{"title":"明确的警告能消除诱导过程中的假设偏差吗?来自体育卡片现场拍卖的证据","authors":"J. List","doi":"10.1257/AER.91.5.1498","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The goal of environmental policy is to protect the well-being of humans and the ecosystems vital to human existence. Because benefit-cost analyses are now required at the federal level, and increasingly at the state level, much attention has been paid to the development of practical, credible approaches for estimating the benefits and costs of environmental programs. Although policy makers have a good handle on measuring the explicit costs associated with increased environmental protection, at present several disparate approaches are utilized to measure economic values of environmental goods and services. Arguably the most contentious of these approaches is the Contingent Valuation Method (CVM), which allows the researcher to measure the total value of the commodity in question (see Peter A. Diamond and Jerry A. Hausman's [1994] critical review). Chief amongst these concems is whether hypothetical bias is inherent in CVM responses. Some recently published studies provide evidence that suggests important differences exist between responses from real and hypothetical valuation questions.1 Recognizing this shortcoming, researchers have adopted both ex ante and ex post techniques to overcome hypothetical bias. Although these attempts have not been completely successful in dealing with hypothetical bias, Ronald G. Cummings and Laura 0. Taylor (1999) present evidence from laboratory experiments that indicates hypothetical bias can be overcome by an appropriate ex ante design they refer to as a \"cheap talk\" scheme.2 The underlying premise behind Cummings and Taylor's design is that by making hypothetical bias an integral part of the contingent value questionnaire, the researcher can induce truthtelling. Their cheap talk experiments validated this conjecture, as hypothetical bias was effectively eliminated in open referenda for three public goods. The goal of this study is to take the cheap talk design to a well-functioning marketplace and auction off sportscards. Besides providing a field validity test, I also explore the generality of Cummings and Taylor's findings by examining whether experience with the good in question affects hypothetical bias through inclusion of both card dealers and nondealers in the field auctions. The theory of value formation suggests that experienced bidders may not be easily swayed by the cheap talk design as they have a well-structured preference ordering for the good in question and should therefore rely on few, if any, external signals when formulating their value.","PeriodicalId":134860,"journal":{"name":"Framed Field Experiments","volume":"5 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2001-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"498","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Do explicit warnings eliminate the hypothetical bias in elicitation procedures? Evidence from field auctions for sportscards\",\"authors\":\"J. List\",\"doi\":\"10.1257/AER.91.5.1498\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"The goal of environmental policy is to protect the well-being of humans and the ecosystems vital to human existence. Because benefit-cost analyses are now required at the federal level, and increasingly at the state level, much attention has been paid to the development of practical, credible approaches for estimating the benefits and costs of environmental programs. Although policy makers have a good handle on measuring the explicit costs associated with increased environmental protection, at present several disparate approaches are utilized to measure economic values of environmental goods and services. Arguably the most contentious of these approaches is the Contingent Valuation Method (CVM), which allows the researcher to measure the total value of the commodity in question (see Peter A. Diamond and Jerry A. Hausman's [1994] critical review). Chief amongst these concems is whether hypothetical bias is inherent in CVM responses. Some recently published studies provide evidence that suggests important differences exist between responses from real and hypothetical valuation questions.1 Recognizing this shortcoming, researchers have adopted both ex ante and ex post techniques to overcome hypothetical bias. Although these attempts have not been completely successful in dealing with hypothetical bias, Ronald G. Cummings and Laura 0. Taylor (1999) present evidence from laboratory experiments that indicates hypothetical bias can be overcome by an appropriate ex ante design they refer to as a \\\"cheap talk\\\" scheme.2 The underlying premise behind Cummings and Taylor's design is that by making hypothetical bias an integral part of the contingent value questionnaire, the researcher can induce truthtelling. Their cheap talk experiments validated this conjecture, as hypothetical bias was effectively eliminated in open referenda for three public goods. The goal of this study is to take the cheap talk design to a well-functioning marketplace and auction off sportscards. Besides providing a field validity test, I also explore the generality of Cummings and Taylor's findings by examining whether experience with the good in question affects hypothetical bias through inclusion of both card dealers and nondealers in the field auctions. The theory of value formation suggests that experienced bidders may not be easily swayed by the cheap talk design as they have a well-structured preference ordering for the good in question and should therefore rely on few, if any, external signals when formulating their value.\",\"PeriodicalId\":134860,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Framed Field Experiments\",\"volume\":\"5 1\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2001-12-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"498\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Framed Field Experiments\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1257/AER.91.5.1498\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Framed Field Experiments","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1257/AER.91.5.1498","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 498
摘要
环境政策的目标是保护人类的福祉和对人类生存至关重要的生态系统。由于效益-成本分析现在在联邦一级被要求,而且越来越多地在州一级被要求,因此人们对开发实用的、可靠的方法来估计环境项目的效益和成本给予了很大的关注。虽然决策者很好地掌握了衡量与加强环境保护有关的显性成本的方法,但目前使用了几种不同的方法来衡量环境货物和服务的经济价值。可以说,这些方法中最具争议的是条件估值法(CVM),它允许研究人员测量所讨论商品的总价值(参见Peter A. Diamond和Jerry A. Hausman[1994]的批判性评论)。这些问题中最主要的是假设偏差是否在CVM反应中固有。最近发表的一些研究提供了证据,表明真实估值问题和假设估值问题的回答之间存在重要差异认识到这一缺点,研究人员采用了事前和事后技术来克服假设的偏见。尽管这些尝试在处理假设性偏见方面并没有完全成功,罗纳德·g·卡明斯和劳拉。Taylor(1999)提出了来自实验室实验的证据,表明假设的偏见可以通过适当的事前设计来克服,他们称之为“廉价谈话”计划卡明斯和泰勒的设计背后的基本前提是,通过将假设偏见作为条件价值问卷的一个组成部分,研究人员可以诱导说实话。他们的廉价谈话实验证实了这一猜想,因为假设性偏见在三种公共产品的公开公投中被有效消除了。这项研究的目的是将这种廉价的设计带到一个运作良好的市场,并拍卖体育卡片。除了提供现场有效性测试之外,我还通过在现场拍卖中包括发牌人和非发牌人,来检验有关商品的经验是否会影响假设的偏见,从而探索卡明斯和泰勒发现的普遍性。价值形成理论表明,经验丰富的竞标者可能不会轻易被廉价言论的设计所左右,因为他们对所讨论的商品有一个结构良好的偏好顺序,因此在形成自己的价值时,应该很少依赖(如果有的话)外部信号。
Do explicit warnings eliminate the hypothetical bias in elicitation procedures? Evidence from field auctions for sportscards
The goal of environmental policy is to protect the well-being of humans and the ecosystems vital to human existence. Because benefit-cost analyses are now required at the federal level, and increasingly at the state level, much attention has been paid to the development of practical, credible approaches for estimating the benefits and costs of environmental programs. Although policy makers have a good handle on measuring the explicit costs associated with increased environmental protection, at present several disparate approaches are utilized to measure economic values of environmental goods and services. Arguably the most contentious of these approaches is the Contingent Valuation Method (CVM), which allows the researcher to measure the total value of the commodity in question (see Peter A. Diamond and Jerry A. Hausman's [1994] critical review). Chief amongst these concems is whether hypothetical bias is inherent in CVM responses. Some recently published studies provide evidence that suggests important differences exist between responses from real and hypothetical valuation questions.1 Recognizing this shortcoming, researchers have adopted both ex ante and ex post techniques to overcome hypothetical bias. Although these attempts have not been completely successful in dealing with hypothetical bias, Ronald G. Cummings and Laura 0. Taylor (1999) present evidence from laboratory experiments that indicates hypothetical bias can be overcome by an appropriate ex ante design they refer to as a "cheap talk" scheme.2 The underlying premise behind Cummings and Taylor's design is that by making hypothetical bias an integral part of the contingent value questionnaire, the researcher can induce truthtelling. Their cheap talk experiments validated this conjecture, as hypothetical bias was effectively eliminated in open referenda for three public goods. The goal of this study is to take the cheap talk design to a well-functioning marketplace and auction off sportscards. Besides providing a field validity test, I also explore the generality of Cummings and Taylor's findings by examining whether experience with the good in question affects hypothetical bias through inclusion of both card dealers and nondealers in the field auctions. The theory of value formation suggests that experienced bidders may not be easily swayed by the cheap talk design as they have a well-structured preference ordering for the good in question and should therefore rely on few, if any, external signals when formulating their value.