Claustral Delusions

Claustrum Pub Date : 2016-01-01 DOI:10.3402/cla.v1.31426
A. Citri, S. Berretta
{"title":"Claustral Delusions","authors":"A. Citri, S. Berretta","doi":"10.3402/cla.v1.31426","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"T he claustrum, a thin strip of neurons located medially to the insula, is as intriguing as it is understudied. With a particularly dense reciprocal connectivity with large swaths of cortex (1), and a plethora of neuromodulatory inputs, the claustrum has been the object of a number of hypotheses regarding its functions. These hypotheses include a role for the claustrum in integration of sensory information to create a unified conscious experience (2), cortical synchronization (3), modulation of cortical functional networks (4), saliency detection (5, 6), active sensing (7), and segregation of attention (8, 9). Interestingly, all these hypotheses revolve around a common theme, that of enabling the formation of an accurate and cohesive representation of the world around us. In their article ‘A new perspective on delusional states evidence for claustrum involvement’, Patru and Reser (10) hypothesize that disruption of claustral function may contribute to the pathogenesis of psychotic symptoms, delusions in particular. The authors’ suggestion is intriguing and should spark interest in an understudied and potentially relevant brain region. Importantly, direct evidence supporting the hypothesis is lacking, and a more cautious approach would be to broaden the discussion to psychoses rather than only delusions. The authors review the anatomy and functional connectivity of the claustrum, as well as hypotheses regarding its function, and place it in the context of the broad, yet sometimes confusing, clinical literature regarding claustral lesions in patients with delusions or psychotic behavior. A major confound of these studies is the extent of the lesions, which in most cases extended well beyond the claustrum. They then provide a number of intriguing hypotheses for how the claustrum may be responsible for the formation of delusions. Interestingly, it has recently been proposed that salvinorin A, a hallucinogenic kappa opioid agonist, may be acting on the claustrum, causing a disruption of the conscious binding of information to form an accurate representation of the surrounding world (11). This hypothesis is compatible with the proposal of Patru and Reser, and both are cohesive with the evolving notion of a potential role for the claustrum in selective allocation of attention, potentially through prioritizing salience to selected objects, reducing the salience of non-prioritized objects. Delusions are profoundly debilitating psychotic symptoms. Although conceptual models for these symptoms are still highly speculative, it is plausible, as suggested by Patru and Reser, that a common feature is impairment of the ability to accurately assess the ‘validity’ of the interpretation of reality. Breakdown of the assessment process may lead patients to adopt constructs of reality that contain implausible characterizations. Altered sensory gating and perception, reported in patients with major psychoses, may represent components of such breakdown and resonate with growing evidence on claustral functions. Several authors have suggested that aberrant salience, involving complex neural circuits including the striatum, amygdala, and several cortical regions, may represent a key element underlying psychoses (12 16). In a classic treatise, Kapur (17) proposed that psychosis is a state of aberrant salience, causing delusions to evolve gradually in schizophrenic patients. Initially, patients develop heightened sensory receptiveness and increased awareness of the environment, in which many individual elements, which were previously ignored, now gain significant salience. This subtly altered perception of the world leaves the patients overwhelmed and confused, and therefore delusions are suggested to be a ‘top down’ cognitive explanation that the individual imposes on experiences of aberrant salience, in an effort to make sense of them. This explanation ties together the proposed role of the claustrum in mediating selective attention (8) with the notion proposed by Patru and Reser (and previously by Cascella (18, 19)) that disruption of the claustrum could lead to the development of delusions in schizophrenia. With this perspective, disturbance of claustral function, either through direct disruption of the claustrum, or by a circuit-level effect on inputs to the claustrum, would disrupt the appropriate segregation of salience to distinct objects in the world, overwhelming the individual with inputs. Interestingly, the most significant inputs to the claustrum arise from the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate (1, 4, 20). Potentially, in the context of the hypothesis raised by Patru and Reser, these prefrontal inputs could be defining the ‘attentional strategy’ implemented by the claustrum. Thus, disruption of the prefrontal inputs to the claustrum may lead to acceptance of non-conventional and implausible constructs emerging from the integration of sensory inputs. With this perspective in mind, similar arguments could be made regarding expected disruptions of the claustrum in other situations of attention deficits, such as attention deficit disorder (21 23). Kapur suggests that dopamine is a major component defining salience (16). As expression of Drd1dopamine receptors appears to extend within the mouse claustrum (www.mouse.brain-map.org), it will be interesting to test","PeriodicalId":92763,"journal":{"name":"Claustrum","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2016-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3402/cla.v1.31426","citationCount":"5","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Claustrum","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.3402/cla.v1.31426","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5

Abstract

T he claustrum, a thin strip of neurons located medially to the insula, is as intriguing as it is understudied. With a particularly dense reciprocal connectivity with large swaths of cortex (1), and a plethora of neuromodulatory inputs, the claustrum has been the object of a number of hypotheses regarding its functions. These hypotheses include a role for the claustrum in integration of sensory information to create a unified conscious experience (2), cortical synchronization (3), modulation of cortical functional networks (4), saliency detection (5, 6), active sensing (7), and segregation of attention (8, 9). Interestingly, all these hypotheses revolve around a common theme, that of enabling the formation of an accurate and cohesive representation of the world around us. In their article ‘A new perspective on delusional states evidence for claustrum involvement’, Patru and Reser (10) hypothesize that disruption of claustral function may contribute to the pathogenesis of psychotic symptoms, delusions in particular. The authors’ suggestion is intriguing and should spark interest in an understudied and potentially relevant brain region. Importantly, direct evidence supporting the hypothesis is lacking, and a more cautious approach would be to broaden the discussion to psychoses rather than only delusions. The authors review the anatomy and functional connectivity of the claustrum, as well as hypotheses regarding its function, and place it in the context of the broad, yet sometimes confusing, clinical literature regarding claustral lesions in patients with delusions or psychotic behavior. A major confound of these studies is the extent of the lesions, which in most cases extended well beyond the claustrum. They then provide a number of intriguing hypotheses for how the claustrum may be responsible for the formation of delusions. Interestingly, it has recently been proposed that salvinorin A, a hallucinogenic kappa opioid agonist, may be acting on the claustrum, causing a disruption of the conscious binding of information to form an accurate representation of the surrounding world (11). This hypothesis is compatible with the proposal of Patru and Reser, and both are cohesive with the evolving notion of a potential role for the claustrum in selective allocation of attention, potentially through prioritizing salience to selected objects, reducing the salience of non-prioritized objects. Delusions are profoundly debilitating psychotic symptoms. Although conceptual models for these symptoms are still highly speculative, it is plausible, as suggested by Patru and Reser, that a common feature is impairment of the ability to accurately assess the ‘validity’ of the interpretation of reality. Breakdown of the assessment process may lead patients to adopt constructs of reality that contain implausible characterizations. Altered sensory gating and perception, reported in patients with major psychoses, may represent components of such breakdown and resonate with growing evidence on claustral functions. Several authors have suggested that aberrant salience, involving complex neural circuits including the striatum, amygdala, and several cortical regions, may represent a key element underlying psychoses (12 16). In a classic treatise, Kapur (17) proposed that psychosis is a state of aberrant salience, causing delusions to evolve gradually in schizophrenic patients. Initially, patients develop heightened sensory receptiveness and increased awareness of the environment, in which many individual elements, which were previously ignored, now gain significant salience. This subtly altered perception of the world leaves the patients overwhelmed and confused, and therefore delusions are suggested to be a ‘top down’ cognitive explanation that the individual imposes on experiences of aberrant salience, in an effort to make sense of them. This explanation ties together the proposed role of the claustrum in mediating selective attention (8) with the notion proposed by Patru and Reser (and previously by Cascella (18, 19)) that disruption of the claustrum could lead to the development of delusions in schizophrenia. With this perspective, disturbance of claustral function, either through direct disruption of the claustrum, or by a circuit-level effect on inputs to the claustrum, would disrupt the appropriate segregation of salience to distinct objects in the world, overwhelming the individual with inputs. Interestingly, the most significant inputs to the claustrum arise from the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate (1, 4, 20). Potentially, in the context of the hypothesis raised by Patru and Reser, these prefrontal inputs could be defining the ‘attentional strategy’ implemented by the claustrum. Thus, disruption of the prefrontal inputs to the claustrum may lead to acceptance of non-conventional and implausible constructs emerging from the integration of sensory inputs. With this perspective in mind, similar arguments could be made regarding expected disruptions of the claustrum in other situations of attention deficits, such as attention deficit disorder (21 23). Kapur suggests that dopamine is a major component defining salience (16). As expression of Drd1dopamine receptors appears to extend within the mouse claustrum (www.mouse.brain-map.org), it will be interesting to test
幽闭错觉
屏状体是位于脑岛中间的一条薄薄的神经元带,它既有趣又未被充分研究。由于屏状体与大面积的皮质具有特别密集的相互连接(1),并且有过多的神经调节输入,因此关于其功能,屏状体一直是许多假设的对象。这些假设包括屏状核在整合感官信息以创造统一的意识体验(2)、皮质同步(3)、皮质功能网络调节(4)、显著性检测(5,6)、主动感知(7)和注意力分离(8,9)中的作用。有趣的是,所有这些假设都围绕着一个共同的主题,即使我们能够形成对周围世界的准确和有凝聚力的表征。Patru和Reser(10)在他们的文章《幻相状态的新视角——幻相参与的证据》中假设,幻相功能的破坏可能有助于精神病症状的发病机制,尤其是妄想。作者的建议很有趣,应该会激发人们对一个尚未得到充分研究但可能相关的大脑区域的兴趣。重要的是,支持这一假设的直接证据是缺乏的,更谨慎的做法是将讨论范围扩大到精神病,而不仅仅是妄想。作者回顾了屏状体的解剖和功能连接,以及关于其功能的假设,并将其置于广泛但有时令人困惑的临床文献中,这些文献涉及患有妄想或精神病行为的患者的屏状体病变。这些研究的一个主要困惑是病变的范围,在大多数情况下,病变的范围远远超出了屏状体。然后,他们提出了一些有趣的假设,说明屏状核是如何导致错觉的形成的。有趣的是,最近有人提出,salvinorin A,一种致幻阿片受体激动剂,可能作用于屏状体,导致有意识的信息绑定中断,以形成对周围世界的准确表征(11)。这一假设与Patru和Reser的建议是一致的,并且两者都与屏状核在选择性注意力分配中的潜在作用的不断发展的概念是一致的,可能通过优先突出选定的物体,降低非优先对象的突出性。妄想是严重的使人衰弱的精神病症状。尽管这些症状的概念模型仍然是高度推测性的,但正如Patru和Reser所提出的,一个共同特征是准确评估对现实解释的“有效性”的能力受损,这是合理的。评估过程的崩溃可能导致患者采用包含不可信特征的现实结构。在重度精神病患者中报告的感觉门控和知觉的改变,可能是这种崩溃的组成部分,并与越来越多的关于闭孔功能的证据产生共鸣。一些作者认为,包括纹状体、杏仁核和几个皮质区域在内的复杂神经回路的异常突出可能是精神病的关键因素(12,16)。Kapur(17)在一篇经典论文中提出,精神病是一种异常突出的状态,导致精神分裂症患者的妄想逐渐演变。最初,患者的感觉接受能力增强,对环境的意识增强,在这种环境中,许多以前被忽视的个体因素现在变得非常突出。这种对世界的感知的微妙改变使患者不知所措和困惑,因此,妄想被认为是一种“自上而下”的认知解释,个体强加于异常突出的经历,以努力理解它们。这一解释将屏状体在调节选择性注意中的作用(8)与Patru和Reser(以及之前的Cascella(18,19))提出的概念联系在一起,即屏状体的破坏可能导致精神分裂症妄想的发展。从这个角度来看,对屏状体功能的干扰,无论是通过直接破坏屏状体,还是通过对屏状体输入的回路级影响,都会破坏对世界上不同物体的适当隔离,使个体无法承受输入。有趣的是,对屏状体最重要的输入来自眶额皮质和前扣带(1,4,20)。在Patru和Reser提出的假设的背景下,这些前额叶输入可能定义了屏状体实施的“注意力策略”。因此,对屏状体的前额叶输入的中断可能导致接受来自感官输入整合的非传统和不可信的构象。 考虑到这一观点,在其他注意缺陷的情况下,如注意缺陷障碍,也可以提出类似的论点(21 23)。Kapur认为多巴胺是决定显著性的主要因素(16)。由于drd1多巴胺受体的表达似乎在小鼠屏状体中延伸(www.mouse.brain-map.org),因此测试将是有趣的
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信