帝企鹅在深海上行捕食及其被捕食动物的反应

IF 4.4 2区 环境科学与生态学 Q1 ECOLOGY
Ecology Pub Date : 2025-05-26 DOI:10.1002/ecy.70117
Leo Uesaka, Charles André Bost, Katsufumi Sato, Kentaro Q. Sakamoto
{"title":"帝企鹅在深海上行捕食及其被捕食动物的反应","authors":"Leo Uesaka,&nbsp;Charles André Bost,&nbsp;Katsufumi Sato,&nbsp;Kentaro Q. Sakamoto","doi":"10.1002/ecy.70117","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Certain air-breathing animals, which spend much of their lives near the ocean surface or on land, are able to capture prey in the deep ocean where the energetics and the time spent foraging are limited by their lung capacity and the amount of dissolved oxygen in their blood. Animal-borne video loggers provide a valuable tool for investigating underwater foraging behavior (Davis et al., <span>1999</span>). However, capturing video footage at dark ocean depths remains challenging, particularly for penguins. While animal-borne cameras with light sources have been successfully used on large-bodied animals such as seals (Adachi et al., <span>2021</span>; Foster-Dyer et al., <span>2023</span>; Heaslip &amp; Hooker, <span>2008</span>), the smaller body size of penguins restricts the types of devices that can be attached. Although some studies have investigated penguin foraging behavior using video recordings (e.g., Ponganis et al., <span>2000</span>; Thiebault et al., <span>2019</span>; Tokunaga et al., <span>2023</span>), these recordings were not obtained in the low-light environment of the deep sea.</p><p>Here, we report a successful recording of deep-sea foraging behavior of a king penguin (<i>Aptenodytes patagonicus</i>) and the prey's response using a video logger with an LED light source (Figure 1). King penguins, the second largest species of penguin (Spheniscidae), rely on visual cues for foraging (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>; Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Martin, <span>1999</span>). They can dive to depths exceeding 100 m, well below the photic zone, in search of food (max: 343 m, Pütz &amp; Cherel, <span>2005</span>). Their diet primarily consists of myctophid fish (Myctophidae) (Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Cherel &amp; Ridoux, <span>1992</span>), the most abundant mesopelagic fish in the ocean (Catul et al., <span>2011</span>; de Busserolles et al., <span>2015</span>). Myctophids also serve as the main food source for other marine species in sub-Antarctic marine regions including southern elephant seals (<i>Mirounga leonina</i>) (Cherel et al., <span>2008</span>) and Antarctic fur seals (<i>Arctocephalus gazella</i>) (Cherel et al., <span>1997</span>). As king penguins forage within the constraints of diving duration, limited light availability, and interspecies competition, their foraging strategy provides a unique model for studying the complex challenges of deep-sea foraging. Additionally, video footage of predation events allows for the analysis of prey response behavior in myctophids to king penguins, which remains largely unexplored (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>).</p><p>In February 2024, we fitted five king penguins breeding at the Ratmanoff colony, Kerguelen Archipelago (southern Indian Ocean, 49°14'S; 70°33'E), with digital video loggers that included red LED light sources (LoggLaw-CAM, Biologging Solutions Inc., Japan, 38 mm diameter, 67 mm length, 128 g, 30 fps, full HD). These loggers also recorded depth every second and were programmed to start video recording upon reaching 80 m depth using the depth sensor trigger function. They were attached to the penguin's back using waterproof tape (Tesa, Germany), Loctite glue (Henkel, Germany), and plastic cable ties (Hellermann Tyton, UK). See Appendix S1: Section S1 for details on the potential effects of the light source on penguin and prey behavior. Four penguins were recaptured, and their loggers were retrieved. One of these penguins repeatedly dived for food during the video recording period (1 h 32 min), which occurred at dawn when king penguins typically begin foraging actively (Watanabe et al., <span>2023</span>). The penguin was on a seven-day foraging trip, with the video recording occurring on the third day, midway through its trip. Although penguins are highly hydrodynamic and the attached logger may have affected the individual's performance, its trip duration, dive depth (mean: 145 m, <i>n</i> = 704), and dive duration (mean: 4.5 min, <i>n</i> = 704) derived from the full trip data were comparable to those reported in previous studies (Charrassin et al., <span>2002</span>; Pütz et al., <span>1998</span>; Scheffer et al., <span>2016</span>). The other loggers failed to function the depth trigger correctly, and no foraging dives were recorded.</p><p>During the video recording period, the penguin made 12 foraging dives (mean dive depth ± SD: 128 ± 31 m) including 10 complete dives and 2 that were only partially recorded (Figure 2a). The penguin exhibited 136 foraging attempts, successfully capturing prey in 118 cases (86.8%). In unsuccessful attempts, the penguin either missed its prey or else detected it and stretched its neck but did not attempt to peck. According to the depth profile, 111 foraging attempts occurred during the penguin's ascent (Figure 2b,c), accounting for 81.6% of total attempts.</p><p>All prey silhouettes in the recordings resembled fish, consistent with findings that the summer diet of king penguins in this region primarily consists of myctophids, specifically <i>Krefftichthys anderssoni</i> (Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Cherel &amp; Ridoux, <span>1992</span>). The silhouettes were approximately 40–50 mm in length based on a comparison with the king penguin's beak (~85 mm). Small particles, often referred to as marine snow, were consistently present in the field of view (Video S1).</p><p>From the video footage, the exact moment when the penguin began targeting prey was distinguishable in 16 cases (Video S2). The penguin started targeting 0.5–1.5 s before capture by directing its head toward the prey, corresponding to a distance of 1–3 m given that the mean cruising speed of king penguins is approximately 2.0 m/s (Culik et al., <span>1996</span>; Sato et al., <span>2010</span>). At the point of capture, the penguin extended its long, flexible neck (Guinard et al., <span>2010</span>) by approximately 10–20 cm to make adjustments by straightening its head toward the prey regardless of direction.</p><p>The recorded fish were either stationary or swimming at a slower speed than the penguin, resulting in minimal positional changes within the frame. Regardless of movement, the fish exhibited similar frequencies of evasive (55 cases) and non-evasive (57 cases) behaviors (Appendix S1: Figure S1; Video S3). The remaining 24 cases were unclear, preventing observation of the prey's reaction. Evasive responses, if exhibited, initiated either within the same video frame as capture or 1–3 frames (i.e., 0.03–0.10 s) before capture, typically too late for the fish to escape. In these instances, just before capture, the fish's body assumed a C-shape, a commonly observed rapid evasive maneuver (Domenici &amp; Blake, <span>1997</span>; Noda et al., <span>2014</span>). In 9 of the 55 evasive cases, the fish successfully avoided capture. However, even without evasive behavior by the fish, the penguin occasionally missed its prey (8 of 57 cases). In most of the instances, the penguin extended its neck but did not attempt to capture the prey, rather than failing due to an unsuccessful strike. After missing prey, the penguin did not pursue further or persisted in re-attempting to capture the fish (cf. Tokunaga et al., <span>2023</span>). Based on the stability of the camera angle and the sound recorded by the microphone in the logger, the penguin frequently adjusted its swimming direction. Although swimming speed can vary from 1.0 to 3.0 m/s (Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>), such variation was not clearly discernible from the video footage.</p><p>Prey–predator interactions in low-light conditions provide crucial insights into the predation strategy of air-breathing animals and the survival tactics of the prey in the deep sea. Although some prey capture attempts undoubtedly occurred outside the camera's view, the high foraging success rate observed in the recorded footage (86.8%) is remarkable, highlighting the need for further video-based approaches.</p><p>From the analyzed video footage, particularly the timing at which the penguin oriented its neck toward the prey, we inferred that king penguins recognize the presence of fish from a distance of at least 1–3 m. Moreover, since targeting occurs after the initial recognition of prey, penguins may detect fish even earlier because they can dilate their pupils as widely as nocturnal owls to maintain vision in dark environments (Martin, <span>1999</span>).</p><p>The idea that king penguins tend to forage during their ascents has been suggested in previous research (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>; Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>); however, visual evidence indicating the precise moments of foraging has been lacking. Although data were obtained from only one individual, our video footage strongly supports this idea. The dive profile obtained in our study shows that the penguin repeatedly ascended and descended during the bottom phase, a behavior described as “wiggle” (Bost et al., <span>2007</span>), allowing the penguin to remain within a specific depth range where prey is abundant even while foraging during ascent.</p><p>This strategy provides several benefits. One is the counter-shading effect, which helps penguins detect fish silhouettes against the bright surface (Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>, <span>2001</span>). Additionally, approaching from the darker, deeper side of the ocean may offer the advantage of reduced visual detection by fish, potentially allowing predators to remain unnoticed for longer periods. Furthermore, the air trapped in the penguin's air sacs and feathers creates positive buoyancy, making it easier to capture prey from below while ascending, as less effort is required compared to descending (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>), even though they must descend before ascending again.</p><p>In addition to penguin behavior, we also examined the behavior of the preyed-upon myctophids, an abundant species in low-light ocean zones. From the video footage, we observed that either the fish did not evade the penguin at all or else their evasion behavior started only shortly before being captured. One possible reason for this lack of evasion, or the delayed evasion, is that they were unable to detect the approaching penguin. Myctophids are also known to exhibit torpor at deep depths during the day (Gon &amp; Heemstra, <span>1990</span>). Although some myctophid species can detect bioluminescence from tens of meters away (Turner et al., <span>2009</span>), this does not apply to penguins, which do not emit light. Detecting penguins becomes even more challenging when they approach from the darker and deeper parts of the ocean. The reactions of the fish just before capture may instead be triggered by sensing turbulence or vibrations in the surrounding water caused by the approaching penguin's head.</p><p>It is important to consider that the prey's response behavior could be a strategic attempt to avoid capture (some evasions in our observations were successful). For fish with considerably slower swimming speeds (0.1–0.3 m/s, Ignatyev <span>1996</span>) than those of penguins, the optimal evasion strategy may be to dodge the penguin rather than swim away (Wilson et al., <span>2015</span>). If evasion occurs too early, it allows the penguin to adjust its direction; thus, remaining stationary until the last moment may be advantageous. However, based on the analyzed video footage, we consider it unlikely that the fish timed their evasions, as nearly all fish displaying evasive behavior were captured immediately after adopting a C-shaped posture, too late to dodge the penguin's strike.</p><p>The foraging behavior of king penguins, as observed in this study, can be described as follows: They dive to a certain depth, ascend while capturing prey, and then descend to repeat the process multiple times. During ascent, they actively adjust their swimming direction and speed to seek prey, targeting stationary or slow-moving fish (or their aggregations) from several meters away before capturing them from below. At the moment of capture, they extend their necks by approximately 10–20 cm to precisely adjust their strike. The fish remain unaware until the moment of capture or just before. If the penguins miss a prey item, they do not pursue it but instead shift their focus to another nearby fish, suggesting an abundance of fish schools in the water column. Although fish occasionally succeed in evasive maneuvers, their attempts are generally ineffective. Their primary survival strategy appears to be blending into a large group rather than escaping individually.</p><p>This study's contribution to understanding the foraging strategies of king penguins raises new questions about how animals sharing the same habitat and competing for the same food sources develop their foraging strategies based on their respective physical and locomotive characteristics. Recent studies on southern elephant seals and Antarctic fur seals, which also inhabit the sub-Antarctic marine region and feed on myctophids, have reported distinct foraging strategies (Chevallay et al., <span>2023</span>, <span>2024</span>). Antarctic fur seals actively chase evading fish, whereas southern elephant seals stealthily approach their prey from distances greater than 10 meters. Both strategies differ from that of king penguins, particularly in the timing of prey targeting and the evasive behavior of their prey. Notably, the behavior of approaching prey from below during ascent, leading to the formation of the “wiggle” pattern in the diving profile, is unique to king penguins among these three species. These differences in foraging strategies result from variations in body size, sensory capabilities, movement capabilities, and maneuverabilities. The repeated ascent-and-descent pattern employed by king penguins may be best aligned with their high maneuverability, fast swimming speed, and relatively small body size compared to other mammals. At broader spatial scales, an important question is how species differ in movement distance and prey encounter rates in the highly reduced light environment, factors that ultimately determine their overall foraging efficiency. Accumulating data from more king penguins could provide deeper insights into predator–prey behavior, allowing for a better understanding of the strategic differences among species competing for the same prey.</p><p><b>Leo Uesaka:</b> Conceptualization, resources, formal analysis and investigation, writing original draft. <b>Charles André Bost:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition. <b>Katsufumi Sato:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration. <b>Kentaro Q. Sakamoto:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p><p>Animal care was performed humanely following the rules issued by the Réserve Nationale des Terres Australes. The experiment was conducted with permission from the Animal Experimental Committee of the Atmosphere and Ocean Research Institute, University of Tokyo (permit number: P23-28), and the Préfet des Terres Australes et Antarctiques Françaises, France, after approval from the Comité National de la Protection de la Nature (CNPN 2024 annual decision).</p>","PeriodicalId":11484,"journal":{"name":"Ecology","volume":"106 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4000,"publicationDate":"2025-05-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ecy.70117","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Deep-sea ascending predation by king penguin and its prey reaction observed by animal-borne video camera\",\"authors\":\"Leo Uesaka,&nbsp;Charles André Bost,&nbsp;Katsufumi Sato,&nbsp;Kentaro Q. Sakamoto\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/ecy.70117\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Certain air-breathing animals, which spend much of their lives near the ocean surface or on land, are able to capture prey in the deep ocean where the energetics and the time spent foraging are limited by their lung capacity and the amount of dissolved oxygen in their blood. Animal-borne video loggers provide a valuable tool for investigating underwater foraging behavior (Davis et al., <span>1999</span>). However, capturing video footage at dark ocean depths remains challenging, particularly for penguins. While animal-borne cameras with light sources have been successfully used on large-bodied animals such as seals (Adachi et al., <span>2021</span>; Foster-Dyer et al., <span>2023</span>; Heaslip &amp; Hooker, <span>2008</span>), the smaller body size of penguins restricts the types of devices that can be attached. Although some studies have investigated penguin foraging behavior using video recordings (e.g., Ponganis et al., <span>2000</span>; Thiebault et al., <span>2019</span>; Tokunaga et al., <span>2023</span>), these recordings were not obtained in the low-light environment of the deep sea.</p><p>Here, we report a successful recording of deep-sea foraging behavior of a king penguin (<i>Aptenodytes patagonicus</i>) and the prey's response using a video logger with an LED light source (Figure 1). King penguins, the second largest species of penguin (Spheniscidae), rely on visual cues for foraging (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>; Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Martin, <span>1999</span>). They can dive to depths exceeding 100 m, well below the photic zone, in search of food (max: 343 m, Pütz &amp; Cherel, <span>2005</span>). Their diet primarily consists of myctophid fish (Myctophidae) (Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Cherel &amp; Ridoux, <span>1992</span>), the most abundant mesopelagic fish in the ocean (Catul et al., <span>2011</span>; de Busserolles et al., <span>2015</span>). Myctophids also serve as the main food source for other marine species in sub-Antarctic marine regions including southern elephant seals (<i>Mirounga leonina</i>) (Cherel et al., <span>2008</span>) and Antarctic fur seals (<i>Arctocephalus gazella</i>) (Cherel et al., <span>1997</span>). As king penguins forage within the constraints of diving duration, limited light availability, and interspecies competition, their foraging strategy provides a unique model for studying the complex challenges of deep-sea foraging. Additionally, video footage of predation events allows for the analysis of prey response behavior in myctophids to king penguins, which remains largely unexplored (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>).</p><p>In February 2024, we fitted five king penguins breeding at the Ratmanoff colony, Kerguelen Archipelago (southern Indian Ocean, 49°14'S; 70°33'E), with digital video loggers that included red LED light sources (LoggLaw-CAM, Biologging Solutions Inc., Japan, 38 mm diameter, 67 mm length, 128 g, 30 fps, full HD). These loggers also recorded depth every second and were programmed to start video recording upon reaching 80 m depth using the depth sensor trigger function. They were attached to the penguin's back using waterproof tape (Tesa, Germany), Loctite glue (Henkel, Germany), and plastic cable ties (Hellermann Tyton, UK). See Appendix S1: Section S1 for details on the potential effects of the light source on penguin and prey behavior. Four penguins were recaptured, and their loggers were retrieved. One of these penguins repeatedly dived for food during the video recording period (1 h 32 min), which occurred at dawn when king penguins typically begin foraging actively (Watanabe et al., <span>2023</span>). The penguin was on a seven-day foraging trip, with the video recording occurring on the third day, midway through its trip. Although penguins are highly hydrodynamic and the attached logger may have affected the individual's performance, its trip duration, dive depth (mean: 145 m, <i>n</i> = 704), and dive duration (mean: 4.5 min, <i>n</i> = 704) derived from the full trip data were comparable to those reported in previous studies (Charrassin et al., <span>2002</span>; Pütz et al., <span>1998</span>; Scheffer et al., <span>2016</span>). The other loggers failed to function the depth trigger correctly, and no foraging dives were recorded.</p><p>During the video recording period, the penguin made 12 foraging dives (mean dive depth ± SD: 128 ± 31 m) including 10 complete dives and 2 that were only partially recorded (Figure 2a). The penguin exhibited 136 foraging attempts, successfully capturing prey in 118 cases (86.8%). In unsuccessful attempts, the penguin either missed its prey or else detected it and stretched its neck but did not attempt to peck. According to the depth profile, 111 foraging attempts occurred during the penguin's ascent (Figure 2b,c), accounting for 81.6% of total attempts.</p><p>All prey silhouettes in the recordings resembled fish, consistent with findings that the summer diet of king penguins in this region primarily consists of myctophids, specifically <i>Krefftichthys anderssoni</i> (Bost et al., <span>2002</span>; Cherel &amp; Ridoux, <span>1992</span>). The silhouettes were approximately 40–50 mm in length based on a comparison with the king penguin's beak (~85 mm). Small particles, often referred to as marine snow, were consistently present in the field of view (Video S1).</p><p>From the video footage, the exact moment when the penguin began targeting prey was distinguishable in 16 cases (Video S2). The penguin started targeting 0.5–1.5 s before capture by directing its head toward the prey, corresponding to a distance of 1–3 m given that the mean cruising speed of king penguins is approximately 2.0 m/s (Culik et al., <span>1996</span>; Sato et al., <span>2010</span>). At the point of capture, the penguin extended its long, flexible neck (Guinard et al., <span>2010</span>) by approximately 10–20 cm to make adjustments by straightening its head toward the prey regardless of direction.</p><p>The recorded fish were either stationary or swimming at a slower speed than the penguin, resulting in minimal positional changes within the frame. Regardless of movement, the fish exhibited similar frequencies of evasive (55 cases) and non-evasive (57 cases) behaviors (Appendix S1: Figure S1; Video S3). The remaining 24 cases were unclear, preventing observation of the prey's reaction. Evasive responses, if exhibited, initiated either within the same video frame as capture or 1–3 frames (i.e., 0.03–0.10 s) before capture, typically too late for the fish to escape. In these instances, just before capture, the fish's body assumed a C-shape, a commonly observed rapid evasive maneuver (Domenici &amp; Blake, <span>1997</span>; Noda et al., <span>2014</span>). In 9 of the 55 evasive cases, the fish successfully avoided capture. However, even without evasive behavior by the fish, the penguin occasionally missed its prey (8 of 57 cases). In most of the instances, the penguin extended its neck but did not attempt to capture the prey, rather than failing due to an unsuccessful strike. After missing prey, the penguin did not pursue further or persisted in re-attempting to capture the fish (cf. Tokunaga et al., <span>2023</span>). Based on the stability of the camera angle and the sound recorded by the microphone in the logger, the penguin frequently adjusted its swimming direction. Although swimming speed can vary from 1.0 to 3.0 m/s (Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>), such variation was not clearly discernible from the video footage.</p><p>Prey–predator interactions in low-light conditions provide crucial insights into the predation strategy of air-breathing animals and the survival tactics of the prey in the deep sea. Although some prey capture attempts undoubtedly occurred outside the camera's view, the high foraging success rate observed in the recorded footage (86.8%) is remarkable, highlighting the need for further video-based approaches.</p><p>From the analyzed video footage, particularly the timing at which the penguin oriented its neck toward the prey, we inferred that king penguins recognize the presence of fish from a distance of at least 1–3 m. Moreover, since targeting occurs after the initial recognition of prey, penguins may detect fish even earlier because they can dilate their pupils as widely as nocturnal owls to maintain vision in dark environments (Martin, <span>1999</span>).</p><p>The idea that king penguins tend to forage during their ascents has been suggested in previous research (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>; Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>); however, visual evidence indicating the precise moments of foraging has been lacking. Although data were obtained from only one individual, our video footage strongly supports this idea. The dive profile obtained in our study shows that the penguin repeatedly ascended and descended during the bottom phase, a behavior described as “wiggle” (Bost et al., <span>2007</span>), allowing the penguin to remain within a specific depth range where prey is abundant even while foraging during ascent.</p><p>This strategy provides several benefits. One is the counter-shading effect, which helps penguins detect fish silhouettes against the bright surface (Ropert-Coudert et al., <span>2000</span>, <span>2001</span>). Additionally, approaching from the darker, deeper side of the ocean may offer the advantage of reduced visual detection by fish, potentially allowing predators to remain unnoticed for longer periods. Furthermore, the air trapped in the penguin's air sacs and feathers creates positive buoyancy, making it easier to capture prey from below while ascending, as less effort is required compared to descending (Ainley &amp; Wilson, <span>2023</span>), even though they must descend before ascending again.</p><p>In addition to penguin behavior, we also examined the behavior of the preyed-upon myctophids, an abundant species in low-light ocean zones. From the video footage, we observed that either the fish did not evade the penguin at all or else their evasion behavior started only shortly before being captured. One possible reason for this lack of evasion, or the delayed evasion, is that they were unable to detect the approaching penguin. Myctophids are also known to exhibit torpor at deep depths during the day (Gon &amp; Heemstra, <span>1990</span>). Although some myctophid species can detect bioluminescence from tens of meters away (Turner et al., <span>2009</span>), this does not apply to penguins, which do not emit light. Detecting penguins becomes even more challenging when they approach from the darker and deeper parts of the ocean. The reactions of the fish just before capture may instead be triggered by sensing turbulence or vibrations in the surrounding water caused by the approaching penguin's head.</p><p>It is important to consider that the prey's response behavior could be a strategic attempt to avoid capture (some evasions in our observations were successful). For fish with considerably slower swimming speeds (0.1–0.3 m/s, Ignatyev <span>1996</span>) than those of penguins, the optimal evasion strategy may be to dodge the penguin rather than swim away (Wilson et al., <span>2015</span>). If evasion occurs too early, it allows the penguin to adjust its direction; thus, remaining stationary until the last moment may be advantageous. However, based on the analyzed video footage, we consider it unlikely that the fish timed their evasions, as nearly all fish displaying evasive behavior were captured immediately after adopting a C-shaped posture, too late to dodge the penguin's strike.</p><p>The foraging behavior of king penguins, as observed in this study, can be described as follows: They dive to a certain depth, ascend while capturing prey, and then descend to repeat the process multiple times. During ascent, they actively adjust their swimming direction and speed to seek prey, targeting stationary or slow-moving fish (or their aggregations) from several meters away before capturing them from below. At the moment of capture, they extend their necks by approximately 10–20 cm to precisely adjust their strike. The fish remain unaware until the moment of capture or just before. If the penguins miss a prey item, they do not pursue it but instead shift their focus to another nearby fish, suggesting an abundance of fish schools in the water column. Although fish occasionally succeed in evasive maneuvers, their attempts are generally ineffective. Their primary survival strategy appears to be blending into a large group rather than escaping individually.</p><p>This study's contribution to understanding the foraging strategies of king penguins raises new questions about how animals sharing the same habitat and competing for the same food sources develop their foraging strategies based on their respective physical and locomotive characteristics. Recent studies on southern elephant seals and Antarctic fur seals, which also inhabit the sub-Antarctic marine region and feed on myctophids, have reported distinct foraging strategies (Chevallay et al., <span>2023</span>, <span>2024</span>). Antarctic fur seals actively chase evading fish, whereas southern elephant seals stealthily approach their prey from distances greater than 10 meters. Both strategies differ from that of king penguins, particularly in the timing of prey targeting and the evasive behavior of their prey. Notably, the behavior of approaching prey from below during ascent, leading to the formation of the “wiggle” pattern in the diving profile, is unique to king penguins among these three species. These differences in foraging strategies result from variations in body size, sensory capabilities, movement capabilities, and maneuverabilities. The repeated ascent-and-descent pattern employed by king penguins may be best aligned with their high maneuverability, fast swimming speed, and relatively small body size compared to other mammals. At broader spatial scales, an important question is how species differ in movement distance and prey encounter rates in the highly reduced light environment, factors that ultimately determine their overall foraging efficiency. Accumulating data from more king penguins could provide deeper insights into predator–prey behavior, allowing for a better understanding of the strategic differences among species competing for the same prey.</p><p><b>Leo Uesaka:</b> Conceptualization, resources, formal analysis and investigation, writing original draft. <b>Charles André Bost:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition. <b>Katsufumi Sato:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration. <b>Kentaro Q. Sakamoto:</b> Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p><p>Animal care was performed humanely following the rules issued by the Réserve Nationale des Terres Australes. The experiment was conducted with permission from the Animal Experimental Committee of the Atmosphere and Ocean Research Institute, University of Tokyo (permit number: P23-28), and the Préfet des Terres Australes et Antarctiques Françaises, France, after approval from the Comité National de la Protection de la Nature (CNPN 2024 annual decision).</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":11484,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Ecology\",\"volume\":\"106 5\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":4.4000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-05-26\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ecy.70117\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Ecology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"93\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecy.70117\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"环境科学与生态学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"ECOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ecology","FirstCategoryId":"93","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecy.70117","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"ECOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

某些呼吸空气的动物一生中大部分时间生活在海洋表面或陆地上,它们能够在深海中捕捉猎物。在深海中,它们的能量和觅食时间受到肺活量和血液中溶解氧含量的限制。动物传播的视频记录器为调查水下觅食行为提供了一个有价值的工具(Davis et al., 1999)。然而,在黑暗的海洋深处拍摄视频仍然具有挑战性,尤其是对企鹅来说。而带光源的动物相机已成功用于海豹等大型动物(Adachi et al., 2021;Foster-Dyer等,2023;Heaslip,Hooker, 2008),企鹅较小的体型限制了可以连接的设备类型。尽管一些研究使用视频记录调查了企鹅的觅食行为(例如,Ponganis等人,2000;Thiebault等人,2019;Tokunaga et al., 2023),这些记录不是在深海低光环境下获得的。在这里,我们报告了一个成功的记录王企鹅(Aptenodytes patagonicus)的深海觅食行为和猎物的反应使用视频记录器与LED光源(图1)。帝企鹅是企鹅中的第二大物种(企鹅科),它们依靠视觉线索觅食(Ainley &amp;威尔逊,2023;Bost et al., 2002;马丁,1999)。它们可以潜到超过100米的深度,远低于光区,以寻找食物(最大深度:343米,pz & &;Cherel, 2005)。它们的主要食物是嗜菌鱼(嗜菌鱼科)(Bost et al., 2002;Cherel,Ridoux, 1992),是海洋中最丰富的中上层鱼类(Catul et al., 2011;de Busserolles et al., 2015)。在亚南极海域,孢子虫也是其他海洋物种的主要食物来源,包括南象海豹(miounga leonina) (Cherel et al., 2008)和南极毛海豹(Arctocephalus gazella) (Cherel et al., 1997)。由于帝企鹅的觅食受到潜水时间、光照条件和物种间竞争的限制,它们的觅食策略为研究深海觅食的复杂挑战提供了一个独特的模型。此外,捕食事件的视频片段可以用来分析嗜孢子虫对王企鹅的猎物反应行为,这在很大程度上仍未被探索(Ainley &amp;威尔逊,2023)。2024年2月,我们在南印度洋,49°14'S, Kerguelen群岛的Ratmanoff企鹅群对5只王企鹅进行了测试;70°33'E),配有数字视频记录仪,包括红色LED光源(LoggLaw-CAM, Biologging Solutions Inc.,日本,直径38毫米,长度67毫米,128克,30 fps,全高清)。这些记录仪每秒记录深度,并使用深度传感器触发功能在达到80米深度时开始视频记录。它们被用防水胶带(德国德莎)、乐泰胶水(德国汉高)和塑料电缆带(英国Hellermann Tyton)固定在企鹅的背上。关于光源对企鹅和猎物行为的潜在影响,详见附录S1:第S1节。四只企鹅被重新捕获,它们的伐木者也被找回。在视频录制期间(1小时32分钟),其中一只企鹅多次潜水觅食,这发生在黎明时分,王企鹅通常开始积极觅食(Watanabe et al., 2023)。这只企鹅进行了为期7天的觅食之旅,视频是在第三天拍摄的,也就是旅程的中途。尽管企鹅具有高度的水动力,并且附带的记录仪可能会影响个体的表现,但从整个行程数据中得出的行程时长、潜水深度(平均145米,n = 704)和潜水持续时间(平均4.5分钟,n = 704)与之前的研究报告相当(Charrassin et al., 2002;p<e:1> tz et al., 1998;Scheffer et al., 2016)。其他记录器未能正确操作深度触发器,并且没有记录觅食潜水。在录像期间,企鹅共进行了12次觅食潜水(平均潜水深度±SD: 128±31 m),其中10次完全潜水,2次仅部分记录(图2a)。企鹅共觅食136次,成功捕获118次(86.8%)。在不成功的尝试中,企鹅要么错过了猎物,要么发现了猎物,伸长了脖子,但没有试图去啄。根据深度剖面,企鹅在上升过程中进行了111次觅食尝试(图2b,c),占总尝试次数的81.6%。记录中所有猎物的轮廓都与鱼类相似,这与该地区王企鹅夏季饮食主要由孢子虫组成的发现一致,特别是Krefftichthys anderssoni (Bost et al., 2002;Cherel,Ridoux, 1992)。根据与帝企鹅喙(约85毫米)的比较,轮廓长度约为40-50毫米。 通常被称为海洋雪的小颗粒始终出现在视野中(视频S1)。从视频片段中,企鹅开始瞄准猎物的确切时刻在16个案例中被区分出来(视频S2)。帝企鹅在捕获前0.5-1.5秒开始瞄准猎物,将头部指向猎物,考虑到帝企鹅的平均巡航速度约为2.0米/秒,对应的距离为1-3米(Culik et al., 1996;Sato et al., 2010)。在捕获时,企鹅将其长而灵活的脖子(Guinard et al., 2010)伸长约10-20厘米,以便无论方向如何,都将头部朝着猎物伸直,以进行调整。被记录的鱼要么静止不动,要么游得比企鹅慢,这导致画面中的位置变化最小。无论运动如何,鱼表现出相似的回避(55例)和非回避(57例)行为频率(附录S1:图S1;视频S3)。其余24个案例尚不清楚,因此无法观察猎物的反应。如果出现逃避反应,要么在捕获的同一视频帧内开始,要么在捕获前1-3帧(即0.03-0.10秒)开始,通常对鱼逃跑来说太晚了。在这些情况下,就在捕获之前,鱼的身体呈c形,这是一种常见的快速躲避动作(多梅尼奇&amp;布莱克,1997;Noda et al., 2014)。在55个逃避案例中,有9个成功地逃脱了捕获。然而,即使没有鱼的躲避行为,企鹅偶尔也会错过猎物(57例中有8例)。在大多数情况下,企鹅伸长了脖子,但并没有试图捕捉猎物,而不是因为攻击失败而失败。在失去猎物后,企鹅不会继续追逐或坚持再次尝试捕获鱼(cf. Tokunaga et al., 2023)。根据摄像机角度的稳定性和记录仪中麦克风记录的声音,企鹅会频繁地调整自己的游泳方向。虽然游泳速度可以从1.0米/秒到3.0米/秒不等(Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000),但从视频片段中并不能清楚地看出这种变化。在弱光条件下,捕食者与猎物之间的相互作用为了解呼吸空气的动物的捕食策略和深海中猎物的生存策略提供了重要的见解。尽管一些捕食尝试无疑发生在摄像机视野之外,但在记录的镜头中观察到的高觅食成功率(86.8%)是显着的,这突出了进一步基于视频的方法的必要性。从分析的视频片段中,特别是企鹅将脖子朝向猎物的时间,我们推断王企鹅在至少1-3米的距离外就能识别出鱼的存在。此外,由于定位发生在最初识别猎物之后,企鹅可能更早发现鱼,因为它们可以像夜猫子一样扩大瞳孔,在黑暗环境中保持视力(Martin, 1999)。以前的研究已经提出了王企鹅倾向于在上升过程中觅食的想法(Ainley &amp;威尔逊,2023;Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000);然而,视觉证据显示觅食的精确时刻一直缺乏。虽然数据只来自一个人,但我们的视频片段有力地支持了这一观点。在我们的研究中获得的潜水剖面显示,企鹅在底部阶段反复上升和下降,这种行为被描述为“摆动”(Bost et al., 2007),这使得企鹅即使在上升过程中觅食,也能保持在猎物丰富的特定深度范围内。这种策略提供了几个好处。一种是反阴影效应,它可以帮助企鹅在明亮的水面上发现鱼的轮廓(Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000,2001)。此外,从海洋较暗、较深的一侧靠近可能会减少鱼类的视觉检测,从而可能使捕食者在较长时间内不被发现。此外,困在企鹅气囊和羽毛中的空气产生了正浮力,这使得它在上升时更容易从下面捕获猎物,因为与下降相比,需要更少的努力(Ainley &amp;Wilson, 2023),尽管它们必须在再次上升之前下降。除了企鹅的行为,我们还研究了被捕食的孢子虫的行为,这是一种在低光海洋地区丰富的物种。从视频片段中,我们观察到,要么鱼根本没有躲避企鹅,要么它们的躲避行为是在被捕获前不久才开始的。一个可能的原因是,他们没有躲避,或者延迟躲避,是他们无法发现接近的企鹅。我们还知道,在白天的深海中,嗜芽胞菌也会表现出迟钝(Gon &amp;Heemstra, 1990)。尽管一些真菌种类可以在几十米外检测到生物发光(Turner等)。 (2009),但这并不适用于企鹅,因为企鹅不发光。当企鹅从海洋中较暗、较深的地方靠近时,探测它们变得更加困难。在被捕获之前,鱼的反应可能是由感知周围水域的湍流或震动引起的,而这些湍流或振动是由靠近的企鹅头部引起的。重要的是要考虑到猎物的反应行为可能是一种逃避捕获的战略尝试(在我们的观察中,一些逃避是成功的)。对于游泳速度比企鹅慢得多的鱼(0.1-0.3 m/s, Ignatyev 1996),最佳的逃避策略可能是躲避企鹅而不是游开(Wilson et al., 2015)。如果躲避发生得太早,企鹅就可以调整方向;因此,保持静止直到最后一刻可能是有利的。然而,根据分析的视频片段,我们认为这些鱼不太可能定时躲避,因为几乎所有表现出躲避行为的鱼都是在采取c形姿势后立即被捕获的,太晚了,无法躲避企鹅的攻击。本研究观察到的帝企鹅觅食行为可以描述为:它们潜入一定深度,在捕捉猎物的同时上升,然后下降,重复多次。在上升过程中,它们主动调整游动方向和速度来寻找猎物,在几米远的地方瞄准静止或缓慢移动的鱼(或它们的聚集),然后从下面捕获它们。在捕获的那一刻,它们将脖子伸长大约10-20厘米,以精确调整攻击。鱼在被捕获的那一刻或之前都不知道。如果企鹅错过了猎物,它们不会继续追逐,而是将注意力转移到附近的另一条鱼身上,这表明水体中有大量的鱼群。虽然鱼类偶尔会成功地逃避,但它们的尝试通常是无效的。它们的主要生存策略似乎是融入一个大群体,而不是单独逃跑。这项研究对理解王企鹅的觅食策略的贡献提出了新的问题,即共享同一栖息地并争夺同一食物来源的动物如何根据各自的身体和运动特征制定觅食策略。最近对南象海豹和南极海狗的研究报告了它们不同的觅食策略(Chevallay et al., 2023,2024),这两种海狗也栖息在亚南极海域,以嗜菌动物为食。南极海狗会积极地追逐逃跑的鱼,而南方海象则会在10米以上的距离上悄悄接近猎物。这两种策略都与帝企鹅不同,特别是在猎物瞄准的时间和猎物的躲避行为上。值得注意的是,在上升过程中从下方接近猎物的行为,导致在潜水剖面中形成“摆动”模式,这是这三个物种中王企鹅所特有的。这些觅食策略的差异是由体型、感觉能力、运动能力和机动性的差异造成的。王企鹅采用的反复上升和下降的模式可能与它们的高机动性、快速游泳速度和与其他哺乳动物相比相对较小的体型最吻合。在更广阔的空间尺度上,一个重要的问题是物种在高度减弱的光环境中如何在移动距离和猎物相遇率上有所不同,这些因素最终决定了它们的整体觅食效率。从更多的王企鹅身上收集数据可以更深入地了解捕食者-猎物的行为,从而更好地理解争夺同一猎物的物种之间的策略差异。Leo Uesaka:概念化,资源,正式分析和调查,撰写原稿。查尔斯·安德烈·博斯特:概念化、资源、审查和编辑、项目管理、资金获取。佐藤克文:概念化、资源、审查和编辑、项目管理。Kentaro Q. Sakamoto:概念化、资源、审查和编辑、项目管理、资金获取。作者声明无利益冲突。动物护理是按照澳大利亚国家土地管理局颁布的规定进行的。该实验得到了东京大学大气与海洋研究所动物实验委员会(许可号:P23-28)和法国南极和南极土地计划的许可,并得到了国家自然保护委员会(CNPN 2024年年度决定)的批准。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Deep-sea ascending predation by king penguin and its prey reaction observed by animal-borne video camera

Certain air-breathing animals, which spend much of their lives near the ocean surface or on land, are able to capture prey in the deep ocean where the energetics and the time spent foraging are limited by their lung capacity and the amount of dissolved oxygen in their blood. Animal-borne video loggers provide a valuable tool for investigating underwater foraging behavior (Davis et al., 1999). However, capturing video footage at dark ocean depths remains challenging, particularly for penguins. While animal-borne cameras with light sources have been successfully used on large-bodied animals such as seals (Adachi et al., 2021; Foster-Dyer et al., 2023; Heaslip & Hooker, 2008), the smaller body size of penguins restricts the types of devices that can be attached. Although some studies have investigated penguin foraging behavior using video recordings (e.g., Ponganis et al., 2000; Thiebault et al., 2019; Tokunaga et al., 2023), these recordings were not obtained in the low-light environment of the deep sea.

Here, we report a successful recording of deep-sea foraging behavior of a king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus) and the prey's response using a video logger with an LED light source (Figure 1). King penguins, the second largest species of penguin (Spheniscidae), rely on visual cues for foraging (Ainley & Wilson, 2023; Bost et al., 2002; Martin, 1999). They can dive to depths exceeding 100 m, well below the photic zone, in search of food (max: 343 m, Pütz & Cherel, 2005). Their diet primarily consists of myctophid fish (Myctophidae) (Bost et al., 2002; Cherel & Ridoux, 1992), the most abundant mesopelagic fish in the ocean (Catul et al., 2011; de Busserolles et al., 2015). Myctophids also serve as the main food source for other marine species in sub-Antarctic marine regions including southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) (Cherel et al., 2008) and Antarctic fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella) (Cherel et al., 1997). As king penguins forage within the constraints of diving duration, limited light availability, and interspecies competition, their foraging strategy provides a unique model for studying the complex challenges of deep-sea foraging. Additionally, video footage of predation events allows for the analysis of prey response behavior in myctophids to king penguins, which remains largely unexplored (Ainley & Wilson, 2023).

In February 2024, we fitted five king penguins breeding at the Ratmanoff colony, Kerguelen Archipelago (southern Indian Ocean, 49°14'S; 70°33'E), with digital video loggers that included red LED light sources (LoggLaw-CAM, Biologging Solutions Inc., Japan, 38 mm diameter, 67 mm length, 128 g, 30 fps, full HD). These loggers also recorded depth every second and were programmed to start video recording upon reaching 80 m depth using the depth sensor trigger function. They were attached to the penguin's back using waterproof tape (Tesa, Germany), Loctite glue (Henkel, Germany), and plastic cable ties (Hellermann Tyton, UK). See Appendix S1: Section S1 for details on the potential effects of the light source on penguin and prey behavior. Four penguins were recaptured, and their loggers were retrieved. One of these penguins repeatedly dived for food during the video recording period (1 h 32 min), which occurred at dawn when king penguins typically begin foraging actively (Watanabe et al., 2023). The penguin was on a seven-day foraging trip, with the video recording occurring on the third day, midway through its trip. Although penguins are highly hydrodynamic and the attached logger may have affected the individual's performance, its trip duration, dive depth (mean: 145 m, n = 704), and dive duration (mean: 4.5 min, n = 704) derived from the full trip data were comparable to those reported in previous studies (Charrassin et al., 2002; Pütz et al., 1998; Scheffer et al., 2016). The other loggers failed to function the depth trigger correctly, and no foraging dives were recorded.

During the video recording period, the penguin made 12 foraging dives (mean dive depth ± SD: 128 ± 31 m) including 10 complete dives and 2 that were only partially recorded (Figure 2a). The penguin exhibited 136 foraging attempts, successfully capturing prey in 118 cases (86.8%). In unsuccessful attempts, the penguin either missed its prey or else detected it and stretched its neck but did not attempt to peck. According to the depth profile, 111 foraging attempts occurred during the penguin's ascent (Figure 2b,c), accounting for 81.6% of total attempts.

All prey silhouettes in the recordings resembled fish, consistent with findings that the summer diet of king penguins in this region primarily consists of myctophids, specifically Krefftichthys anderssoni (Bost et al., 2002; Cherel & Ridoux, 1992). The silhouettes were approximately 40–50 mm in length based on a comparison with the king penguin's beak (~85 mm). Small particles, often referred to as marine snow, were consistently present in the field of view (Video S1).

From the video footage, the exact moment when the penguin began targeting prey was distinguishable in 16 cases (Video S2). The penguin started targeting 0.5–1.5 s before capture by directing its head toward the prey, corresponding to a distance of 1–3 m given that the mean cruising speed of king penguins is approximately 2.0 m/s (Culik et al., 1996; Sato et al., 2010). At the point of capture, the penguin extended its long, flexible neck (Guinard et al., 2010) by approximately 10–20 cm to make adjustments by straightening its head toward the prey regardless of direction.

The recorded fish were either stationary or swimming at a slower speed than the penguin, resulting in minimal positional changes within the frame. Regardless of movement, the fish exhibited similar frequencies of evasive (55 cases) and non-evasive (57 cases) behaviors (Appendix S1: Figure S1; Video S3). The remaining 24 cases were unclear, preventing observation of the prey's reaction. Evasive responses, if exhibited, initiated either within the same video frame as capture or 1–3 frames (i.e., 0.03–0.10 s) before capture, typically too late for the fish to escape. In these instances, just before capture, the fish's body assumed a C-shape, a commonly observed rapid evasive maneuver (Domenici & Blake, 1997; Noda et al., 2014). In 9 of the 55 evasive cases, the fish successfully avoided capture. However, even without evasive behavior by the fish, the penguin occasionally missed its prey (8 of 57 cases). In most of the instances, the penguin extended its neck but did not attempt to capture the prey, rather than failing due to an unsuccessful strike. After missing prey, the penguin did not pursue further or persisted in re-attempting to capture the fish (cf. Tokunaga et al., 2023). Based on the stability of the camera angle and the sound recorded by the microphone in the logger, the penguin frequently adjusted its swimming direction. Although swimming speed can vary from 1.0 to 3.0 m/s (Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000), such variation was not clearly discernible from the video footage.

Prey–predator interactions in low-light conditions provide crucial insights into the predation strategy of air-breathing animals and the survival tactics of the prey in the deep sea. Although some prey capture attempts undoubtedly occurred outside the camera's view, the high foraging success rate observed in the recorded footage (86.8%) is remarkable, highlighting the need for further video-based approaches.

From the analyzed video footage, particularly the timing at which the penguin oriented its neck toward the prey, we inferred that king penguins recognize the presence of fish from a distance of at least 1–3 m. Moreover, since targeting occurs after the initial recognition of prey, penguins may detect fish even earlier because they can dilate their pupils as widely as nocturnal owls to maintain vision in dark environments (Martin, 1999).

The idea that king penguins tend to forage during their ascents has been suggested in previous research (Ainley & Wilson, 2023; Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000); however, visual evidence indicating the precise moments of foraging has been lacking. Although data were obtained from only one individual, our video footage strongly supports this idea. The dive profile obtained in our study shows that the penguin repeatedly ascended and descended during the bottom phase, a behavior described as “wiggle” (Bost et al., 2007), allowing the penguin to remain within a specific depth range where prey is abundant even while foraging during ascent.

This strategy provides several benefits. One is the counter-shading effect, which helps penguins detect fish silhouettes against the bright surface (Ropert-Coudert et al., 2000, 2001). Additionally, approaching from the darker, deeper side of the ocean may offer the advantage of reduced visual detection by fish, potentially allowing predators to remain unnoticed for longer periods. Furthermore, the air trapped in the penguin's air sacs and feathers creates positive buoyancy, making it easier to capture prey from below while ascending, as less effort is required compared to descending (Ainley & Wilson, 2023), even though they must descend before ascending again.

In addition to penguin behavior, we also examined the behavior of the preyed-upon myctophids, an abundant species in low-light ocean zones. From the video footage, we observed that either the fish did not evade the penguin at all or else their evasion behavior started only shortly before being captured. One possible reason for this lack of evasion, or the delayed evasion, is that they were unable to detect the approaching penguin. Myctophids are also known to exhibit torpor at deep depths during the day (Gon & Heemstra, 1990). Although some myctophid species can detect bioluminescence from tens of meters away (Turner et al., 2009), this does not apply to penguins, which do not emit light. Detecting penguins becomes even more challenging when they approach from the darker and deeper parts of the ocean. The reactions of the fish just before capture may instead be triggered by sensing turbulence or vibrations in the surrounding water caused by the approaching penguin's head.

It is important to consider that the prey's response behavior could be a strategic attempt to avoid capture (some evasions in our observations were successful). For fish with considerably slower swimming speeds (0.1–0.3 m/s, Ignatyev 1996) than those of penguins, the optimal evasion strategy may be to dodge the penguin rather than swim away (Wilson et al., 2015). If evasion occurs too early, it allows the penguin to adjust its direction; thus, remaining stationary until the last moment may be advantageous. However, based on the analyzed video footage, we consider it unlikely that the fish timed their evasions, as nearly all fish displaying evasive behavior were captured immediately after adopting a C-shaped posture, too late to dodge the penguin's strike.

The foraging behavior of king penguins, as observed in this study, can be described as follows: They dive to a certain depth, ascend while capturing prey, and then descend to repeat the process multiple times. During ascent, they actively adjust their swimming direction and speed to seek prey, targeting stationary or slow-moving fish (or their aggregations) from several meters away before capturing them from below. At the moment of capture, they extend their necks by approximately 10–20 cm to precisely adjust their strike. The fish remain unaware until the moment of capture or just before. If the penguins miss a prey item, they do not pursue it but instead shift their focus to another nearby fish, suggesting an abundance of fish schools in the water column. Although fish occasionally succeed in evasive maneuvers, their attempts are generally ineffective. Their primary survival strategy appears to be blending into a large group rather than escaping individually.

This study's contribution to understanding the foraging strategies of king penguins raises new questions about how animals sharing the same habitat and competing for the same food sources develop their foraging strategies based on their respective physical and locomotive characteristics. Recent studies on southern elephant seals and Antarctic fur seals, which also inhabit the sub-Antarctic marine region and feed on myctophids, have reported distinct foraging strategies (Chevallay et al., 2023, 2024). Antarctic fur seals actively chase evading fish, whereas southern elephant seals stealthily approach their prey from distances greater than 10 meters. Both strategies differ from that of king penguins, particularly in the timing of prey targeting and the evasive behavior of their prey. Notably, the behavior of approaching prey from below during ascent, leading to the formation of the “wiggle” pattern in the diving profile, is unique to king penguins among these three species. These differences in foraging strategies result from variations in body size, sensory capabilities, movement capabilities, and maneuverabilities. The repeated ascent-and-descent pattern employed by king penguins may be best aligned with their high maneuverability, fast swimming speed, and relatively small body size compared to other mammals. At broader spatial scales, an important question is how species differ in movement distance and prey encounter rates in the highly reduced light environment, factors that ultimately determine their overall foraging efficiency. Accumulating data from more king penguins could provide deeper insights into predator–prey behavior, allowing for a better understanding of the strategic differences among species competing for the same prey.

Leo Uesaka: Conceptualization, resources, formal analysis and investigation, writing original draft. Charles André Bost: Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition. Katsufumi Sato: Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration. Kentaro Q. Sakamoto: Conceptualization, resources, review and editing, project administration, funding acquisition.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Animal care was performed humanely following the rules issued by the Réserve Nationale des Terres Australes. The experiment was conducted with permission from the Animal Experimental Committee of the Atmosphere and Ocean Research Institute, University of Tokyo (permit number: P23-28), and the Préfet des Terres Australes et Antarctiques Françaises, France, after approval from the Comité National de la Protection de la Nature (CNPN 2024 annual decision).

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Ecology
Ecology 环境科学-生态学
CiteScore
8.30
自引率
2.10%
发文量
332
审稿时长
3 months
期刊介绍: Ecology publishes articles that report on the basic elements of ecological research. Emphasis is placed on concise, clear articles documenting important ecological phenomena. The journal publishes a broad array of research that includes a rapidly expanding envelope of subject matter, techniques, approaches, and concepts: paleoecology through present-day phenomena; evolutionary, population, physiological, community, and ecosystem ecology, as well as biogeochemistry; inclusive of descriptive, comparative, experimental, mathematical, statistical, and interdisciplinary approaches.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信