{"title":"情商与阅读理解:研究单词阅读、词汇量和工作记忆的中介作用","authors":"Liyan Yu , Xiuhong Tong","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102574","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study examined the potential pathways connecting emotional intelligence to reading comprehension of narrative and non-narrative texts, and the influence of family socioeconomic status (SES) on these pathways. We tracked 689 Chinese third-grade students (341 female; Mage = 9.23 years, SD = 0.66) from wave 1 (grade three) to wave 3 (grade five). Results revealed that after controlling for gender, reading comprehension, and non-verbal intelligence at wave 1, word reading and vocabulary knowledge at wave 2 were significant mediators linking emotional intelligence at wave 1 to reading comprehension of both narrative and non-narrative texts at wave 3. In contrast, working memory at wave 2 served as a mediator only between emotional intelligence at wave 1 and reading comprehension of non-narrative texts at wave 3. Additionally, family SES moderated the pathways linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension. These findings provide a comprehensive understanding of mechanisms linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications statement</h3><div>Using both narrative and non-narrative texts, our study found that for Chinese primary school students, word reading (<em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.07] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts) and vocabulary knowledge at age 10 (<em>β</em> = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts) significantly mediate the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11. These results highlight the importance of combining word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and emotional intelligence during intervention to improve children's reading comprehension. We also found that working memory at age 10 significantly mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11 for non-narrative (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.03]), but not narrative, texts (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [−0.004, 0.02]). These findings underscore the importance of considering text genre when examining the development of reading comprehension, designing reading instruction, and establishing reading interventions. Additionally, emotional intelligence at age 9 is more strongly associated with word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and working memory at age 10 for children from low socioeconomic status (SES) families compared to those from high-SES families, highlighting the need for more attention to the emotional intelligence development in children from low-SES backgrounds.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.8000,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Emotional intelligence and reading comprehension: Examining mediating roles of word reading, vocabulary, and working memory\",\"authors\":\"Liyan Yu , Xiuhong Tong\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102574\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>This study examined the potential pathways connecting emotional intelligence to reading comprehension of narrative and non-narrative texts, and the influence of family socioeconomic status (SES) on these pathways. We tracked 689 Chinese third-grade students (341 female; Mage = 9.23 years, SD = 0.66) from wave 1 (grade three) to wave 3 (grade five). Results revealed that after controlling for gender, reading comprehension, and non-verbal intelligence at wave 1, word reading and vocabulary knowledge at wave 2 were significant mediators linking emotional intelligence at wave 1 to reading comprehension of both narrative and non-narrative texts at wave 3. In contrast, working memory at wave 2 served as a mediator only between emotional intelligence at wave 1 and reading comprehension of non-narrative texts at wave 3. Additionally, family SES moderated the pathways linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension. These findings provide a comprehensive understanding of mechanisms linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications statement</h3><div>Using both narrative and non-narrative texts, our study found that for Chinese primary school students, word reading (<em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.07] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts) and vocabulary knowledge at age 10 (<em>β</em> = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts) significantly mediate the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11. These results highlight the importance of combining word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and emotional intelligence during intervention to improve children's reading comprehension. We also found that working memory at age 10 significantly mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11 for non-narrative (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.03]), but not narrative, texts (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [−0.004, 0.02]). These findings underscore the importance of considering text genre when examining the development of reading comprehension, designing reading instruction, and establishing reading interventions. Additionally, emotional intelligence at age 9 is more strongly associated with word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and working memory at age 10 for children from low socioeconomic status (SES) families compared to those from high-SES families, highlighting the need for more attention to the emotional intelligence development in children from low-SES backgrounds.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":48336,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Learning and Individual Differences\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-10-17\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Learning and Individual Differences\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1041608024001675\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Learning and Individual Differences","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1041608024001675","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
摘要
本研究探讨了连接情商与叙事和非叙事文本阅读理解的潜在途径,以及家庭社会经济地位(SES)对这些途径的影响。我们对689名中国三年级学生(341名女生;年龄=9.23岁,SD=0.66)进行了从第一波(三年级)到第三波(五年级)的跟踪调查。结果显示,在控制了第一阶段的性别、阅读理解能力和非语言智能后,第二阶段的单词阅读和词汇知识是第一阶段情商与第三阶段叙事和非叙事文本阅读理解能力之间的重要中介。相比之下,第二阶段的工作记忆仅是第一阶段情商与第三阶段非叙事文本阅读理解之间的中介。此外,家庭经济状况也调节了情商与阅读理解之间的联系。这些发现为我们全面了解情商与阅读理解之间的关联机制提供了依据。教育相关性及启示声明通过对叙事文本和非叙事文本的研究,我们发现对于中国小学生而言,单词阅读(β = 0.04,95 % CI [0.03, 0.07]; β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts)和10岁时的词汇知识(β = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; β = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts)显著地调节了9岁时的情绪智力和11岁时的阅读理解能力之间的关系。这些结果凸显了在干预过程中将单词阅读、词汇知识和情商结合起来以提高儿童阅读理解能力的重要性。我们还发现,对于非叙事文本(β = 0.01,95 % CI [0.01,0.03])而非叙事文本(β = 0.01,95 % CI [-0.004,0.02]),10 岁时的工作记忆对 9 岁时的情商和 11 岁时的阅读理解能力之间的关系有显著的中介作用。这些发现强调了在研究阅读理解能力的发展、设计阅读教学和制定阅读干预措施时考虑文本体裁的重要性。此外,与来自高社会经济地位家庭的儿童相比,来自低社会经济地位家庭的儿童在9岁时的情商与10岁时的单词阅读、词汇知识和工作记忆有更密切的关系,这突出表明需要更多地关注来自低社会经济地位家庭的儿童的情商发展。
Emotional intelligence and reading comprehension: Examining mediating roles of word reading, vocabulary, and working memory
This study examined the potential pathways connecting emotional intelligence to reading comprehension of narrative and non-narrative texts, and the influence of family socioeconomic status (SES) on these pathways. We tracked 689 Chinese third-grade students (341 female; Mage = 9.23 years, SD = 0.66) from wave 1 (grade three) to wave 3 (grade five). Results revealed that after controlling for gender, reading comprehension, and non-verbal intelligence at wave 1, word reading and vocabulary knowledge at wave 2 were significant mediators linking emotional intelligence at wave 1 to reading comprehension of both narrative and non-narrative texts at wave 3. In contrast, working memory at wave 2 served as a mediator only between emotional intelligence at wave 1 and reading comprehension of non-narrative texts at wave 3. Additionally, family SES moderated the pathways linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension. These findings provide a comprehensive understanding of mechanisms linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension.
Educational relevance and implications statement
Using both narrative and non-narrative texts, our study found that for Chinese primary school students, word reading (β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.07] for narrative texts; β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts) and vocabulary knowledge at age 10 (β = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; β = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts) significantly mediate the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11. These results highlight the importance of combining word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and emotional intelligence during intervention to improve children's reading comprehension. We also found that working memory at age 10 significantly mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11 for non-narrative (β = 0.01, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.03]), but not narrative, texts (β = 0.01, 95 % CI [−0.004, 0.02]). These findings underscore the importance of considering text genre when examining the development of reading comprehension, designing reading instruction, and establishing reading interventions. Additionally, emotional intelligence at age 9 is more strongly associated with word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and working memory at age 10 for children from low socioeconomic status (SES) families compared to those from high-SES families, highlighting the need for more attention to the emotional intelligence development in children from low-SES backgrounds.
期刊介绍:
Learning and Individual Differences is a research journal devoted to publishing articles of individual differences as they relate to learning within an educational context. The Journal focuses on original empirical studies of high theoretical and methodological rigor that that make a substantial scientific contribution. Learning and Individual Differences publishes original research. Manuscripts should be no longer than 7500 words of primary text (not including tables, figures, references).