Mummers and moshers : Two rituals of trust in changing social environments

Ethnology Pub Date : 2005-03-22 DOI:10.2307/3773994
C. Palmer
{"title":"Mummers and moshers : Two rituals of trust in changing social environments","authors":"C. Palmer","doi":"10.2307/3773994","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"This article compares two seemingly disparate rituals: the disguised house-visiting in rural Newfoundland known as \"mumming,\" and the aggressive dancing known as \"moshing\" that occurs at concerts of some popular music. Both are activities where participants place themselves at risk of harm at the hands of other participants, and both can be seen as rituals simultaneously demonstrating trust and trustworthiness. For such rituals to be successful in promoting trusting relationships, there must be a pre-existing trust among the participants, and the rituals must be closely governed by spoken or unspoken rules that maintain the fine line separating demonstrations of trust from a violence that can injure and destroy relationships. This hypothesis is examined by the effect of the absence of these conditions on the two rituals. (Risk, trust, Mumming, Moshing) This article compares two activities that appear to be very different from each other: \"mumming\" and \"moshing.\" Mumming (also known as \"mummering,\" or \"janneying\") was a traditional form of disguised Christmas house-visiting that occurred in the small fishing villages of Newfoundland, Canada. Moshing (also known as slam dancing) is a seemingly aggressive colliding of bodies that occurs with members of the audience at concerts featuring such forms of music as punk rock and metal in an area known as a \"mosh pit.\" Despite their apparent differences, both modes of behavior are a form of costly signaling that I refer to as rituals of trust. This hypothesis is evaluated by examining how both rituals are altered by changes in the social environment in which they occur. TRUST, RITUALS, AND COSTLY SIGNALING THEORY \"For the better part of our evolutionary history ... the well-being of any individual rested heavily in the hands of others\" (Wiessner 2002:21). Humans are constantly faced with situations where there are benefits to co-operating with others, but these may involve the risk that the other person may defect, cheat, or take advantage for short-term benefits. Trust is what reduces that risk and allows having the long-term mutual benefits of co-operation, or what is known in evolutionary theory as reciprocal altruism (Trivers 1971). The confidence that the other person will act as expected is trust, that which allows overcoming \"immediate self-interest in anticipation of delayed returns\" (Wiessner 2002:23). Because \"trust can form the basis for mutually supportive relationships that would not be possible without such trust\" (Irons 2001:292), it is of great \"interest in organizational research and the social sciences\" (Fichman 2003:133), with examples ranging from team-building exercises in corporations (Dyer 1987) to political science studies of trust among nation states (Kydd 2000). As the familiarity of the phrases \"rites of intensification\" and \"rites of solidarity\" suggest, many anthropologists have assumed that certain rituals--i.e., repetitive forms of stereotyped social co-operation (see Lavenda and Schultz 2003:72; Levinson 1996:194)--are forms of communication that influence people to refrain from taking short-term advantages in order to reap larger, longer-term benefits, and to trust other individuals to do the same. However, the link between rituals and co-operative relationships is still a subject of debate. Recently there has been an attempt to use Costly Signaling Theory (CST) to explain exactly how some rituals have the communicative effect of building trust and commitment in long-term co-operative relationships (Atran 2002; Cronk 1994; Frank 1988; Irons 2001; Smith and Bird 1999; Sosis 2000a, 2000b, 2003, 2004; Sosis and Alcorta 2003; Sosis and Bressler 2003). This approach attempts to account for religious rituals \"that are costly in time [e.g., praying five times per day] and sometimes in other ways\" (Irons 2001:293), such as pain (e.g., the sun dance), energy (e.g., the sacrifice of food or money, or prolonged dancing), or risk (e. …","PeriodicalId":81209,"journal":{"name":"Ethnology","volume":"44 1","pages":"147-166"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2005-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2307/3773994","citationCount":"14","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ethnology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.2307/3773994","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14

Abstract

This article compares two seemingly disparate rituals: the disguised house-visiting in rural Newfoundland known as "mumming," and the aggressive dancing known as "moshing" that occurs at concerts of some popular music. Both are activities where participants place themselves at risk of harm at the hands of other participants, and both can be seen as rituals simultaneously demonstrating trust and trustworthiness. For such rituals to be successful in promoting trusting relationships, there must be a pre-existing trust among the participants, and the rituals must be closely governed by spoken or unspoken rules that maintain the fine line separating demonstrations of trust from a violence that can injure and destroy relationships. This hypothesis is examined by the effect of the absence of these conditions on the two rituals. (Risk, trust, Mumming, Moshing) This article compares two activities that appear to be very different from each other: "mumming" and "moshing." Mumming (also known as "mummering," or "janneying") was a traditional form of disguised Christmas house-visiting that occurred in the small fishing villages of Newfoundland, Canada. Moshing (also known as slam dancing) is a seemingly aggressive colliding of bodies that occurs with members of the audience at concerts featuring such forms of music as punk rock and metal in an area known as a "mosh pit." Despite their apparent differences, both modes of behavior are a form of costly signaling that I refer to as rituals of trust. This hypothesis is evaluated by examining how both rituals are altered by changes in the social environment in which they occur. TRUST, RITUALS, AND COSTLY SIGNALING THEORY "For the better part of our evolutionary history ... the well-being of any individual rested heavily in the hands of others" (Wiessner 2002:21). Humans are constantly faced with situations where there are benefits to co-operating with others, but these may involve the risk that the other person may defect, cheat, or take advantage for short-term benefits. Trust is what reduces that risk and allows having the long-term mutual benefits of co-operation, or what is known in evolutionary theory as reciprocal altruism (Trivers 1971). The confidence that the other person will act as expected is trust, that which allows overcoming "immediate self-interest in anticipation of delayed returns" (Wiessner 2002:23). Because "trust can form the basis for mutually supportive relationships that would not be possible without such trust" (Irons 2001:292), it is of great "interest in organizational research and the social sciences" (Fichman 2003:133), with examples ranging from team-building exercises in corporations (Dyer 1987) to political science studies of trust among nation states (Kydd 2000). As the familiarity of the phrases "rites of intensification" and "rites of solidarity" suggest, many anthropologists have assumed that certain rituals--i.e., repetitive forms of stereotyped social co-operation (see Lavenda and Schultz 2003:72; Levinson 1996:194)--are forms of communication that influence people to refrain from taking short-term advantages in order to reap larger, longer-term benefits, and to trust other individuals to do the same. However, the link between rituals and co-operative relationships is still a subject of debate. Recently there has been an attempt to use Costly Signaling Theory (CST) to explain exactly how some rituals have the communicative effect of building trust and commitment in long-term co-operative relationships (Atran 2002; Cronk 1994; Frank 1988; Irons 2001; Smith and Bird 1999; Sosis 2000a, 2000b, 2003, 2004; Sosis and Alcorta 2003; Sosis and Bressler 2003). This approach attempts to account for religious rituals "that are costly in time [e.g., praying five times per day] and sometimes in other ways" (Irons 2001:293), such as pain (e.g., the sun dance), energy (e.g., the sacrifice of food or money, or prolonged dancing), or risk (e. …
装模作样者和捣碎者:变化的社会环境中的两种信任仪式
这篇文章比较了两种看似完全不同的仪式:在纽芬兰农村被称为“mumming”的变相家访,以及在一些流行音乐演唱会上被称为“moshing”的激进舞蹈。两者都是参与者将自己置于被其他参与者伤害的风险之中的活动,两者都可以被视为同时展示信任和可信赖性的仪式。这样的仪式要想成功地促进信任关系,参与者之间必须有一种预先存在的信任,仪式必须受到口头或隐性规则的严格约束,以保持信任展示与可能伤害和破坏关系的暴力之间的微妙界限。这一假设是由缺乏这些条件对两种仪式的影响来检验的。这篇文章比较了两种看起来非常不同的活动:“Mumming”和“Moshing”。Mumming(也被称为“mummering”或“janneying”)是一种传统的伪装圣诞拜访形式,发生在加拿大纽芬兰的小渔村。Moshing(也被称为slam dancing)是一种看似激烈的身体碰撞,发生在以朋克摇滚和金属等音乐形式为特色的音乐会上,在一个被称为“mosh pit”的地方。尽管存在明显的差异,但这两种行为模式都是一种代价高昂的信号形式,我称之为信任的仪式。通过研究这两种仪式是如何随着它们发生的社会环境的变化而改变的,可以对这一假设进行评估。信任、仪式和昂贵的信号理论“在我们进化史的大部分时间里……个人的幸福很大程度上取决于他人”(Wiessner 2002:21)。人类经常面临与他人合作有好处的情况,但这可能涉及到另一个人可能背叛、欺骗或利用短期利益的风险。信任可以降低这种风险,并允许长期互利合作,或者在进化理论中被称为互惠利他主义(Trivers 1971)。相信他人会按照预期行事就是信任,它可以克服“对延迟回报的预期带来的直接利益”(Wiessner 2002:23)。因为“信任可以形成相互支持关系的基础,如果没有这种信任,这种关系是不可能存在的”(Irons 2001:292),它是“组织研究和社会科学的极大兴趣”(Fichman 2003:133),其例子从公司的团队建设练习(Dyer 1987)到民族国家之间信任的政治科学研究(Kydd 2000)。正如人们对“强化仪式”和“团结仪式”这两个短语所熟悉的那样,许多人类学家认为,某些仪式——例如:-重复形式的刻板社会合作(见Lavenda and Schultz 2003:72);Levinson 1996:194)是一种影响人们不为获得更大、更长期利益而利用短期利益,并信任其他人也会这样做的沟通形式。然而,仪式和合作关系之间的联系仍然是一个有争议的话题。最近,有人尝试使用成本信号理论(CST)来准确解释一些仪式如何在长期合作关系中建立信任和承诺的沟通效应(Atran 2002;体弱的1994;弗兰克1988;铁2001;Smith and Bird 1999;Sosis 2000a, 2000b, 2003, 2004;Sosis and Alcorta 2003;Sosis and Bressler 2003)。这种方法试图解释宗教仪式“在时间上(例如,每天祈祷五次),有时以其他方式”(Irons 2001:293),如痛苦(例如,太阳舞),能量(例如,牺牲食物或金钱,或长时间的舞蹈),或风险(例如. ...)
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信