{"title":"收缩皮肤:运动导致运动后10秒施加刺激的压缩定位错误","authors":"Tatjana Seizova-Cajic, Jack Brooks, Janet Taylor","doi":"10.1111/ejn.70076","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Localizing touch on the skin requires integration of multiple spatial signals, including reference landmarks and motion cues. It is well known that motion patterns can bias the perceived endpoint of motion. However, it is unknown whether <i>static</i> touch presented <i>post-motion</i> is also distorted. To investigate this, we presented space-changing motion patterns and tested position perception 1 s and 10 s post-motion. We used a brush moving along the forearm at 15 cm s<sup>−1</sup>, brushing 4.5 cm skin patches near the elbow and near the wrist, skipping a 10-cm long metal-shielded patch in the middle (‘numb spot’). It accelerated to 100 cm s<sup>−1</sup> across the shielded gap in an attempt to create an illusion of continuous motion between the separate brushed areas. After several such deceptive motions, 12 participants indicated the locations touched by a von Frey filament near the elbow and wrist, all within the previously brushed areas. Localization responses shifted 4–10 mm towards the numb spot in the skipped-patch condition compared with controls with either continuous brushing across the full forearm, or brushing the same patches without acceleration. This spatial distortion was equally strong 1 s and 10 s after motion offset with only isolated location-specific differences between delays. In addition, participants' sketches indicated a reduction in perceived gap size. We propose that participants used the brushed fields as reference frame for localisation, with the high-velocity motion compressing the perceived space between them. This means that motion-defined boundaries can serve as spatial landmarks for static touch.</p>","PeriodicalId":11993,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"61 7","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7000,"publicationDate":"2025-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ejn.70076","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Shrinking the Skin: Motion Results in Compressive Mislocalization of Stimuli Applied 10 s Post-Motion\",\"authors\":\"Tatjana Seizova-Cajic, Jack Brooks, Janet Taylor\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/ejn.70076\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Localizing touch on the skin requires integration of multiple spatial signals, including reference landmarks and motion cues. It is well known that motion patterns can bias the perceived endpoint of motion. However, it is unknown whether <i>static</i> touch presented <i>post-motion</i> is also distorted. To investigate this, we presented space-changing motion patterns and tested position perception 1 s and 10 s post-motion. We used a brush moving along the forearm at 15 cm s<sup>−1</sup>, brushing 4.5 cm skin patches near the elbow and near the wrist, skipping a 10-cm long metal-shielded patch in the middle (‘numb spot’). It accelerated to 100 cm s<sup>−1</sup> across the shielded gap in an attempt to create an illusion of continuous motion between the separate brushed areas. After several such deceptive motions, 12 participants indicated the locations touched by a von Frey filament near the elbow and wrist, all within the previously brushed areas. Localization responses shifted 4–10 mm towards the numb spot in the skipped-patch condition compared with controls with either continuous brushing across the full forearm, or brushing the same patches without acceleration. This spatial distortion was equally strong 1 s and 10 s after motion offset with only isolated location-specific differences between delays. In addition, participants' sketches indicated a reduction in perceived gap size. We propose that participants used the brushed fields as reference frame for localisation, with the high-velocity motion compressing the perceived space between them. This means that motion-defined boundaries can serve as spatial landmarks for static touch.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":11993,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"European Journal of Neuroscience\",\"volume\":\"61 7\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.7000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-04-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ejn.70076\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"European Journal of Neuroscience\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.70076\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"NEUROSCIENCES\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"European Journal of Neuroscience","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.70076","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"NEUROSCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
摘要
定位皮肤上的触摸需要整合多个空间信号,包括参考地标和运动线索。众所周知,运动模式会使感知到的运动终点产生偏差。然而,尚不清楚运动后呈现的静态触摸是否也会扭曲。为了研究这一点,我们提出了空间变化的运动模式,并测试了运动后1秒和10秒的位置感知。我们使用一个以15 cm s - 1的速度沿着前臂移动的刷,刷肘部附近和手腕附近4.5 cm的皮肤斑块,跳过中间10 cm长的金属屏蔽斑块(“麻木点”)。它加速到100 cm s - 1,穿过屏蔽的间隙,试图在不同的刷刷区域之间创造连续运动的错觉。经过几次这样的欺骗动作后,12名参与者指出了肘部和手腕附近冯·弗雷丝触摸过的位置,这些位置都在之前刷过的区域内。与连续刷遍整个前臂或在没有加速的情况下刷遍相同的贴片的对照组相比,在跳过贴片的情况下,定位反应向麻木点移动了4-10毫米。在运动偏移后1秒和10秒,这种空间畸变同样强烈,延迟之间只有孤立的位置特异性差异。此外,参与者的草图表明他们感知到的差距减小了。我们建议参与者使用刷场作为定位的参考框架,高速运动压缩它们之间的感知空间。这意味着运动定义的边界可以作为静态触摸的空间地标。
Shrinking the Skin: Motion Results in Compressive Mislocalization of Stimuli Applied 10 s Post-Motion
Localizing touch on the skin requires integration of multiple spatial signals, including reference landmarks and motion cues. It is well known that motion patterns can bias the perceived endpoint of motion. However, it is unknown whether static touch presented post-motion is also distorted. To investigate this, we presented space-changing motion patterns and tested position perception 1 s and 10 s post-motion. We used a brush moving along the forearm at 15 cm s−1, brushing 4.5 cm skin patches near the elbow and near the wrist, skipping a 10-cm long metal-shielded patch in the middle (‘numb spot’). It accelerated to 100 cm s−1 across the shielded gap in an attempt to create an illusion of continuous motion between the separate brushed areas. After several such deceptive motions, 12 participants indicated the locations touched by a von Frey filament near the elbow and wrist, all within the previously brushed areas. Localization responses shifted 4–10 mm towards the numb spot in the skipped-patch condition compared with controls with either continuous brushing across the full forearm, or brushing the same patches without acceleration. This spatial distortion was equally strong 1 s and 10 s after motion offset with only isolated location-specific differences between delays. In addition, participants' sketches indicated a reduction in perceived gap size. We propose that participants used the brushed fields as reference frame for localisation, with the high-velocity motion compressing the perceived space between them. This means that motion-defined boundaries can serve as spatial landmarks for static touch.
期刊介绍:
EJN is the journal of FENS and supports the international neuroscientific community by publishing original high quality research articles and reviews in all fields of neuroscience. In addition, to engage with issues that are of interest to the science community, we also publish Editorials, Meetings Reports and Neuro-Opinions on topics that are of current interest in the fields of neuroscience research and training in science. We have recently established a series of ‘Profiles of Women in Neuroscience’. Our goal is to provide a vehicle for publications that further the understanding of the structure and function of the nervous system in both health and disease and to provide a vehicle to engage the neuroscience community. As the official journal of FENS, profits from the journal are re-invested in the neuroscientific community through the activities of FENS.