Kinship and marriage among the Omaha, 1886-1902

Ethnology Pub Date : 2003-01-01 DOI:10.2307/3773806
B. Ensor
{"title":"Kinship and marriage among the Omaha, 1886-1902","authors":"B. Ensor","doi":"10.2307/3773806","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"\"Omaha\" kinship is a major model for patrilineal kinship and marital exchanges. However, some authors have suggested that kinship rules and unilineal descent are merely theoretical constructs of anthropologists or cultural ideals usually not followed in practice. Given the importance of \"Omaha\" kinship for theory, this article tests the normative rules for marriage against empirical data on actual marriage behavior among the late-nineteenth-century Omaha tribe of Nebraska using Bureau of Indian Affairs census rolls. The results confirm that the majority of Omaha did indeed follow the normative rules upon which the \"Omaha\" model is based. The implications for kinship studies is that descent theory and alliance models can still be considered valid approaches to societies prior to historic changes. (Omaha, Crow-Omaha exchange, patrilocal, patrilineal) ********** A trend among some anthropologists is to claim that classificatory kinship models are theoretical constructs imposed upon cultures by Euro-American ethnographers or that the models are normative descriptions of cultural ideals rarely practiced. Schneider (1984) critiques several preconceived assumptions on kinship that influence ethnographers' interpretations. One of Schneider's main criticisms is the assumption that non-Western cultures conceptualize biological relations in the same way as Western cultures, which he claims is the basis for descent theory (Carsten 2000; Franklin and McKinnon 2001; Schneider 1984). The realization that biological affinity is not conceptualized, operationalized, or even important to social life from one culture to another has given way to studies on relatedness in an effort to reinvent understandings of kinship (e.g., Carsten 2000; Franklin and McKinnon 2001). These studies emphasize multiple ways that individuals relate to one another: through descent relations, affinal relations, friendship relations, political relations, and economic relations (e.g., Hutchinson 2000; Stafford 2000). Relations are thus seen as actively manipulated and reconceptualized within changing cultural contexts. Another manner in which anthropologists are beginning to understand relatedness is through \"house\" theory. Although Levi-Strauss (1982, 1987) originally thought of his proposed house societies as one category alongside descent categories, more recent proponents of house theory tend to argue that people's relations rarely conform to the descent models (e.g., Gillespie 2000a; Carsten and Hugh-Jones 1995). Gillespie (2000b: 1) summarizes this view in her opening paragraph: \"Ethnographic descriptions have dispelled the notion that prescriptive and proscriptive kinship `rules' govern social life.\" Schneider (1984) also claimed that an illogical separation of biological relations from political and economic relations characterized much of kinship theory prior to the 1970s. However, there is a tradition in anthropology for viewing a relationship between these inseparable parts. Social organization, and hence descent, are interrelated with political and economic relations, while marriage preferences and postmarital residence reproduce those social relations (e.g., Gjessing 1975; Godelier 1984; Morgan 1870; Moore 1991; Peletz 1995; Schweizer and White 1998; Wolf 1982:88-96). If we accept this premise, which seems to be the direction the \"reinvented\" (e.g., Carsten 2000) understanding of kinship is going, then in a modern era of intensified incorporation in global capitalism we should expect changes in the ways people understand their relatedness. However, we should not conclude that modern changing cultures reflect social relations in their nineteenth-century and earlier ascendant cultures, which much of descent theory was based upon. With the exception of Levi-Strauss's (1982, 1987) house societies that were based on ethnohistorical documentation, many ethnographies purported to contradict descent theory are in fact post-World War II. …","PeriodicalId":81209,"journal":{"name":"Ethnology","volume":"44 1","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2003-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2307/3773806","citationCount":"15","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ethnology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.2307/3773806","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15

Abstract

"Omaha" kinship is a major model for patrilineal kinship and marital exchanges. However, some authors have suggested that kinship rules and unilineal descent are merely theoretical constructs of anthropologists or cultural ideals usually not followed in practice. Given the importance of "Omaha" kinship for theory, this article tests the normative rules for marriage against empirical data on actual marriage behavior among the late-nineteenth-century Omaha tribe of Nebraska using Bureau of Indian Affairs census rolls. The results confirm that the majority of Omaha did indeed follow the normative rules upon which the "Omaha" model is based. The implications for kinship studies is that descent theory and alliance models can still be considered valid approaches to societies prior to historic changes. (Omaha, Crow-Omaha exchange, patrilocal, patrilineal) ********** A trend among some anthropologists is to claim that classificatory kinship models are theoretical constructs imposed upon cultures by Euro-American ethnographers or that the models are normative descriptions of cultural ideals rarely practiced. Schneider (1984) critiques several preconceived assumptions on kinship that influence ethnographers' interpretations. One of Schneider's main criticisms is the assumption that non-Western cultures conceptualize biological relations in the same way as Western cultures, which he claims is the basis for descent theory (Carsten 2000; Franklin and McKinnon 2001; Schneider 1984). The realization that biological affinity is not conceptualized, operationalized, or even important to social life from one culture to another has given way to studies on relatedness in an effort to reinvent understandings of kinship (e.g., Carsten 2000; Franklin and McKinnon 2001). These studies emphasize multiple ways that individuals relate to one another: through descent relations, affinal relations, friendship relations, political relations, and economic relations (e.g., Hutchinson 2000; Stafford 2000). Relations are thus seen as actively manipulated and reconceptualized within changing cultural contexts. Another manner in which anthropologists are beginning to understand relatedness is through "house" theory. Although Levi-Strauss (1982, 1987) originally thought of his proposed house societies as one category alongside descent categories, more recent proponents of house theory tend to argue that people's relations rarely conform to the descent models (e.g., Gillespie 2000a; Carsten and Hugh-Jones 1995). Gillespie (2000b: 1) summarizes this view in her opening paragraph: "Ethnographic descriptions have dispelled the notion that prescriptive and proscriptive kinship `rules' govern social life." Schneider (1984) also claimed that an illogical separation of biological relations from political and economic relations characterized much of kinship theory prior to the 1970s. However, there is a tradition in anthropology for viewing a relationship between these inseparable parts. Social organization, and hence descent, are interrelated with political and economic relations, while marriage preferences and postmarital residence reproduce those social relations (e.g., Gjessing 1975; Godelier 1984; Morgan 1870; Moore 1991; Peletz 1995; Schweizer and White 1998; Wolf 1982:88-96). If we accept this premise, which seems to be the direction the "reinvented" (e.g., Carsten 2000) understanding of kinship is going, then in a modern era of intensified incorporation in global capitalism we should expect changes in the ways people understand their relatedness. However, we should not conclude that modern changing cultures reflect social relations in their nineteenth-century and earlier ascendant cultures, which much of descent theory was based upon. With the exception of Levi-Strauss's (1982, 1987) house societies that were based on ethnohistorical documentation, many ethnographies purported to contradict descent theory are in fact post-World War II. …
奥马哈人的亲属关系和婚姻,1886-1902
“奥马哈”亲属关系是父系亲属关系和婚姻交往的主要模式。然而,一些作者认为,亲属关系规则和单系血统仅仅是人类学家的理论建构或文化理想,通常在实践中不被遵循。鉴于“奥马哈”亲属关系在理论上的重要性,本文利用印第安事务局的人口普查数据,对19世纪后期内布拉斯加州奥马哈部落的实际婚姻行为的经验数据进行了婚姻规范规则的检验。结果证实,奥马哈的大多数人确实遵循了“奥马哈”模型所依据的规范性规则。对亲属关系研究的启示是,血统理论和联盟模型仍然可以被认为是研究历史变化之前社会的有效方法。(奥马哈,克劳-奥马哈交流,父系,父系)**********在一些人类学家中有一种趋势,认为分类亲属模型是欧美民族志学家强加给文化的理论建构,或者这些模型是对很少实践的文化理想的规范性描述。施耐德(1984)批评了几个先入为主的亲属关系假设,这些假设影响了民族志学者的解释。施耐德的主要批评之一是假设非西方文化以与西方文化相同的方式概念化生物关系,他声称这是血统理论的基础(Carsten 2000;Franklin and McKinnon 2001;施耐德1984)。认识到生物亲缘关系不是概念化的、可操作的,甚至对从一种文化到另一种文化的社会生活也不重要,这让位于对亲缘关系的研究,以努力重塑对亲属关系的理解(例如,Carsten 2000;富兰克林和麦金农2001)。这些研究强调个体相互联系的多种方式:通过血统关系、亲缘关系、友谊关系、政治关系和经济关系(例如,Hutchinson 2000;斯塔福德2000)。因此,在不断变化的文化背景下,关系被视为积极地操纵和重新概念化。人类学家开始理解亲缘关系的另一种方式是通过“房屋”理论。虽然Levi-Strauss(1982,1987)最初认为他提出的家庭社会是与血统类别一起的一个类别,但最近房屋理论的支持者倾向于认为人们的关系很少符合血统模型(例如,Gillespie 2000a;Carsten and Hugh-Jones 1995)。Gillespie (2000b: 1)在她的开篇段落中总结了这一观点:“人种学的描述已经消除了规定的和禁止的亲属‘规则’支配社会生活的观念。”施耐德(1984)还声称,在20世纪70年代之前,血缘关系与政治和经济关系的不合逻辑的分离是亲属理论的主要特征。然而,在人类学中有一种传统,即观察这些不可分割部分之间的关系。社会组织和血统与政治和经济关系是相互关联的,而婚姻偏好和婚后居住再现了这些社会关系(例如,Gjessing 1975;Godelier 1984;摩根1870;摩尔1991;Peletz 1995;Schweizer and White 1998;狼1982:88 - 96)。如果我们接受这个前提,这似乎是“重新发明”(例如,Carsten 2000)对亲属关系的理解的方向,那么在与全球资本主义紧密结合的现代时代,我们应该期待人们理解他们之间关系的方式发生变化。然而,我们不应该得出这样的结论,即现代不断变化的文化反映了19世纪和更早的上升文化中的社会关系,而这是大部分血统理论所依据的。除了Levi-Strauss(1982, 1987)基于民族历史文献的家庭社会之外,许多声称与血统理论相矛盾的民族志实际上是在第二次世界大战之后。…
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信