Supreet Saluja , Talia Ciscato , Anjoli Mistry , Thomas Hummel , Charles Spence , Richard J. Stevenson
{"title":"涩味溶液和基本口味的跨模式关联的性质和起源","authors":"Supreet Saluja , Talia Ciscato , Anjoli Mistry , Thomas Hummel , Charles Spence , Richard J. Stevenson","doi":"10.1016/j.foodqual.2025.105518","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>People match gustatory (basic tastes) and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, and textures) to each other in a reliable manner. Yet, whether other attributes of the experience of flavor, such as, for example, astringency also evidence such reliable crossmodal mappings is currently unknown. Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess whether individuals make reliable crossmodal mappings between astringent solutions and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, hand-felt roughness/smoothness), and to examine the basis of any mappings observed. Participants sampled 6 solutions – astringent and 5 basic tastants – at 2 concentrations each (low, high), and for each selected their best matching color, texture, and shape (in three separate counterbalanced blocks). After making their selections, the participants evaluated the valence of the solutions, their colors, textures, and shapes, as well as the qualities/intensities of the solutions, and their confidence in the matches they made. Participants reported the rationale for their color-, texture- and shape matches at the end of the study. Color, texture, and shape selections evidenced consensual mappings, and participants were generally confident in all matches to a comparable level. Whilst people typically reported that their color matches were driven by real world associations (e.g., yellow chosen for sour, as lemons are yellow and sour), followed by valence (liking; e.g., pink for sweet, as both are liked), texture and shape matches to solutions were more attributable to intensity (e.g., rougher textures, selected for rougher feeling [e.g., astringent] solutions) as well as valence. Implications for flavor binding and marketing are discussed.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":322,"journal":{"name":"Food Quality and Preference","volume":"130 ","pages":"Article 105518"},"PeriodicalIF":4.9000,"publicationDate":"2025-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"The nature and origins of crossmodal associations to astringent solutions and basic tastes\",\"authors\":\"Supreet Saluja , Talia Ciscato , Anjoli Mistry , Thomas Hummel , Charles Spence , Richard J. Stevenson\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.foodqual.2025.105518\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>People match gustatory (basic tastes) and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, and textures) to each other in a reliable manner. Yet, whether other attributes of the experience of flavor, such as, for example, astringency also evidence such reliable crossmodal mappings is currently unknown. Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess whether individuals make reliable crossmodal mappings between astringent solutions and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, hand-felt roughness/smoothness), and to examine the basis of any mappings observed. Participants sampled 6 solutions – astringent and 5 basic tastants – at 2 concentrations each (low, high), and for each selected their best matching color, texture, and shape (in three separate counterbalanced blocks). After making their selections, the participants evaluated the valence of the solutions, their colors, textures, and shapes, as well as the qualities/intensities of the solutions, and their confidence in the matches they made. Participants reported the rationale for their color-, texture- and shape matches at the end of the study. Color, texture, and shape selections evidenced consensual mappings, and participants were generally confident in all matches to a comparable level. Whilst people typically reported that their color matches were driven by real world associations (e.g., yellow chosen for sour, as lemons are yellow and sour), followed by valence (liking; e.g., pink for sweet, as both are liked), texture and shape matches to solutions were more attributable to intensity (e.g., rougher textures, selected for rougher feeling [e.g., astringent] solutions) as well as valence. Implications for flavor binding and marketing are discussed.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":322,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Food Quality and Preference\",\"volume\":\"130 \",\"pages\":\"Article 105518\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":4.9000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-03-28\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Food Quality and Preference\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"97\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S095032932500093X\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"农林科学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"FOOD SCIENCE & TECHNOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Food Quality and Preference","FirstCategoryId":"97","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S095032932500093X","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"FOOD SCIENCE & TECHNOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
The nature and origins of crossmodal associations to astringent solutions and basic tastes
People match gustatory (basic tastes) and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, and textures) to each other in a reliable manner. Yet, whether other attributes of the experience of flavor, such as, for example, astringency also evidence such reliable crossmodal mappings is currently unknown. Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess whether individuals make reliable crossmodal mappings between astringent solutions and non-gustatory stimuli (e.g., colors, shapes, hand-felt roughness/smoothness), and to examine the basis of any mappings observed. Participants sampled 6 solutions – astringent and 5 basic tastants – at 2 concentrations each (low, high), and for each selected their best matching color, texture, and shape (in three separate counterbalanced blocks). After making their selections, the participants evaluated the valence of the solutions, their colors, textures, and shapes, as well as the qualities/intensities of the solutions, and their confidence in the matches they made. Participants reported the rationale for their color-, texture- and shape matches at the end of the study. Color, texture, and shape selections evidenced consensual mappings, and participants were generally confident in all matches to a comparable level. Whilst people typically reported that their color matches were driven by real world associations (e.g., yellow chosen for sour, as lemons are yellow and sour), followed by valence (liking; e.g., pink for sweet, as both are liked), texture and shape matches to solutions were more attributable to intensity (e.g., rougher textures, selected for rougher feeling [e.g., astringent] solutions) as well as valence. Implications for flavor binding and marketing are discussed.
期刊介绍:
Food Quality and Preference is a journal devoted to sensory, consumer and behavioural research in food and non-food products. It publishes original research, critical reviews, and short communications in sensory and consumer science, and sensometrics. In addition, the journal publishes special invited issues on important timely topics and from relevant conferences. These are aimed at bridging the gap between research and application, bringing together authors and readers in consumer and market research, sensory science, sensometrics and sensory evaluation, nutrition and food choice, as well as food research, product development and sensory quality assurance. Submissions to Food Quality and Preference are limited to papers that include some form of human measurement; papers that are limited to physical/chemical measures or the routine application of sensory, consumer or econometric analysis will not be considered unless they specifically make a novel scientific contribution in line with the journal''s coverage as outlined below.