辩诉交易决策中的框架偏见:来自医疗决策心理学的见解

IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED
Christopher R. Wolfe
{"title":"辩诉交易决策中的框架偏见:来自医疗决策心理学的见解","authors":"Christopher R. Wolfe","doi":"10.1002/bdm.70018","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The vast majority of criminal cases in the United States are resolved by plea bargains, yet the philosophical underpinnings of “bargaining in the shadow of trial” are a strong version of rationality that appears to be at odds with research conducted by cognitive psychologists on framing. Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) provide empirical evidence that directly demonstrates that the way identical plea bargain options are framed has a significant influence on decision making. Of course, even the best of studies are not in and of themselves the final word on a topic of this importance. With a dearth of research on framing effects in plea bargaining, it seems useful to consider relevant research in related domains. The psychology of medical decision making, especially judgments and decisions made by patients in consultation with healthcare providers, is ripe for instructive comparisons. Like the decisions of criminal defendants, medical decisions are among the most important ordinary people can make. Such decisions are often made between unfavorable options (e.g., chemotherapy or invasive surgery), in unfamiliar and complex knowledge domains, and with the assistance of professionals who may not be fully trusted. Yet, they are also distinct from the well-worn lessons of market economics.</p><p>Given these structural parallels, it is worth asking whether the way options and decision-relevant information are framed have any measurable influence on judgments and decisions in medicine. The answer is decisively yes. For example, Armstrong et al. (<span>2002</span>) presented participants with graphs of curves representing how long people live after being treated for a disease over several years. These representations are useful, for example, in comparing different treatment options. Two mathematically identical ways of presenting the same data are survival curves, showing how many people are still alive after different time intervals, and mortality curves, showing how many people have died after those same intervals. Despite their mathematical equivalence, Armstrong et al. (<span>2002</span>) found that participants who were given survival curves (or both survival and mortality curves together) preferred preventive surgery significantly more often, and were significantly more accurate in answering knowledge questions, than participants who only received mortality curves. Donovan and Jalleh (<span>2000</span>) told participants about a hypothetical immunization that protected infants against respiratory problems. Side effects were framed either negatively (10% chance of side effects) or positively (90% chance of no side effects). They found that positive framing yielded better performance for participants without a young infant at home. Farrell et al. (<span>2001</span>) presented information to participants about the safety of donated blood in either a gain frame (lives saved), a loss frame (lives lost), or a combined loss frame presented in a positive context. They found that participants in the gain-frame condition were significantly more confident of blood safety for transfusions than in the loss-frame or combined conditions. These findings are far from unique. The Making Numbers Meaningful systematic literature review covers dozens of published studies demonstrating framing effects in judgment and decision making in medical contexts (see Ancker et al. <span>2025</span>; Zikmund-Fisher and Ancker <span>2025</span> and other related articles in the same issue of <i>Medical Decision Making Policy &amp; Practice</i> for a detailed review of the literature). My own lived experience as a survivor of Stage III renal cell carcinoma is quite consistent with these findings. An 80% chance of survival <i>feels</i> different than a 20% chance of dying—even for a reasonably numerate JDM researcher.</p><p>There are several take-aways from the literature on framing effects in medical judgment and decision making relevant to plea bargaining. First, they are pervasive, encompassing a number of dimensions including probability, risk, and the quality of outcomes. Thus, with respect to decisions about plea bargaining, in addition to the effects Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) found for time served, it is reasonable to predict comparable framing effects for other decision-relevant judgments including the likelihood of conviction, time pressure before a plea deal “goes off the table,” and the harshness of detention facilities. Second, framing effects often interact with other factors including participant characteristics. Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) discovered that criminal history interacts with framing due to the presumed higher propensity for risk taking of those who acknowledge previous criminal behavior. It would not be surprising if other individual differences such as tolerance for ambiguity and need for cognition also mediated or moderated framing effects in plea bargaining. Third, simply presenting two opposite frames together (e.g. lives saved and lives lost) has been surprisingly ineffective in medical contexts. It is therefore unlikely that a simple fix such as this will prove sufficient in future research and practice related to plea bargain decisions. Finally, theoretically based cognitive interventions have been found to reduce framing effects in medical decision making. These interventions have theoretical as well as practical implications for plea bargaining.</p><p>Some of the demonstrably effective interventions in medical decision making are based on the fuzzy-trace theory notion that people make judgments and decisions based on a mental representation of the bottom-line meaning—or gist—of decision relevant information (Reyna <span>2008</span>; Reyna et al. <span>2022</span>). For example, people make decisions differently when medical risk information is presented as absolute values compared to relative risk, and the use of stacked bar graphs to convey risk information improves judgments of medical risk (Brust-Renck et al. <span>2013</span>). In my own research on perhaps the first web-based Intelligent Tutoring Systems to help women understand and make decisions about genetic cancer risk, icon arrays (sometimes called bathroom figures) along with other gist-based visual representations, gist-based tutorial dialogues, and gist explanations significantly improved risk assessment, knowledge, and comprehension (Wolfe et al. <span>2015</span>; Wolfe et al. <span>2016</span>). Another risk-related judgment is when two options are roughly the same or approximately equal in riskiness. This is important, for example, when two medical treatments are approximately equal in efficacy, but one has more serious side effects than the other. Here too, the way risks are framed influences judgment. However, a simple verbal intervention, “Think about the bottom-line gist. Is this a meaningful difference …” (Wolfe et al. <span>2018</span>, 158), significantly increased appropriate approximately equal judgments, as did semantic context and source attribution. Given the evidence from the realm of medical decision making, comparable techniques involving asking defendants for gist explanations of plea-related decisions, simple gist-invoking wording, and even images to counteract quantitative framing effects are all promising approaches to helping defendants understand the gist of information related to plea bargains.</p><p>The US public seems deeply divided over whether too many people are suffering imprisonment in an age of mass incarceration (see Fleetwood <span>2020</span> for a deeply humanistic treatment) or whether the justice systems coddles criminals (“hugs for thugs”) who should be kept off the streets for as long as possible. Yet surely we can agree that it would be incredibly expensive if every criminal case that was currently resolved via plea bargaining went to trial. The American tradition of judicial fairness demands that people understand what they are agreeing to before waiving their right to a trial by a jury of their peers. Adherents to a belief in the strong form of human rationality sometimes argue that without this assumption, the very basis for the right to autonomous legal decision making disappears. However, decades of research in cognitive psychology, including multidisciplinary research on medical decision making, suggest that people who understand the bottom-line meaning of their circumstances can, and regularly do, make good decisions aligned with their own goals and values, about important and complex issues. This is the case with complex decisions such as whether to undergo invasive surgery, and it should be the case with decisions about plea bargaining too. Although there are lively debates in the field of judgment and decision making, I am persuaded that people are most likely to make judgments and decisions in their own best interest when they understand the bottom-line meaning (i.e., have a sound gist mental representation) of the decision-relevant information at hand. If I had my way, officers of the court, including public defenders and prosecutors, would come to understand that they have an obligation to ensure that defendants actually understand the gist of plea agreements, however framed. These mental representations would include a gist understanding of the prevalence of plea bargaining in American jurisprudence and the likelihood and nature of positive and negative outcomes in both chosen and relinquished decision options. The more judges, attorneys, and prosecutors understand the way people actually make life-altering decisions, the better positioned they will be to ensure that every time a defendant pleads guilty as part of a plea bargain, justice is served.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":"38 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2025-04-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.70018","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Framing Biases in Plea Bargaining Decisions: Insights From the Psychology of Medical Decision Making\",\"authors\":\"Christopher R. Wolfe\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/bdm.70018\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>The vast majority of criminal cases in the United States are resolved by plea bargains, yet the philosophical underpinnings of “bargaining in the shadow of trial” are a strong version of rationality that appears to be at odds with research conducted by cognitive psychologists on framing. Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) provide empirical evidence that directly demonstrates that the way identical plea bargain options are framed has a significant influence on decision making. Of course, even the best of studies are not in and of themselves the final word on a topic of this importance. With a dearth of research on framing effects in plea bargaining, it seems useful to consider relevant research in related domains. The psychology of medical decision making, especially judgments and decisions made by patients in consultation with healthcare providers, is ripe for instructive comparisons. Like the decisions of criminal defendants, medical decisions are among the most important ordinary people can make. Such decisions are often made between unfavorable options (e.g., chemotherapy or invasive surgery), in unfamiliar and complex knowledge domains, and with the assistance of professionals who may not be fully trusted. Yet, they are also distinct from the well-worn lessons of market economics.</p><p>Given these structural parallels, it is worth asking whether the way options and decision-relevant information are framed have any measurable influence on judgments and decisions in medicine. The answer is decisively yes. For example, Armstrong et al. (<span>2002</span>) presented participants with graphs of curves representing how long people live after being treated for a disease over several years. These representations are useful, for example, in comparing different treatment options. Two mathematically identical ways of presenting the same data are survival curves, showing how many people are still alive after different time intervals, and mortality curves, showing how many people have died after those same intervals. Despite their mathematical equivalence, Armstrong et al. (<span>2002</span>) found that participants who were given survival curves (or both survival and mortality curves together) preferred preventive surgery significantly more often, and were significantly more accurate in answering knowledge questions, than participants who only received mortality curves. Donovan and Jalleh (<span>2000</span>) told participants about a hypothetical immunization that protected infants against respiratory problems. Side effects were framed either negatively (10% chance of side effects) or positively (90% chance of no side effects). They found that positive framing yielded better performance for participants without a young infant at home. Farrell et al. (<span>2001</span>) presented information to participants about the safety of donated blood in either a gain frame (lives saved), a loss frame (lives lost), or a combined loss frame presented in a positive context. They found that participants in the gain-frame condition were significantly more confident of blood safety for transfusions than in the loss-frame or combined conditions. These findings are far from unique. The Making Numbers Meaningful systematic literature review covers dozens of published studies demonstrating framing effects in judgment and decision making in medical contexts (see Ancker et al. <span>2025</span>; Zikmund-Fisher and Ancker <span>2025</span> and other related articles in the same issue of <i>Medical Decision Making Policy &amp; Practice</i> for a detailed review of the literature). My own lived experience as a survivor of Stage III renal cell carcinoma is quite consistent with these findings. An 80% chance of survival <i>feels</i> different than a 20% chance of dying—even for a reasonably numerate JDM researcher.</p><p>There are several take-aways from the literature on framing effects in medical judgment and decision making relevant to plea bargaining. First, they are pervasive, encompassing a number of dimensions including probability, risk, and the quality of outcomes. Thus, with respect to decisions about plea bargaining, in addition to the effects Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) found for time served, it is reasonable to predict comparable framing effects for other decision-relevant judgments including the likelihood of conviction, time pressure before a plea deal “goes off the table,” and the harshness of detention facilities. Second, framing effects often interact with other factors including participant characteristics. Reyna et al. (<span>2025</span>) discovered that criminal history interacts with framing due to the presumed higher propensity for risk taking of those who acknowledge previous criminal behavior. It would not be surprising if other individual differences such as tolerance for ambiguity and need for cognition also mediated or moderated framing effects in plea bargaining. Third, simply presenting two opposite frames together (e.g. lives saved and lives lost) has been surprisingly ineffective in medical contexts. It is therefore unlikely that a simple fix such as this will prove sufficient in future research and practice related to plea bargain decisions. Finally, theoretically based cognitive interventions have been found to reduce framing effects in medical decision making. These interventions have theoretical as well as practical implications for plea bargaining.</p><p>Some of the demonstrably effective interventions in medical decision making are based on the fuzzy-trace theory notion that people make judgments and decisions based on a mental representation of the bottom-line meaning—or gist—of decision relevant information (Reyna <span>2008</span>; Reyna et al. <span>2022</span>). For example, people make decisions differently when medical risk information is presented as absolute values compared to relative risk, and the use of stacked bar graphs to convey risk information improves judgments of medical risk (Brust-Renck et al. <span>2013</span>). In my own research on perhaps the first web-based Intelligent Tutoring Systems to help women understand and make decisions about genetic cancer risk, icon arrays (sometimes called bathroom figures) along with other gist-based visual representations, gist-based tutorial dialogues, and gist explanations significantly improved risk assessment, knowledge, and comprehension (Wolfe et al. <span>2015</span>; Wolfe et al. <span>2016</span>). Another risk-related judgment is when two options are roughly the same or approximately equal in riskiness. This is important, for example, when two medical treatments are approximately equal in efficacy, but one has more serious side effects than the other. Here too, the way risks are framed influences judgment. However, a simple verbal intervention, “Think about the bottom-line gist. Is this a meaningful difference …” (Wolfe et al. <span>2018</span>, 158), significantly increased appropriate approximately equal judgments, as did semantic context and source attribution. Given the evidence from the realm of medical decision making, comparable techniques involving asking defendants for gist explanations of plea-related decisions, simple gist-invoking wording, and even images to counteract quantitative framing effects are all promising approaches to helping defendants understand the gist of information related to plea bargains.</p><p>The US public seems deeply divided over whether too many people are suffering imprisonment in an age of mass incarceration (see Fleetwood <span>2020</span> for a deeply humanistic treatment) or whether the justice systems coddles criminals (“hugs for thugs”) who should be kept off the streets for as long as possible. Yet surely we can agree that it would be incredibly expensive if every criminal case that was currently resolved via plea bargaining went to trial. The American tradition of judicial fairness demands that people understand what they are agreeing to before waiving their right to a trial by a jury of their peers. Adherents to a belief in the strong form of human rationality sometimes argue that without this assumption, the very basis for the right to autonomous legal decision making disappears. However, decades of research in cognitive psychology, including multidisciplinary research on medical decision making, suggest that people who understand the bottom-line meaning of their circumstances can, and regularly do, make good decisions aligned with their own goals and values, about important and complex issues. This is the case with complex decisions such as whether to undergo invasive surgery, and it should be the case with decisions about plea bargaining too. Although there are lively debates in the field of judgment and decision making, I am persuaded that people are most likely to make judgments and decisions in their own best interest when they understand the bottom-line meaning (i.e., have a sound gist mental representation) of the decision-relevant information at hand. If I had my way, officers of the court, including public defenders and prosecutors, would come to understand that they have an obligation to ensure that defendants actually understand the gist of plea agreements, however framed. These mental representations would include a gist understanding of the prevalence of plea bargaining in American jurisprudence and the likelihood and nature of positive and negative outcomes in both chosen and relinquished decision options. The more judges, attorneys, and prosecutors understand the way people actually make life-altering decisions, the better positioned they will be to ensure that every time a defendant pleads guilty as part of a plea bargain, justice is served.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":48112,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making\",\"volume\":\"38 2\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-04-14\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.70018\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bdm.70018\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bdm.70018","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

美国绝大多数刑事案件都是通过辩诉交易解决的,然而 "审判阴影下的讨价还价 "的哲学基础是一种强烈的理性,这似乎与认知心理学家关于框架的研究相悖。雷纳等人(2025 年)提供的经验证据直接表明,相同的认罪求情协议选项的框定方式对决策有重大影响。当然,对于如此重要的课题,即使是最好的研究本身也不能一锤定音。由于有关辩诉交易中框架效应的研究十分匮乏,我们不妨考虑一下相关领域的相关研究。医疗决策心理学,尤其是患者在咨询医疗服务提供者时做出的判断和决定,已经成熟,可以进行有启发性的比较。与刑事被告的决定一样,医疗决定也是普通人能够做出的最重要的决定之一。这些决定往往是在不利的选择(如化疗或侵入性手术)之间、在不熟悉的复杂知识领域、在可能不完全信任的专业人员的协助下做出的。鉴于这些结构上的相似之处,我们不禁要问,制定选择方案和决策相关信息的方式是否会对医学判断和决策产生可衡量的影响。答案是肯定的。例如,Armstrong 等人(2002 年)向参与者展示了一些曲线图,这些曲线代表了人们在接受某种疾病治疗后的几年内的寿命。例如,这些图表在比较不同的治疗方案时非常有用。生存曲线和死亡率曲线是展示相同数据的两种数学上相同的方法,前者表示在不同的时间间隔后有多少人仍然活着,后者则表示在相同的时间间隔后有多少人已经死亡。尽管两者在数学上等同,但 Armstrong 等人(2002 年)发现,获得生存曲线(或同时获得生存曲线和死亡率曲线)的参与者比只获得死亡率曲线的参与者更倾向于选择预防性手术,而且在回答知识问题时也更准确。Donovan 和 Jalleh(2000 年)向参与者介绍了一种假定的免疫接种方法,可以保护婴儿免受呼吸道疾病的困扰。他们对副作用进行了负面(10% 的副作用几率)或正面(90% 的几率没有副作用)的描述。他们发现,对于家中没有年幼婴儿的参与者来说,正面描述的效果更好。Farrell 等人(2001 年)用收益框架(挽救生命)、损失框架(失去生命)或在积极情境下呈现的综合损失框架向参与者提供有关献血安全性的信息。他们发现,收益框架条件下的参与者对输血安全的信心明显高于损失框架或综合框架条件下的参与者。这些发现并非独一无二。让数字变得有意义》系统性文献综述涵盖了数十项已发表的研究,这些研究证明了框架效应在医疗背景下的判断和决策中的作用(详细文献综述见 Ancker 等人 2025 年;Zikmund-Fisher 和 Ancker 2025 年,以及同一期《医疗决策政策与实践》中的其他相关文章)。作为肾细胞癌 III 期的幸存者,我的亲身经历与这些研究结果十分吻合。80%的存活几率与20%的死亡几率给人的感觉是不同的--即使是对于一个对数字相当敏感的JDM研究者来说也是如此。首先,框架效应是普遍存在的,包括概率、风险和结果质量等多个方面。因此,关于认罪求情协议的决策,除了雷纳等人(2025 年)发现的服刑时间效应外,还可以合理预测其他决策相关判断的类似框架效应,包括定罪的可能性、认罪求情协议 "下台 "前的时间压力以及拘留设施的苛刻程度。其次,框架效应往往与包括参与者特征在内的其他因素相互作用。Reyna 等人(2025 年)发现,犯罪史与框架效应相互作用,这是因为那些承认有犯罪前科的人被认为更倾向于承担风险。如果其他个体差异(如对模糊性的容忍度和对认知的需求)也能在认罪求情协议的框架效应中起中介或调节作用,那就不足为奇了。第三,简单地将两个相反的框架(如挽救的生命和失去的生命)放在一起,在医疗环境中的效果令人惊讶。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Framing Biases in Plea Bargaining Decisions: Insights From the Psychology of Medical Decision Making

The vast majority of criminal cases in the United States are resolved by plea bargains, yet the philosophical underpinnings of “bargaining in the shadow of trial” are a strong version of rationality that appears to be at odds with research conducted by cognitive psychologists on framing. Reyna et al. (2025) provide empirical evidence that directly demonstrates that the way identical plea bargain options are framed has a significant influence on decision making. Of course, even the best of studies are not in and of themselves the final word on a topic of this importance. With a dearth of research on framing effects in plea bargaining, it seems useful to consider relevant research in related domains. The psychology of medical decision making, especially judgments and decisions made by patients in consultation with healthcare providers, is ripe for instructive comparisons. Like the decisions of criminal defendants, medical decisions are among the most important ordinary people can make. Such decisions are often made between unfavorable options (e.g., chemotherapy or invasive surgery), in unfamiliar and complex knowledge domains, and with the assistance of professionals who may not be fully trusted. Yet, they are also distinct from the well-worn lessons of market economics.

Given these structural parallels, it is worth asking whether the way options and decision-relevant information are framed have any measurable influence on judgments and decisions in medicine. The answer is decisively yes. For example, Armstrong et al. (2002) presented participants with graphs of curves representing how long people live after being treated for a disease over several years. These representations are useful, for example, in comparing different treatment options. Two mathematically identical ways of presenting the same data are survival curves, showing how many people are still alive after different time intervals, and mortality curves, showing how many people have died after those same intervals. Despite their mathematical equivalence, Armstrong et al. (2002) found that participants who were given survival curves (or both survival and mortality curves together) preferred preventive surgery significantly more often, and were significantly more accurate in answering knowledge questions, than participants who only received mortality curves. Donovan and Jalleh (2000) told participants about a hypothetical immunization that protected infants against respiratory problems. Side effects were framed either negatively (10% chance of side effects) or positively (90% chance of no side effects). They found that positive framing yielded better performance for participants without a young infant at home. Farrell et al. (2001) presented information to participants about the safety of donated blood in either a gain frame (lives saved), a loss frame (lives lost), or a combined loss frame presented in a positive context. They found that participants in the gain-frame condition were significantly more confident of blood safety for transfusions than in the loss-frame or combined conditions. These findings are far from unique. The Making Numbers Meaningful systematic literature review covers dozens of published studies demonstrating framing effects in judgment and decision making in medical contexts (see Ancker et al. 2025; Zikmund-Fisher and Ancker 2025 and other related articles in the same issue of Medical Decision Making Policy & Practice for a detailed review of the literature). My own lived experience as a survivor of Stage III renal cell carcinoma is quite consistent with these findings. An 80% chance of survival feels different than a 20% chance of dying—even for a reasonably numerate JDM researcher.

There are several take-aways from the literature on framing effects in medical judgment and decision making relevant to plea bargaining. First, they are pervasive, encompassing a number of dimensions including probability, risk, and the quality of outcomes. Thus, with respect to decisions about plea bargaining, in addition to the effects Reyna et al. (2025) found for time served, it is reasonable to predict comparable framing effects for other decision-relevant judgments including the likelihood of conviction, time pressure before a plea deal “goes off the table,” and the harshness of detention facilities. Second, framing effects often interact with other factors including participant characteristics. Reyna et al. (2025) discovered that criminal history interacts with framing due to the presumed higher propensity for risk taking of those who acknowledge previous criminal behavior. It would not be surprising if other individual differences such as tolerance for ambiguity and need for cognition also mediated or moderated framing effects in plea bargaining. Third, simply presenting two opposite frames together (e.g. lives saved and lives lost) has been surprisingly ineffective in medical contexts. It is therefore unlikely that a simple fix such as this will prove sufficient in future research and practice related to plea bargain decisions. Finally, theoretically based cognitive interventions have been found to reduce framing effects in medical decision making. These interventions have theoretical as well as practical implications for plea bargaining.

Some of the demonstrably effective interventions in medical decision making are based on the fuzzy-trace theory notion that people make judgments and decisions based on a mental representation of the bottom-line meaning—or gist—of decision relevant information (Reyna 2008; Reyna et al. 2022). For example, people make decisions differently when medical risk information is presented as absolute values compared to relative risk, and the use of stacked bar graphs to convey risk information improves judgments of medical risk (Brust-Renck et al. 2013). In my own research on perhaps the first web-based Intelligent Tutoring Systems to help women understand and make decisions about genetic cancer risk, icon arrays (sometimes called bathroom figures) along with other gist-based visual representations, gist-based tutorial dialogues, and gist explanations significantly improved risk assessment, knowledge, and comprehension (Wolfe et al. 2015; Wolfe et al. 2016). Another risk-related judgment is when two options are roughly the same or approximately equal in riskiness. This is important, for example, when two medical treatments are approximately equal in efficacy, but one has more serious side effects than the other. Here too, the way risks are framed influences judgment. However, a simple verbal intervention, “Think about the bottom-line gist. Is this a meaningful difference …” (Wolfe et al. 2018, 158), significantly increased appropriate approximately equal judgments, as did semantic context and source attribution. Given the evidence from the realm of medical decision making, comparable techniques involving asking defendants for gist explanations of plea-related decisions, simple gist-invoking wording, and even images to counteract quantitative framing effects are all promising approaches to helping defendants understand the gist of information related to plea bargains.

The US public seems deeply divided over whether too many people are suffering imprisonment in an age of mass incarceration (see Fleetwood 2020 for a deeply humanistic treatment) or whether the justice systems coddles criminals (“hugs for thugs”) who should be kept off the streets for as long as possible. Yet surely we can agree that it would be incredibly expensive if every criminal case that was currently resolved via plea bargaining went to trial. The American tradition of judicial fairness demands that people understand what they are agreeing to before waiving their right to a trial by a jury of their peers. Adherents to a belief in the strong form of human rationality sometimes argue that without this assumption, the very basis for the right to autonomous legal decision making disappears. However, decades of research in cognitive psychology, including multidisciplinary research on medical decision making, suggest that people who understand the bottom-line meaning of their circumstances can, and regularly do, make good decisions aligned with their own goals and values, about important and complex issues. This is the case with complex decisions such as whether to undergo invasive surgery, and it should be the case with decisions about plea bargaining too. Although there are lively debates in the field of judgment and decision making, I am persuaded that people are most likely to make judgments and decisions in their own best interest when they understand the bottom-line meaning (i.e., have a sound gist mental representation) of the decision-relevant information at hand. If I had my way, officers of the court, including public defenders and prosecutors, would come to understand that they have an obligation to ensure that defendants actually understand the gist of plea agreements, however framed. These mental representations would include a gist understanding of the prevalence of plea bargaining in American jurisprudence and the likelihood and nature of positive and negative outcomes in both chosen and relinquished decision options. The more judges, attorneys, and prosecutors understand the way people actually make life-altering decisions, the better positioned they will be to ensure that every time a defendant pleads guilty as part of a plea bargain, justice is served.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
4.40
自引率
5.00%
发文量
40
期刊介绍: The Journal of Behavioral Decision Making is a multidisciplinary journal with a broad base of content and style. It publishes original empirical reports, critical review papers, theoretical analyses and methodological contributions. The Journal also features book, software and decision aiding technique reviews, abstracts of important articles published elsewhere and teaching suggestions. The objective of the Journal is to present and stimulate behavioral research on decision making and to provide a forum for the evaluation of complementary, contrasting and conflicting perspectives. These perspectives include psychology, management science, sociology, political science and economics. Studies of behavioral decision making in naturalistic and applied settings are encouraged.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信