纠正多数群体归属而不疏远少数群体?少数群体间接触和不平等的经历

IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL
{"title":"纠正多数群体归属而不疏远少数群体?少数群体间接触和不平等的经历","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/bjso.12912","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Kende, J., Baysu, G., Van Laar, C., &amp; Phalet, K. (2021), Majority group belonging without minority group distancing? Minority experiences of intergroup contact and inequality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 60: 121–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12382</p><p>Figures 1–3 were presented in the wrong order in the original publication. The labels and captions in the figures are correct, and so is the description of the results, but the description of the results does not correspond to the numbering of the figures.</p><p>The results illustrated in Figure 1 on p. 12 (<i>Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact</i>) are described in the text on p. 13 (<i>Examining this interaction further (see Figure 2), more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of perceived unfairness (p = .003), yet minority attitudes were unrelated to majority contact when minority youth perceived less unfair treatment (p = .526). Likewise, when minority youth perceived more unfair treatment, their attitudes were significantly less positive only at higher levels of majority contact (p = .003), yet at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to perceived unfair treatment (p = .649)</i>, as being illustrated in Figure 2. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated Figure 1.</p><p>Similarly, the results illustrated in Figure 2 on p. 13 (<i>Minority identification at low and high perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact</i>) are described in the text on p. 14. (<i>The same interaction was also significant on strength of minority identification (see Table 2). As Figure 3 shows, more majority contact was significantly related to weaker minority identification at higher levels of perceived unfair treatment in school (p &lt; .001), yet majority contact was unrelated to minority identification when minority students perceived less unfairness (p = .182). Also, at high levels of majority contact, majority contact was related to weaker minority identification, although this association did not quite reach significance (p = .070</i>).as being illustrated in Figure 3 instead of Figure 1.</p><p>Finally, the results illustrated in Figure 3 on p. 14 <i>(Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual experiences of discrimination and low and high majority contact)</i> are described in the text on p. 12. <i>Moreover, in line with H2 on minority group distancing, there was a significant two-way interaction of positive majority contact with individual discrimination experiences on attitudes towards the minority group (see Table S3 in Appendix S1). To interpret the interaction, we tested simple effects of majority contact at high versus low levels of experienced discrimination, and of discrimination at high versus low levels of contact (-1 SD) with the Wald test, which is similar to a z-test. While more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of discrimination (p &lt; .001), this association was not significant when minorities experienced less discrimination (p = .641) (see Figure 1). Likewise, attitudes towards the minority group were less positive when minorities experienced more discrimination at higher levels of majority contact (p = .002), but at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to their discrimination experiences (p = .594)</i>, as illustrated in Figure 1. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated in Figure 3.</p><p>We apologize for this error.</p>","PeriodicalId":48304,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"64 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2000,"publicationDate":"2025-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjso.12912","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Correction to Majority group belonging without minority group distancing? Minority experiences of intergroup contact and inequality\",\"authors\":\"\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/bjso.12912\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Kende, J., Baysu, G., Van Laar, C., &amp; Phalet, K. (2021), Majority group belonging without minority group distancing? Minority experiences of intergroup contact and inequality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 60: 121–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12382</p><p>Figures 1–3 were presented in the wrong order in the original publication. The labels and captions in the figures are correct, and so is the description of the results, but the description of the results does not correspond to the numbering of the figures.</p><p>The results illustrated in Figure 1 on p. 12 (<i>Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact</i>) are described in the text on p. 13 (<i>Examining this interaction further (see Figure 2), more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of perceived unfairness (p = .003), yet minority attitudes were unrelated to majority contact when minority youth perceived less unfair treatment (p = .526). Likewise, when minority youth perceived more unfair treatment, their attitudes were significantly less positive only at higher levels of majority contact (p = .003), yet at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to perceived unfair treatment (p = .649)</i>, as being illustrated in Figure 2. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated Figure 1.</p><p>Similarly, the results illustrated in Figure 2 on p. 13 (<i>Minority identification at low and high perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact</i>) are described in the text on p. 14. (<i>The same interaction was also significant on strength of minority identification (see Table 2). As Figure 3 shows, more majority contact was significantly related to weaker minority identification at higher levels of perceived unfair treatment in school (p &lt; .001), yet majority contact was unrelated to minority identification when minority students perceived less unfairness (p = .182). Also, at high levels of majority contact, majority contact was related to weaker minority identification, although this association did not quite reach significance (p = .070</i>).as being illustrated in Figure 3 instead of Figure 1.</p><p>Finally, the results illustrated in Figure 3 on p. 14 <i>(Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual experiences of discrimination and low and high majority contact)</i> are described in the text on p. 12. <i>Moreover, in line with H2 on minority group distancing, there was a significant two-way interaction of positive majority contact with individual discrimination experiences on attitudes towards the minority group (see Table S3 in Appendix S1). To interpret the interaction, we tested simple effects of majority contact at high versus low levels of experienced discrimination, and of discrimination at high versus low levels of contact (-1 SD) with the Wald test, which is similar to a z-test. While more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of discrimination (p &lt; .001), this association was not significant when minorities experienced less discrimination (p = .641) (see Figure 1). Likewise, attitudes towards the minority group were less positive when minorities experienced more discrimination at higher levels of majority contact (p = .002), but at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to their discrimination experiences (p = .594)</i>, as illustrated in Figure 1. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated in Figure 3.</p><p>We apologize for this error.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":48304,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"British Journal of Social Psychology\",\"volume\":\"64 3\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.2000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-06-12\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjso.12912\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"British Journal of Social Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/bjso.12912\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"British Journal of Social Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/bjso.12912","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

Kende, J., Baysu, G., Van Laar, C., &;Phalet, K.(2021),多数群体的归属与少数群体的距离?少数群体间接触和不平等的经历。社会心理学杂志,30(2):391 - 391。https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12382Figures 1-3在原出版物中顺序排列错误。图中的标签和说明是正确的,结果的描述也是正确的,但结果的描述与图的编号不对应。第13页的文本描述了第12页图1所示的结果(对少数群体的态度,个人对不公平待遇的看法不同,对少数群体的态度也不同)(进一步检查这种相互作用(见图2),更多的多数接触预示着对少数群体的积极态度在感知不公平的较高水平上显著降低(p = 0.003)。然而,当少数民族青年感受到较少的不公平待遇时,少数民族的态度与多数接触无关(p = .526)。同样,当少数民族青年感受到更多不公平待遇时,他们的态度只有在与大多数人接触的水平较高时才明显不那么积极(p = 0.003),而在与大多数人接触的水平较低时,少数民族的态度与感受到的不公平待遇无关(p = 0.649),如图2所示。正确的描述应该如图1所示。同样,第13页图2所示的结果(少数民族对不公平待遇的高度和低感知以及多数接触的高度和低感知)见第14页。(同样的相互作用在少数民族识别的强度上也很显著(见表2)。如图3所示,在对学校不公平待遇的感知程度较高时,更多的多数接触与较弱的少数群体认同显著相关(p < .001),而当少数群体学生对不公平待遇的感知程度较低时,多数接触与少数群体认同无关(p = .182)。此外,在高水平的多数接触中,多数接触与较弱的少数群体识别相关,尽管这种关联没有达到显著性(p = 0.070)。如图3而不是图1所示。最后,第12页的文本描述了第14页图3所示的结果(对少数群体的态度,低和高的个人歧视经历以及低和高的多数接触)。此外,与H2关于少数群体距离一致,积极的多数接触与个人歧视经历对少数群体态度的双向交互作用显著(见附录S1表S3)。为了解释这种相互作用,我们用Wald检验(类似于z检验)测试了高水平与低水平经验歧视的大多数接触的简单影响,以及高水平与低水平接触的歧视(-1 SD)的简单影响。虽然在歧视程度较高的情况下,与多数人接触越多,对少数群体的积极态度就会显著降低(p < .001),但当少数群体遭受较少歧视时,这种关联并不显著(p = .641)(见图1)。同样,当少数群体在较高的多数接触水平上遭受更多歧视时,对少数群体的态度不那么积极(p = 0.002),但在较低的多数接触水平上,少数群体的态度与他们的歧视经历无关(p = 0.594),如图1所示。正确的描述应该如图3所示。我们为这个错误道歉。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Correction to Majority group belonging without minority group distancing? Minority experiences of intergroup contact and inequality

Kende, J., Baysu, G., Van Laar, C., & Phalet, K. (2021), Majority group belonging without minority group distancing? Minority experiences of intergroup contact and inequality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 60: 121–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12382

Figures 1–3 were presented in the wrong order in the original publication. The labels and captions in the figures are correct, and so is the description of the results, but the description of the results does not correspond to the numbering of the figures.

The results illustrated in Figure 1 on p. 12 (Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact) are described in the text on p. 13 (Examining this interaction further (see Figure 2), more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of perceived unfairness (p = .003), yet minority attitudes were unrelated to majority contact when minority youth perceived less unfair treatment (p = .526). Likewise, when minority youth perceived more unfair treatment, their attitudes were significantly less positive only at higher levels of majority contact (p = .003), yet at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to perceived unfair treatment (p = .649), as being illustrated in Figure 2. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated Figure 1.

Similarly, the results illustrated in Figure 2 on p. 13 (Minority identification at low and high perceptions of unfair treatment and low and high majority contact) are described in the text on p. 14. (The same interaction was also significant on strength of minority identification (see Table 2). As Figure 3 shows, more majority contact was significantly related to weaker minority identification at higher levels of perceived unfair treatment in school (p < .001), yet majority contact was unrelated to minority identification when minority students perceived less unfairness (p = .182). Also, at high levels of majority contact, majority contact was related to weaker minority identification, although this association did not quite reach significance (p = .070).as being illustrated in Figure 3 instead of Figure 1.

Finally, the results illustrated in Figure 3 on p. 14 (Attitudes towards the minority group at low and high individual experiences of discrimination and low and high majority contact) are described in the text on p. 12. Moreover, in line with H2 on minority group distancing, there was a significant two-way interaction of positive majority contact with individual discrimination experiences on attitudes towards the minority group (see Table S3 in Appendix S1). To interpret the interaction, we tested simple effects of majority contact at high versus low levels of experienced discrimination, and of discrimination at high versus low levels of contact (-1 SD) with the Wald test, which is similar to a z-test. While more majority contact predicted significantly less positive attitudes towards the minority group at higher levels of discrimination (p < .001), this association was not significant when minorities experienced less discrimination (p = .641) (see Figure 1). Likewise, attitudes towards the minority group were less positive when minorities experienced more discrimination at higher levels of majority contact (p = .002), but at lower levels of majority contact minority attitudes were unrelated to their discrimination experiences (p = .594), as illustrated in Figure 1. Correctly, it should be described as illustrated in Figure 3.

We apologize for this error.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
9.50
自引率
7.40%
发文量
85
期刊介绍: The British Journal of Social Psychology publishes work from scholars based in all parts of the world, and manuscripts that present data on a wide range of populations inside and outside the UK. It publishes original papers in all areas of social psychology including: • social cognition • attitudes • group processes • social influence • intergroup relations • self and identity • nonverbal communication • social psychological aspects of personality, affect and emotion • language and discourse Submissions addressing these topics from a variety of approaches and methods, both quantitative and qualitative are welcomed. We publish papers of the following kinds: • empirical papers that address theoretical issues; • theoretical papers, including analyses of existing social psychological theories and presentations of theoretical innovations, extensions, or integrations; • review papers that provide an evaluation of work within a given area of social psychology and that present proposals for further research in that area; • methodological papers concerning issues that are particularly relevant to a wide range of social psychologists; • an invited agenda article as the first article in the first part of every volume. The editorial team aims to handle papers as efficiently as possible. In 2016, papers were triaged within less than a week, and the average turnaround time from receipt of the manuscript to first decision sent back to the authors was 47 days.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信