Antécédents cognitifs de l’activisme professionnel : schémas précoces inadaptés et profils d’activisme professionnel

Élisabeth Grebot , Marie Olivier
{"title":"Antécédents cognitifs de l’activisme professionnel : schémas précoces inadaptés et profils d’activisme professionnel","authors":"Élisabeth Grebot ,&nbsp;Marie Olivier","doi":"10.1016/j.jtcc.2017.09.001","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>L’objectif de l’étude était d’identifier les schémas précoces inadaptés suractivés dans l’activisme professionnel (<em>workaholism</em>) en distinguant l’activisme enthousiaste (<em>enthusiastic workaholism</em>) de l’activisme non enthousiaste (<em>no enthusiastic workaholism</em>), selon la catégorisation de Spence et Robbins. Cent trente-cinq participants ont complété trois questionnaires évaluant : (1) les quinze schémas précoces inadaptés de Young (assujettissement, exigences élevées, manque d’autocontrôle, sacrifice de soi, etc.) ; (2) la triade workaholique de Spence et Robbins composée de l’implication au travail, la tendance compulsive à travailler et la satisfaction au travail et (3) les deux composantes de l’échelle Dutch Work Addiction Scale (travailler excessivement, travailler compulsivement). Nous avons comparé l’activation des schémas précoces inadaptés dans trois groupes (non-activisme, activisme enthousiaste et activisme non enthousiaste). Deux schémas précoces inadaptés (abus–méfiance, exigences élevées) sont significativement plus activés dans l’activisme que dans le non-activisme et le schéma manque d’autocontrôle est significativement plus activé dans l’activisme non enthousiaste que dans l’activisme enthousiaste. L’activation différentielle de schémas précoces inadaptés dans les deux formes d’activisme professionnel a des implications thérapeutiques cognitives qui sont discutées.</p></div><div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>This study explores the relationship between early maladaptive schemas identified by Young (2005) and workaholism according to Spence and Robbins (1992). These authors differentiate four non-workaholic employee profiles (satisfied, unengaged, disenchanted and relaxed professionals) and two workaholic profiles (enthusiasts and non enthusiasts). According to Spence and Robbins, the only true workaholics are non-enthusiastic workaholics whose satisfaction scores are low, contrary to enthusiastic workaholics who obtain high work enjoyment scores. However, these two workaholic profiles share high work involvement and a high drive to work. This research aims to compare the activation of schemas in different professional profiles (non-workaholics, enthusiastic workaholics, non enthusiastic workaholics) for the purpose of completing current data, which agrees on the implication of excellence and performance beliefs in workaholics without distinguishing the enthusiast profile from the non-enthusiast. This study postulates an overactivation of unrelenting standards in non-enthusiastic workaholics.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Participants (<em>n</em> <!-->=<!--> <!-->135) completed three questionnaires: (1) the French version of Young's early maladaptive schemas questionnaire containing 75 items evaluating fifteen schemas (emotional deprivation, abandonment, mistrust/abuse, social isolation, defectiveness, failure, dependency and incompetence, vulnerability to harm, enmeshment, subjugation, self-sacrifice, emotional inhibition, unrelenting standards, entitlement, insufficient self-control); (2) the French version of Spence and Robbins questionnaire (Workaholism Battery – WorkBAT) composed of 25 items evaluating compulsive drive to work (8 items) and job satisfaction (7 items) and (3) the French version of the Dutch Work Addiction Scale (DUWAS) composed of 10 items of which 5 items evaluate the excessive work dimension and 5 items focus on the compulsive work dimension. Only the scores on the excessive work sub-scale were used as the compulsive tendency to work was evaluated using items from WorkBat, five of which are contained in the compulsive work scale of the DUWAS, which has lower homogeneity than the WorkBat compulsive tendency to work sub-scale. In accordance with Spence and Robbins, the subjects were divided into three groups based on scores above or below the averages obtained in workaholism questionnaires during their French validation. Participants were separated into a group of 77 non-workaholics, a group of 31 enthusiastic workaholics and a group of non-enthusiast workaholics.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The results show a differential activation: (1) of the unrelenting standards schema between the three groups (non-workaholics, enthusiastic workaholics, non-enthusiastic workaholics); (2) unrelenting standards and mistrust/abuse schemas between non-workaholics and workaholics; and (3) of the lack of self-control schema between enthusiastic workaholics and nonenthusiastic workaholics.</p></div><div><h3>Discussion</h3><p>The results confirm the implication of the mistrust/abuse and unrelenting standards schemas mentioned in the literature in workaholism. However, in the current study the hypothesis of an activation of higher EMS unrelenting standards in non-enthusiastic workaholism compared to enthusiastic workaholism is not validated. This result indicates that high performance demands do not generate a feeling of ill ease in professionals with a workaholic profile conforming to the observations of Taris et al., where excessive work is not predicative of workaholism although compulsive work is. In contrast, the results show that the lack of self-control schema is more active in non-enthusiast workaholics than in enthusiastic workaholics. The activation of beliefs linked to lack of self-control questions the implication of an overcompensation adaptation style to the schema which, according to Young consists of fighting apprehension by adopting behavior contrary to that of the schema or of seeking to control it.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>The lack of self-control schema differentiates the two workaholic profiles, whilst the mistrust/abuse and unrelenting standards schemas differentiate workaholics from non-workaholics. Specific therapeutic responses must be offered to these different types of professional behavior. Cognitive-behavioral therapy offers a sufficiently diverse theoretical, methodological and therapeutic range to allow therapists to help patients to identify and modify dysfunctional professional behavior such as non enthusiastic workaholism.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100746,"journal":{"name":"Journal de Thérapie Comportementale et Cognitive","volume":"28 1","pages":"Pages 23-32"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2018-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.jtcc.2017.09.001","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal de Thérapie Comportementale et Cognitive","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1155170417300861","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

L’objectif de l’étude était d’identifier les schémas précoces inadaptés suractivés dans l’activisme professionnel (workaholism) en distinguant l’activisme enthousiaste (enthusiastic workaholism) de l’activisme non enthousiaste (no enthusiastic workaholism), selon la catégorisation de Spence et Robbins. Cent trente-cinq participants ont complété trois questionnaires évaluant : (1) les quinze schémas précoces inadaptés de Young (assujettissement, exigences élevées, manque d’autocontrôle, sacrifice de soi, etc.) ; (2) la triade workaholique de Spence et Robbins composée de l’implication au travail, la tendance compulsive à travailler et la satisfaction au travail et (3) les deux composantes de l’échelle Dutch Work Addiction Scale (travailler excessivement, travailler compulsivement). Nous avons comparé l’activation des schémas précoces inadaptés dans trois groupes (non-activisme, activisme enthousiaste et activisme non enthousiaste). Deux schémas précoces inadaptés (abus–méfiance, exigences élevées) sont significativement plus activés dans l’activisme que dans le non-activisme et le schéma manque d’autocontrôle est significativement plus activé dans l’activisme non enthousiaste que dans l’activisme enthousiaste. L’activation différentielle de schémas précoces inadaptés dans les deux formes d’activisme professionnel a des implications thérapeutiques cognitives qui sont discutées.

Introduction

This study explores the relationship between early maladaptive schemas identified by Young (2005) and workaholism according to Spence and Robbins (1992). These authors differentiate four non-workaholic employee profiles (satisfied, unengaged, disenchanted and relaxed professionals) and two workaholic profiles (enthusiasts and non enthusiasts). According to Spence and Robbins, the only true workaholics are non-enthusiastic workaholics whose satisfaction scores are low, contrary to enthusiastic workaholics who obtain high work enjoyment scores. However, these two workaholic profiles share high work involvement and a high drive to work. This research aims to compare the activation of schemas in different professional profiles (non-workaholics, enthusiastic workaholics, non enthusiastic workaholics) for the purpose of completing current data, which agrees on the implication of excellence and performance beliefs in workaholics without distinguishing the enthusiast profile from the non-enthusiast. This study postulates an overactivation of unrelenting standards in non-enthusiastic workaholics.

Method

Participants (n = 135) completed three questionnaires: (1) the French version of Young's early maladaptive schemas questionnaire containing 75 items evaluating fifteen schemas (emotional deprivation, abandonment, mistrust/abuse, social isolation, defectiveness, failure, dependency and incompetence, vulnerability to harm, enmeshment, subjugation, self-sacrifice, emotional inhibition, unrelenting standards, entitlement, insufficient self-control); (2) the French version of Spence and Robbins questionnaire (Workaholism Battery – WorkBAT) composed of 25 items evaluating compulsive drive to work (8 items) and job satisfaction (7 items) and (3) the French version of the Dutch Work Addiction Scale (DUWAS) composed of 10 items of which 5 items evaluate the excessive work dimension and 5 items focus on the compulsive work dimension. Only the scores on the excessive work sub-scale were used as the compulsive tendency to work was evaluated using items from WorkBat, five of which are contained in the compulsive work scale of the DUWAS, which has lower homogeneity than the WorkBat compulsive tendency to work sub-scale. In accordance with Spence and Robbins, the subjects were divided into three groups based on scores above or below the averages obtained in workaholism questionnaires during their French validation. Participants were separated into a group of 77 non-workaholics, a group of 31 enthusiastic workaholics and a group of non-enthusiast workaholics.

Results

The results show a differential activation: (1) of the unrelenting standards schema between the three groups (non-workaholics, enthusiastic workaholics, non-enthusiastic workaholics); (2) unrelenting standards and mistrust/abuse schemas between non-workaholics and workaholics; and (3) of the lack of self-control schema between enthusiastic workaholics and nonenthusiastic workaholics.

Discussion

The results confirm the implication of the mistrust/abuse and unrelenting standards schemas mentioned in the literature in workaholism. However, in the current study the hypothesis of an activation of higher EMS unrelenting standards in non-enthusiastic workaholism compared to enthusiastic workaholism is not validated. This result indicates that high performance demands do not generate a feeling of ill ease in professionals with a workaholic profile conforming to the observations of Taris et al., where excessive work is not predicative of workaholism although compulsive work is. In contrast, the results show that the lack of self-control schema is more active in non-enthusiast workaholics than in enthusiastic workaholics. The activation of beliefs linked to lack of self-control questions the implication of an overcompensation adaptation style to the schema which, according to Young consists of fighting apprehension by adopting behavior contrary to that of the schema or of seeking to control it.

Conclusion

The lack of self-control schema differentiates the two workaholic profiles, whilst the mistrust/abuse and unrelenting standards schemas differentiate workaholics from non-workaholics. Specific therapeutic responses must be offered to these different types of professional behavior. Cognitive-behavioral therapy offers a sufficiently diverse theoretical, methodological and therapeutic range to allow therapists to help patients to identify and modify dysfunctional professional behavior such as non enthusiastic workaholism.

职业活动的认知史:不适当的早期模式和职业活动概况
在Spence和Robbins的分类中,职业活动(工作狂)与积极活动(热情的工作狂)和非积极活动(没有热情的工作主义)的区别是不合适的。五位参与者完成了三份有价值的问卷调查:(1)年轻人不适合的五种情况(假设、紧急情况、自动控制、社会牺牲等);(2) Spence和Robbins的工作狂试验包括对工作的影响、对工作的强迫性和对工作的满意度,以及(3)荷兰工作成瘾量表的两个组成部分(工作过度、工作强迫)。Nous avons比较了三个群体(非活动性、积极性和非积极性)不适合的学校活动。两个不合适的方案(abus–mé未婚夫,紧急情况)是有意义的,加上活动和非活动的活动,以及自动控制的方案是有意义和非活动和活动的活动。根据Spence和Robbins(1992)的研究,本研究探讨了Young(2005)发现的早期适应不良图式与工作狂之间的关系。这些作者区分了四种非工作狂员工档案(满意、不投入、失望和放松的专业人士)和两种工作狂档案(爱好者和非爱好者)。根据Spence和Robbins的研究,唯一真正的工作狂是满意度得分较低的非热情工作狂,而热情工作狂的工作享受得分较高。然而,这两种工作狂的性格都有很高的工作参与度和工作动力。本研究旨在比较不同职业档案(非工作狂、热情工作狂、非热情工作狂)中图式的激活情况,以完成当前数据,这在没有区分爱好者档案和非爱好者档案的情况下,同意了工作狂卓越和绩效信念的含义。这项研究假设,在不热心的工作狂中,无情的标准过于活跃。方法参与者(n=135)完成了三份问卷:(1)法国版Young早期适应不良图式问卷,包含75个项目,评估15个图式(情感剥夺、遗弃、不信任/虐待、社会孤立、缺陷、失败、依赖和无能、易受伤害、陷入困境、征服、自我牺牲、情感抑制、无情的标准、权利、自制力不足);(2) Spence和Robbins的法语版问卷(Workaholism Battery–WorkBAT)由25个项目组成,评估强迫性工作动力(8个项目)和工作满意度(7个项目);(3)荷兰工作成瘾量表(DUWAS)的法语版由10个项目组成。其中5个项目评估过度工作维度,5个项目关注强迫性工作维度。只有过度工作子量表上的分数被用作强迫性工作倾向的评估,使用WorkBat的项目,其中五个项目包含在DUWAS的强迫性工作量表中,其同质性低于WorkBat强迫性工作子量。根据Spence和Robbins的研究,受试者根据在法语验证期间工作狂问卷中获得的平均值以上或以下的分数被分为三组。参与者被分为77名非工作狂、31名热心工作狂和一组非热心工作狂。结果三组(非工作狂、热情工作狂、非热情工作狂)之间的持续标准图式激活存在差异;(2) 非工作狂和工作狂之间的无情标准和不信任/滥用模式;(3)热情工作狂与非热情工作狂之间缺乏自我控制图式。讨论结果证实了文献中提到的工作狂中的不信任/滥用和无情的标准图式的含义。然而,在目前的研究中,与热情工作狂相比,非热情工作狂激活更高EMS无情标准的假设没有得到验证。这一结果表明,高绩效要求不会让工作狂型的专业人士感到不自在,这符合Taris等人的观察结果,尽管强迫性工作是工作狂,但过度工作并不能预示工作狂。 相反,研究结果表明,非狂热工作狂的缺乏自我控制模式比热情工作狂更为活跃。与缺乏自制力有关的信念的激活质疑了对图式的过度补偿适应风格的含义,根据杨的说法,这种风格包括通过采取与图式相反的行为来对抗恐惧或寻求控制它,而不信任/滥用和无情的标准模式将工作狂与非工作狂区分开来。必须为这些不同类型的职业行为提供特定的治疗反应。认知行为疗法提供了足够多样化的理论、方法和治疗范围,使治疗师能够帮助患者识别和改变功能失调的职业行为,如非热情的工作狂。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信