session. Immediately following completion of the intervention, participants provided acceptability feedback using a modified Likert scale.</p>\\n </section>\\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> Consumer and Community Involvement</h3>\\n \\n <p>Teachers and occupational therapists working with 4- to 5-year-old children provided feedback on the intervention.</p>\\n </section>\\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> Results</h3>\\n \\n <p>There was a statistically significant main effect of time on the PWA score (<i>p</i> = 0.003); however, the main effect of group and the interaction of group and time were non-significant (<i>p</i> = 0.070 and <i>p</i> = 0.46). The intervention was implemented with high levels of fidelity with 19 sessions (<i>n</i> = 34) achieving 100% fidelity. Eligibility was deemed feasible with 60% of those enrolled for the study meeting eligibility criteria. Most participants (<i>n</i> = 17) completed six intervention sessions. Most participants found the intervention acceptable (<i>n</i> = 13).</p>\\n </section>\\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> Conclusions</h3>\\n \\n <p>Exploratory analysis showed all children significantly improved their prewriting ability; however, it is likely that this effect is not attributed to intervention alone. This pilot randomised control trial is deemed feasible in terms of recruitment, retention of participants, and data collection. Further research on the efficacy of this intervention is justified.</p>\\n </section>\\n \\n <section>\\n \\n <h3> PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY</h3>\\n \\n <p>For writing to be easy to read, students need to form letters the right way and with good control. Before they can do this, they need to learn basic pencil strokes used to make letters. Handwriting programmes are used in schools and with children who find writing hard. We know some of these programmes help, but we do not know which prewriting programmes work best. We wanted to find out if a programme called Peggy Lego helps and if it is easy to use. We did six Peggy Lego sessions with 18 kindergarten children in Western Australia. All of the children got better at their prewriting skills, but we could not tell if Peggy Lego helped more than routine teaching, or if the children improved over time. We asked the students how they felt about the programme. Most said they liked it and thought their drawing got better, even though it was a bit hard. Future research could look at whether feeling more confident helps children when they start learning to write letters.</p>\\n </section>\\n </div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":55418,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Australian Occupational Therapy Journal\",\"volume\":\"72 4\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-08-12\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/1440-1630.70043\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Australian Occupational Therapy Journal\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1440-1630.70043\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"REHABILITATION\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Australian Occupational Therapy Journal","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1440-1630.70043","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"REHABILITATION","Score":null,"Total":0}
Efficacy of a prewriting intervention: A pilot randomised control trial
Introduction
Successful handwriting is dependent on accurate and efficient letter formation, which is dependent on drawing sub-strokes of letters and prewriting patterns. Currently, there is no prewriting intervention programmes with established efficacy, and little is known about children's perceptions of engaging in these programmes. This study aimed to determine the efficacy and feasibility of a prewriting intervention.
Methods
A pilot randomised control trial was conducted with embedded aspects of fidelity and acceptability. Participants included 18 typically developing 4- to 5-year-old children, attending a Western Australian kindergarten (year before first formal schooling year), randomly allocated to an intervention or waitlist control group. Baseline and post intervention data were collected using the Developmental Test of Visual Perception (Third Edition) and the Prewriting Assessment (PWA). Participants received six Peggy Lego intervention sessions, and a fidelity checklist was completed following each session. Immediately following completion of the intervention, participants provided acceptability feedback using a modified Likert scale.
Consumer and Community Involvement
Teachers and occupational therapists working with 4- to 5-year-old children provided feedback on the intervention.
Results
There was a statistically significant main effect of time on the PWA score (p = 0.003); however, the main effect of group and the interaction of group and time were non-significant (p = 0.070 and p = 0.46). The intervention was implemented with high levels of fidelity with 19 sessions (n = 34) achieving 100% fidelity. Eligibility was deemed feasible with 60% of those enrolled for the study meeting eligibility criteria. Most participants (n = 17) completed six intervention sessions. Most participants found the intervention acceptable (n = 13).
Conclusions
Exploratory analysis showed all children significantly improved their prewriting ability; however, it is likely that this effect is not attributed to intervention alone. This pilot randomised control trial is deemed feasible in terms of recruitment, retention of participants, and data collection. Further research on the efficacy of this intervention is justified.
PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY
For writing to be easy to read, students need to form letters the right way and with good control. Before they can do this, they need to learn basic pencil strokes used to make letters. Handwriting programmes are used in schools and with children who find writing hard. We know some of these programmes help, but we do not know which prewriting programmes work best. We wanted to find out if a programme called Peggy Lego helps and if it is easy to use. We did six Peggy Lego sessions with 18 kindergarten children in Western Australia. All of the children got better at their prewriting skills, but we could not tell if Peggy Lego helped more than routine teaching, or if the children improved over time. We asked the students how they felt about the programme. Most said they liked it and thought their drawing got better, even though it was a bit hard. Future research could look at whether feeling more confident helps children when they start learning to write letters.
期刊介绍:
The Australian Occupational Therapy Journal is a leading international peer reviewed publication presenting influential, high quality innovative scholarship and research relevant to occupational therapy. The aim of the journal is to be a leader in the dissemination of scholarship and evidence to substantiate, influence and shape policy and occupational therapy practice locally and globally. The journal publishes empirical studies, theoretical papers, and reviews. Preference will be given to manuscripts that have a sound theoretical basis, methodological rigour with sufficient scope and scale to make important new contributions to the occupational therapy body of knowledge. AOTJ does not publish protocols for any study design
The journal will consider multidisciplinary or interprofessional studies that include occupational therapy, occupational therapists or occupational therapy students, so long as ‘key points’ highlight the specific implications for occupational therapy, occupational therapists and/or occupational therapy students and/or consumers.