我们需要更好的措施来了解社交媒体对药物使用的影响。

IF 5.3 1区 医学 Q1 PSYCHIATRY
Addiction Pub Date : 2025-07-10 DOI:10.1111/add.70138
Benjamin Riordan, Damian Scarf, Megan Strowger, Gedefaw Alen, Taylor Winter, Emmanuel Kuntsche
{"title":"我们需要更好的措施来了解社交媒体对药物使用的影响。","authors":"Benjamin Riordan,&nbsp;Damian Scarf,&nbsp;Megan Strowger,&nbsp;Gedefaw Alen,&nbsp;Taylor Winter,&nbsp;Emmanuel Kuntsche","doi":"10.1111/add.70138","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Social media use is extremely common, with an estimated 63.9% of the global population using social media in 2024 [<span>1</span>]. Adolescents and young adults (aged 16–24 years) stand out compared to other age groups, and spend approximately 180 minutes per day on social media [<span>2</span>]. Together with the reported impact on wellbeing, policymakers globally are considering drastic changes to limit youth access to social media platforms [<span>3</span>]. For instance, Australia recently passed a law to ban people under 16 years of age from using social media and France, the United Kingdom and Norway are also considering implementing age restrictions [<span>3</span>].</p><p>To justify these bans, policymakers have cited evidence of the link between social media use and negative health outcomes among young people, including the impact on substance use [<span>4</span>]. Indeed, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis found a positive link between social media use and alcohol, drugs, tobacco and vaping [<span>5</span>]. However, there is an ongoing debate about the quality of the evidence assessing the impact of social media use, with concerns about: small effects, a lack of theoretical models, the design (often cross-sectional), failing to control for confounders or consideration of the positive effects of social media use [<span>6-8</span>].</p><p>One of the most critical methodological limitations is with how social media use or exposure to certain content on social media has been measured [<span>7, 8</span>]. Recent reviews have found that most studies used self-reported social media use (e.g. how much time/how often do you use social media) and exposure to substance use (e.g. how often do you see alcohol on social media) [<span>4, 5</span>]. For example, in a recent meta-analysis, only six of 94 studies used an objective assessment of social media use [<span>7</span>]. Unfortunately, self-reports for digital media use in general and exposure to substance-related content have seldom shown comparable accuracy to objective measures [<span>8</span>]. In this editorial, we briefly overview the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures and identify alternative options for measuring social media use or exposure to substance-related content.</p><p>Parry <i>et al</i>. [<span>8</span>] meta-analysed 45 studies that measured both self-report and logged digital media use (including 4 that measured social media use) and found only a modest correlation between the two measures. They concluded that ‘when asked to estimate their usage, participants are rarely accurate.’ More recent studies have found similar discrepancies when comparing objective and self-reported social media use [<span>9-11</span>]. There are a number of reasons why self-report may be inaccurate, like recall bias (social media is used so frequently that it is unmemorable) or limitations with the measures used (e.g. asking participants to report their social media use averaged over longer time periods, assessing use of all social media platforms with one question, limited response options, lack of using existing validated questions) [<span>7</span>].</p><p>For researchers aiming to measure social media use, there are several solutions to obtain more accurate or objective estimates. Most smartphones monitor screen and app use, and researchers can ask participants to use these logs to inform their self-report estimates or to upload screenshots of app use in a survey [<span>12</span>]. Additionally, researchers have developed apps (e.g. eMoodie, Minuku) that can be installed to not only monitor time and app use (when used, how long accessed), but can also be integrated with other data streams such as surveys [<span>13, 14</span>]. For example, the eMoodie app not only passively collected time spent and frequency of social media app usage, but also sent twice daily surveys to assess psychological constructs such as affect and mental wellbeing. Although these approaches (and those outlined below) are important, one key limitation is that they require more effort from participants, which may impact who is willing to take part in research and potentially introduce participant selection bias.</p><p>There is limited evidence regarding the accuracy of self-reported exposure to substance-related content on social media, and further research is needed in this area. In our work with other digital media, we found that the actual occurrences of exposure to alcohol was 5× higher when participants estimated alcohol content in their favourite movies and nearly 2× higher when they estimated alcohol content immediately after having seen a 10-minute video [<span>15, 16</span>]. Although there is limited evidence from self-reports of exposure on social media, one study found that participants were even inaccurate when recalling how often they themselves had posted about alcohol, finding that there was only a medium correlation between self-reports and objective measures [<span>17</span>].</p><p>Collecting objective exposure to substance-related content is difficult, but recently researchers have established some exciting and innovative solutions. Angus <i>et al</i>. [<span>18</span>] developed a browser plugin and a mobile application that can automatically identify and collect the ads people are exposed to on-line. Similarly, the Stanford Screenomics project developed an app with open-source code that can take intermittent screenshots while people are using their smartphone [<span>19</span>]. Collecting images is particularly exciting because it allows researchers to not only consider how often people are exposed to substance-related content, but to also understand the nature of that content (e.g. positive/social depictions) or the source of the content (e.g. ads, influencer posts, posts from friends). Considering the nature and source can help determine what type of exposure is related to more substance use or what type of exposure may be preventative.</p><p>Although collecting and analysing images is exciting, there are ethical implications to collecting this content and researchers should be guided by ethics boards and current best practices (see Angus <i>et al</i>. and Carrière <i>et al</i>., for best practices) [<span>18, 20</span>]. However, future applications could ensure that data never leaves a participant's device. For example, thanks to improvements in artificial intelligence (AI), future computational applications could use approaches like browser plugins to passively count exposure to certain content [<span>21</span>].</p><p>There are also relatively simple solutions to improve reporting that may be more accessible for researchers and do not involve additional computational resources. For example, Rutherford <i>et al</i>. [<span>22</span>] asked participants to ‘scroll through’ social media for 30 minutes and take screenshots of alcohol-related ads. To avoid collecting images, this approach could be adapted so participants are prompted to scroll through for shorter periods and manually count and describe substance-related content they see. This places more burden on participants and still involves self-report, but the focus on specific periods of time could produce more accurate estimates than current self-report methods and does not require participants to upload potentially sensitive data.</p><p>It is important for future research to consider both self-report and objective measures to help to understand the interplay between the two [<span>23</span>]. In the context of exposure to substance-related content on social media, very few studies have included both self-report and objective measures and doing so will allow us to determine the accuracy of self-reported exposure or which self-report questions yield more accurate estimates. Furthermore, it may be important to understand whether the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures is systematic and what implications this may have for interpreting the current evidence [<span>23</span>]. For example, self-reported exposure may be very similar to social normative perceptions. That is, those who believe substance use is more common may also believe that they see more oftensubstance-related content on social media (even though they do not).</p><p>The literature on the impact of social media use and exposure on substance use is currently dominated by self-report, which tends to be inaccurate. With today's technology (e.g. screen recordings, apps, AI) objective measurement can now be more easily obtained. This is particularly important given the desire of policymakers to regulate social media. Therefore, objective data can help shine light on the true amount of social media use and the exposure to unhealthy content including substance use to inform policy.</p><p><b>Benjamin Riordan:</b> Conceptualization (lead); writing—original draft (lead). <b>Damian Scarf:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—original draft (supporting). <b>Megan Strowger:</b> Writing—original draft (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Gedefaw Alen:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Taylor Winter:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Emmanuel Kuntsche:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (lead).</p><p>None.</p>","PeriodicalId":109,"journal":{"name":"Addiction","volume":"120 11","pages":"2162-2164"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3000,"publicationDate":"2025-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/add.70138","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"We need better measures to understand the influence of social media on substance use\",\"authors\":\"Benjamin Riordan,&nbsp;Damian Scarf,&nbsp;Megan Strowger,&nbsp;Gedefaw Alen,&nbsp;Taylor Winter,&nbsp;Emmanuel Kuntsche\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/add.70138\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Social media use is extremely common, with an estimated 63.9% of the global population using social media in 2024 [<span>1</span>]. Adolescents and young adults (aged 16–24 years) stand out compared to other age groups, and spend approximately 180 minutes per day on social media [<span>2</span>]. Together with the reported impact on wellbeing, policymakers globally are considering drastic changes to limit youth access to social media platforms [<span>3</span>]. For instance, Australia recently passed a law to ban people under 16 years of age from using social media and France, the United Kingdom and Norway are also considering implementing age restrictions [<span>3</span>].</p><p>To justify these bans, policymakers have cited evidence of the link between social media use and negative health outcomes among young people, including the impact on substance use [<span>4</span>]. Indeed, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis found a positive link between social media use and alcohol, drugs, tobacco and vaping [<span>5</span>]. However, there is an ongoing debate about the quality of the evidence assessing the impact of social media use, with concerns about: small effects, a lack of theoretical models, the design (often cross-sectional), failing to control for confounders or consideration of the positive effects of social media use [<span>6-8</span>].</p><p>One of the most critical methodological limitations is with how social media use or exposure to certain content on social media has been measured [<span>7, 8</span>]. Recent reviews have found that most studies used self-reported social media use (e.g. how much time/how often do you use social media) and exposure to substance use (e.g. how often do you see alcohol on social media) [<span>4, 5</span>]. For example, in a recent meta-analysis, only six of 94 studies used an objective assessment of social media use [<span>7</span>]. Unfortunately, self-reports for digital media use in general and exposure to substance-related content have seldom shown comparable accuracy to objective measures [<span>8</span>]. In this editorial, we briefly overview the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures and identify alternative options for measuring social media use or exposure to substance-related content.</p><p>Parry <i>et al</i>. [<span>8</span>] meta-analysed 45 studies that measured both self-report and logged digital media use (including 4 that measured social media use) and found only a modest correlation between the two measures. They concluded that ‘when asked to estimate their usage, participants are rarely accurate.’ More recent studies have found similar discrepancies when comparing objective and self-reported social media use [<span>9-11</span>]. There are a number of reasons why self-report may be inaccurate, like recall bias (social media is used so frequently that it is unmemorable) or limitations with the measures used (e.g. asking participants to report their social media use averaged over longer time periods, assessing use of all social media platforms with one question, limited response options, lack of using existing validated questions) [<span>7</span>].</p><p>For researchers aiming to measure social media use, there are several solutions to obtain more accurate or objective estimates. Most smartphones monitor screen and app use, and researchers can ask participants to use these logs to inform their self-report estimates or to upload screenshots of app use in a survey [<span>12</span>]. Additionally, researchers have developed apps (e.g. eMoodie, Minuku) that can be installed to not only monitor time and app use (when used, how long accessed), but can also be integrated with other data streams such as surveys [<span>13, 14</span>]. For example, the eMoodie app not only passively collected time spent and frequency of social media app usage, but also sent twice daily surveys to assess psychological constructs such as affect and mental wellbeing. Although these approaches (and those outlined below) are important, one key limitation is that they require more effort from participants, which may impact who is willing to take part in research and potentially introduce participant selection bias.</p><p>There is limited evidence regarding the accuracy of self-reported exposure to substance-related content on social media, and further research is needed in this area. In our work with other digital media, we found that the actual occurrences of exposure to alcohol was 5× higher when participants estimated alcohol content in their favourite movies and nearly 2× higher when they estimated alcohol content immediately after having seen a 10-minute video [<span>15, 16</span>]. Although there is limited evidence from self-reports of exposure on social media, one study found that participants were even inaccurate when recalling how often they themselves had posted about alcohol, finding that there was only a medium correlation between self-reports and objective measures [<span>17</span>].</p><p>Collecting objective exposure to substance-related content is difficult, but recently researchers have established some exciting and innovative solutions. Angus <i>et al</i>. [<span>18</span>] developed a browser plugin and a mobile application that can automatically identify and collect the ads people are exposed to on-line. Similarly, the Stanford Screenomics project developed an app with open-source code that can take intermittent screenshots while people are using their smartphone [<span>19</span>]. Collecting images is particularly exciting because it allows researchers to not only consider how often people are exposed to substance-related content, but to also understand the nature of that content (e.g. positive/social depictions) or the source of the content (e.g. ads, influencer posts, posts from friends). Considering the nature and source can help determine what type of exposure is related to more substance use or what type of exposure may be preventative.</p><p>Although collecting and analysing images is exciting, there are ethical implications to collecting this content and researchers should be guided by ethics boards and current best practices (see Angus <i>et al</i>. and Carrière <i>et al</i>., for best practices) [<span>18, 20</span>]. However, future applications could ensure that data never leaves a participant's device. For example, thanks to improvements in artificial intelligence (AI), future computational applications could use approaches like browser plugins to passively count exposure to certain content [<span>21</span>].</p><p>There are also relatively simple solutions to improve reporting that may be more accessible for researchers and do not involve additional computational resources. For example, Rutherford <i>et al</i>. [<span>22</span>] asked participants to ‘scroll through’ social media for 30 minutes and take screenshots of alcohol-related ads. To avoid collecting images, this approach could be adapted so participants are prompted to scroll through for shorter periods and manually count and describe substance-related content they see. This places more burden on participants and still involves self-report, but the focus on specific periods of time could produce more accurate estimates than current self-report methods and does not require participants to upload potentially sensitive data.</p><p>It is important for future research to consider both self-report and objective measures to help to understand the interplay between the two [<span>23</span>]. In the context of exposure to substance-related content on social media, very few studies have included both self-report and objective measures and doing so will allow us to determine the accuracy of self-reported exposure or which self-report questions yield more accurate estimates. Furthermore, it may be important to understand whether the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures is systematic and what implications this may have for interpreting the current evidence [<span>23</span>]. For example, self-reported exposure may be very similar to social normative perceptions. That is, those who believe substance use is more common may also believe that they see more oftensubstance-related content on social media (even though they do not).</p><p>The literature on the impact of social media use and exposure on substance use is currently dominated by self-report, which tends to be inaccurate. With today's technology (e.g. screen recordings, apps, AI) objective measurement can now be more easily obtained. This is particularly important given the desire of policymakers to regulate social media. Therefore, objective data can help shine light on the true amount of social media use and the exposure to unhealthy content including substance use to inform policy.</p><p><b>Benjamin Riordan:</b> Conceptualization (lead); writing—original draft (lead). <b>Damian Scarf:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—original draft (supporting). <b>Megan Strowger:</b> Writing—original draft (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Gedefaw Alen:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Taylor Winter:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). <b>Emmanuel Kuntsche:</b> Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (lead).</p><p>None.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":109,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Addiction\",\"volume\":\"120 11\",\"pages\":\"2162-2164\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":5.3000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-07-10\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/add.70138\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Addiction\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/add.70138\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHIATRY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Addiction","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/add.70138","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

社交媒体的使用非常普遍,预计到2024年,全球有63.9%的人口使用社交媒体。与其他年龄组相比,青少年和年轻人(16-24岁)的表现尤为突出,他们每天花在社交媒体[2]上的时间约为180分钟。考虑到对幸福感的影响,全球政策制定者正在考虑采取重大措施,限制青少年使用社交媒体平台b[3]。例如,澳大利亚最近通过了一项法律,禁止16岁以下的人使用社交媒体,法国、英国和挪威也在考虑实施年龄限制。为了证明这些禁令的合理性,政策制定者引用了社交媒体使用与年轻人的负面健康结果之间存在联系的证据,包括对药物使用的影响。事实上,最近的一项系统回顾和荟萃分析发现,社交媒体的使用与酒精、毒品、烟草和电子烟之间存在正相关。然而,关于评估社交媒体使用影响的证据的质量一直存在争议,人们担心:影响小,缺乏理论模型,设计(通常是横截面的),未能控制混杂因素或考虑社交媒体使用的积极影响[6-8]。最关键的方法限制之一是如何衡量社交媒体的使用或社交媒体上某些内容的曝光[7,8]。最近的评论发现,大多数研究使用了自我报告的社交媒体使用情况(例如,你使用社交媒体的时间/频率)和接触物质使用情况(例如,你在社交媒体上看到酒精的频率)[4,5]。例如,在最近的一项荟萃分析中,94项研究中只有6项使用了对社交媒体使用情况的客观评估。不幸的是,一般数字媒体使用和接触物质相关内容的自我报告很少显示出与客观测量相媲美的准确性。在这篇社论中,我们简要概述了自我报告和客观测量之间的差异,并确定了衡量社交媒体使用或接触物质相关内容的替代选择。Parry et al. b[8]荟萃分析了45项研究,这些研究测量了自我报告和记录的数字媒体使用情况(包括4项测量社交媒体使用情况的研究),发现这两项测量之间只有适度的相关性。他们得出结论:“当被要求估计自己的使用情况时,参与者很少是准确的。最近的研究在比较客观和自我报告的社交媒体使用情况时发现了类似的差异[9-11]。自我报告可能不准确的原因有很多,比如回忆偏差(社交媒体的使用频率如此之高,以至于难以记住)或所使用的测量方法的局限性(例如,要求参与者报告他们在较长时间内平均使用社交媒体的情况,用一个问题评估所有社交媒体平台的使用情况,有限的回答选项,缺乏使用现有的有效问题)[7]。对于旨在衡量社交媒体使用情况的研究人员来说,有几种解决方案可以获得更准确或客观的估计。大多数智能手机都会监控屏幕和应用程序的使用情况,研究人员可以要求参与者使用这些日志来告知他们的自我报告估计,或者在调查中上传应用程序使用情况的截图。此外,研究人员还开发了应用程序(如eMoodie, Minuku),这些应用程序不仅可以监控时间和应用程序的使用情况(何时使用,访问多长时间),还可以与其他数据流(如调查)集成[13,14]。例如,eMoodie应用程序不仅被动地收集使用社交媒体应用程序的时间和频率,还每天发送两次调查,以评估情感和心理健康等心理结构。尽管这些方法(以及下面列出的方法)很重要,但一个关键的限制是,它们需要参与者付出更多的努力,这可能会影响谁愿意参与研究,并可能引入参与者选择偏差。关于自我报告在社交媒体上接触物质相关内容的准确性,证据有限,需要在这一领域进行进一步研究。在我们与其他数字媒体的合作中,我们发现,当参与者估计他们最喜欢的电影中的酒精含量时,实际接触酒精的情况高出5倍,而当他们在看完10分钟的视频后立即估计酒精含量时,实际接触酒精的情况高出近2倍[15,16]。尽管在社交媒体上曝光的自我报告证据有限,但一项研究发现,参与者在回忆自己发布关于酒精的频率时甚至是不准确的,发现自我报告和客观测量指标[17]之间只有中等相关性。收集客观暴露于物质相关内容是困难的,但最近研究人员已经建立了一些令人兴奋和创新的解决方案。安格斯等人。 [18]开发了一个浏览器插件和一个移动应用程序,可以自动识别和收集人们在网上接触到的广告。同样,斯坦福大学屏幕组学项目开发了一款应用程序,使用开源代码,可以在人们使用智能手机b[19]时截取间歇性的屏幕截图。收集图像特别令人兴奋,因为它使研究人员不仅可以考虑人们接触与物质相关内容的频率,还可以了解内容的性质(例如积极/社交描述)或内容的来源(例如广告,网红帖子,朋友的帖子)。考虑性质和来源可以帮助确定哪种类型的接触与更多的物质使用有关,或者哪种类型的接触可能是预防性的。虽然收集和分析图像是令人兴奋的,但收集这些内容存在伦理问题,研究人员应该受到伦理委员会和当前最佳实践的指导(参见Angus等人和carriires等人的最佳实践)[18,20]。然而,未来的应用程序可以确保数据永远不会离开参与者的设备。例如,由于人工智能(AI)的进步,未来的计算应用程序可以使用浏览器插件等方法来被动地计算特定内容的曝光量。也有相对简单的解决方案来改进报告,这些解决方案可能对研究人员来说更容易获得,并且不涉及额外的计算资源。例如,Rutherford等人要求参与者“滚动”社交媒体30分钟,并拍摄与酒精相关的广告截图。为了避免收集图像,可以调整这种方法,以便提示参与者滚动更短的时间,手动计数和描述他们看到的与物质相关的内容。这给参与者带来了更多的负担,并且仍然涉及自我报告,但是专注于特定时间段可以产生比当前的自我报告方法更准确的估计,并且不需要参与者上传可能敏感的数据。对于未来的研究来说,考虑自我报告和客观测量来帮助理解两者之间的相互作用是很重要的。在社交媒体上接触物质相关内容的背景下,很少有研究同时包括自我报告和客观测量,这样做将使我们能够确定自我报告暴露的准确性,或者哪些自我报告问题能产生更准确的估计。此外,了解自我报告和客观测量之间的差异是否是系统性的,以及这对解释当前证据可能产生的影响,可能是很重要的。例如,自我报告的暴露可能与社会规范认知非常相似。也就是说,那些认为药物使用更常见的人可能也认为他们在社交媒体上看到更多与药物相关的内容(即使他们没有)。关于社交媒体使用和暴露对物质使用的影响的文献目前以自我报告为主,这往往是不准确的。有了今天的技术(如屏幕录音、应用程序、人工智能),现在可以更容易地获得客观测量。考虑到政策制定者希望监管社交媒体,这一点尤为重要。因此,客观数据可以帮助揭示社交媒体使用的真实数量和接触不健康内容(包括物质使用)的情况,从而为政策提供信息。Benjamin Riordan:概念化(lead);写作——原稿(引子)。达米安·斯卡夫:概念化(支持);写作-原稿(附)。梅根·斯特罗杰:写作原稿(配角);写作-审查和编辑(支持)。Gedefaw Alen:概念化(支持);写作-审查和编辑(支持)。泰勒·温特:概念化(支持);写作-审查和编辑(支持)。昆切:概念化(支持);写作-审查和编辑(主导)。无。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。

We need better measures to understand the influence of social media on substance use

We need better measures to understand the influence of social media on substance use

Social media use is extremely common, with an estimated 63.9% of the global population using social media in 2024 [1]. Adolescents and young adults (aged 16–24 years) stand out compared to other age groups, and spend approximately 180 minutes per day on social media [2]. Together with the reported impact on wellbeing, policymakers globally are considering drastic changes to limit youth access to social media platforms [3]. For instance, Australia recently passed a law to ban people under 16 years of age from using social media and France, the United Kingdom and Norway are also considering implementing age restrictions [3].

To justify these bans, policymakers have cited evidence of the link between social media use and negative health outcomes among young people, including the impact on substance use [4]. Indeed, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis found a positive link between social media use and alcohol, drugs, tobacco and vaping [5]. However, there is an ongoing debate about the quality of the evidence assessing the impact of social media use, with concerns about: small effects, a lack of theoretical models, the design (often cross-sectional), failing to control for confounders or consideration of the positive effects of social media use [6-8].

One of the most critical methodological limitations is with how social media use or exposure to certain content on social media has been measured [7, 8]. Recent reviews have found that most studies used self-reported social media use (e.g. how much time/how often do you use social media) and exposure to substance use (e.g. how often do you see alcohol on social media) [4, 5]. For example, in a recent meta-analysis, only six of 94 studies used an objective assessment of social media use [7]. Unfortunately, self-reports for digital media use in general and exposure to substance-related content have seldom shown comparable accuracy to objective measures [8]. In this editorial, we briefly overview the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures and identify alternative options for measuring social media use or exposure to substance-related content.

Parry et al. [8] meta-analysed 45 studies that measured both self-report and logged digital media use (including 4 that measured social media use) and found only a modest correlation between the two measures. They concluded that ‘when asked to estimate their usage, participants are rarely accurate.’ More recent studies have found similar discrepancies when comparing objective and self-reported social media use [9-11]. There are a number of reasons why self-report may be inaccurate, like recall bias (social media is used so frequently that it is unmemorable) or limitations with the measures used (e.g. asking participants to report their social media use averaged over longer time periods, assessing use of all social media platforms with one question, limited response options, lack of using existing validated questions) [7].

For researchers aiming to measure social media use, there are several solutions to obtain more accurate or objective estimates. Most smartphones monitor screen and app use, and researchers can ask participants to use these logs to inform their self-report estimates or to upload screenshots of app use in a survey [12]. Additionally, researchers have developed apps (e.g. eMoodie, Minuku) that can be installed to not only monitor time and app use (when used, how long accessed), but can also be integrated with other data streams such as surveys [13, 14]. For example, the eMoodie app not only passively collected time spent and frequency of social media app usage, but also sent twice daily surveys to assess psychological constructs such as affect and mental wellbeing. Although these approaches (and those outlined below) are important, one key limitation is that they require more effort from participants, which may impact who is willing to take part in research and potentially introduce participant selection bias.

There is limited evidence regarding the accuracy of self-reported exposure to substance-related content on social media, and further research is needed in this area. In our work with other digital media, we found that the actual occurrences of exposure to alcohol was 5× higher when participants estimated alcohol content in their favourite movies and nearly 2× higher when they estimated alcohol content immediately after having seen a 10-minute video [15, 16]. Although there is limited evidence from self-reports of exposure on social media, one study found that participants were even inaccurate when recalling how often they themselves had posted about alcohol, finding that there was only a medium correlation between self-reports and objective measures [17].

Collecting objective exposure to substance-related content is difficult, but recently researchers have established some exciting and innovative solutions. Angus et al. [18] developed a browser plugin and a mobile application that can automatically identify and collect the ads people are exposed to on-line. Similarly, the Stanford Screenomics project developed an app with open-source code that can take intermittent screenshots while people are using their smartphone [19]. Collecting images is particularly exciting because it allows researchers to not only consider how often people are exposed to substance-related content, but to also understand the nature of that content (e.g. positive/social depictions) or the source of the content (e.g. ads, influencer posts, posts from friends). Considering the nature and source can help determine what type of exposure is related to more substance use or what type of exposure may be preventative.

Although collecting and analysing images is exciting, there are ethical implications to collecting this content and researchers should be guided by ethics boards and current best practices (see Angus et al. and Carrière et al., for best practices) [18, 20]. However, future applications could ensure that data never leaves a participant's device. For example, thanks to improvements in artificial intelligence (AI), future computational applications could use approaches like browser plugins to passively count exposure to certain content [21].

There are also relatively simple solutions to improve reporting that may be more accessible for researchers and do not involve additional computational resources. For example, Rutherford et al. [22] asked participants to ‘scroll through’ social media for 30 minutes and take screenshots of alcohol-related ads. To avoid collecting images, this approach could be adapted so participants are prompted to scroll through for shorter periods and manually count and describe substance-related content they see. This places more burden on participants and still involves self-report, but the focus on specific periods of time could produce more accurate estimates than current self-report methods and does not require participants to upload potentially sensitive data.

It is important for future research to consider both self-report and objective measures to help to understand the interplay between the two [23]. In the context of exposure to substance-related content on social media, very few studies have included both self-report and objective measures and doing so will allow us to determine the accuracy of self-reported exposure or which self-report questions yield more accurate estimates. Furthermore, it may be important to understand whether the discrepancy between self-report and objective measures is systematic and what implications this may have for interpreting the current evidence [23]. For example, self-reported exposure may be very similar to social normative perceptions. That is, those who believe substance use is more common may also believe that they see more oftensubstance-related content on social media (even though they do not).

The literature on the impact of social media use and exposure on substance use is currently dominated by self-report, which tends to be inaccurate. With today's technology (e.g. screen recordings, apps, AI) objective measurement can now be more easily obtained. This is particularly important given the desire of policymakers to regulate social media. Therefore, objective data can help shine light on the true amount of social media use and the exposure to unhealthy content including substance use to inform policy.

Benjamin Riordan: Conceptualization (lead); writing—original draft (lead). Damian Scarf: Conceptualization (supporting); writing—original draft (supporting). Megan Strowger: Writing—original draft (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). Gedefaw Alen: Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). Taylor Winter: Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (supporting). Emmanuel Kuntsche: Conceptualization (supporting); writing—review and editing (lead).

None.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Addiction
Addiction 医学-精神病学
CiteScore
10.80
自引率
6.70%
发文量
319
审稿时长
3 months
期刊介绍: Addiction publishes peer-reviewed research reports on pharmacological and behavioural addictions, bringing together research conducted within many different disciplines. Its goal is to serve international and interdisciplinary scientific and clinical communication, to strengthen links between science and policy, and to stimulate and enhance the quality of debate. We seek submissions that are not only technically competent but are also original and contain information or ideas of fresh interest to our international readership. We seek to serve low- and middle-income (LAMI) countries as well as more economically developed countries. Addiction’s scope spans human experimental, epidemiological, social science, historical, clinical and policy research relating to addiction, primarily but not exclusively in the areas of psychoactive substance use and/or gambling. In addition to original research, the journal features editorials, commentaries, reviews, letters, and book reviews.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信