Histories of Ethology: Methods, Sites, and Dynamics of an Unbound Discipline

IF 0.6 2区 哲学 Q2 HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE
Sophia Gräfe, Cora Stuhrmann
{"title":"Histories of Ethology: Methods, Sites, and Dynamics of an Unbound Discipline","authors":"Sophia Gräfe,&nbsp;Cora Stuhrmann","doi":"10.1002/bewi.202200026","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ethology is considered the leading biological discipline within behavioral research in the 20th century. Its history is told as a seemingly straightforward narrative: Ethology has its roots in the 1930s in German-speaking countries, a disciplinary heyday in the 1950s and 1960s, after which it slowly lost relevance. It employs a distinct approach to the comparative study of animal behavior, which is characterized by a physiological method of non-invasive, often observational studies of natural behavioral patterns, which were conceived of as shaped by evolution. Ethology contains stories of charismatic research animals such as the jackdaw <i>Tschock</i> or the goose <i>Martina</i>,<sup>1</sup> draws on academic disciplines such as ornithology, ichthyology, and entomology,<sup>2</sup> and also incorporates contexts and practices of animal lovers, bird watchers, and hunters, as well as those involved in animal husbandry, wildlife preservation, and livestock farming, or who work in nature reserves or zoological gardens.<sup>3</sup> Ethology is further connected to the development of certain visual media, such as chronophotography and the film loop,<sup>4</sup> and corresponding forms of perception, such as pattern recognition<sup>5</sup> or comparative visual analysis.<sup>6</sup> Other methodical highlights include the ethogram,<sup>7</sup> dummies,<sup>8</sup> and the Kaspar Hauser experiment.<sup>9</sup> The history of ethology conventionally focuses on several elements: an illustrious circle of founding figures, indeed founding “fathers,” such as Konrad Lorenz (1903–1989), Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907–1988), Karl von Frisch (1886–1982), Jakob von Uexküll (1864–1944),<sup>10</sup> Oskar Heinroth (1871–1945),<sup>11</sup> Erwin Stresemann (1889–1972),<sup>12</sup> and Otto Koehler (1889–1974),<sup>13</sup> the importance of the <i>Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie</i> (later renamed <i>Ethology</i>),<sup>14</sup> legendary encounters of individual scholars with their research subjects<sup>15</sup> and colleagues during conferences<sup>16</sup>, and towering intellectual achievements such as famous talks<sup>17</sup> or foundational monographs such as Tinbergen's <i>The Study of Instinct, published in 1951</i><sup>18</sup>. Further markers of ethology's disciplinary history are the recognition of its achievement and disciplinary status with the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine awarded to Lorenz, Tinbergen and von Frisch in 1973 and the ensuing controversy about Lorenz's political background.<sup>19</sup></p><p>All scientific disciplines, of course, are outlined by a set of protagonists, places, publications, and practices.<sup>20</sup> However, ethology is characterized by an unusual preoccupation with its own disciplinary status, demarcating its core and periphery, its borders and boundaries as well as delineating its historical lineage and possible trajectories—all adding to the sense that ethologists’ historical accounts are full of strategic self-historization and purposeful narratives. Consequently, historians of science have engaged with this core question of disciplinary identity, unveiling the layers of strategic narratives employed by the actors and adding complexity to their presentations of disciplinary success stories. Historians of science, ethologists turned historians and ethologists themselves offered narratives of ethology, some motivated by scholarly curiosity, others by a desire for commemoration, historical legitimacy, or strategic considerations.</p><p>This special issue contributes to the historiography of ethology beyond the classical phase and reflects ethology's nature as an unbound discipline that is not as uniform and cohesive as the historical efforts of Lorenz and other early ethologists made it out to be.<sup>21</sup> It invites an interdisciplinary audience to rediscover the history of ethology from the perspectives of epistemology, animal studies, media studies as well as publication and data history. Contributions in this issue focus on novel methods, hidden sites, and overlooked dynamics and exemplify a wide range of approaches, including archival work, multi-media analysis, oral history interviews, and publication tracing for a multi-focal history of ethology. We engage with central countries in the history and historiography of ethology: German-speaking countries, the United Kingdom, and USA. While we acknowledge the limits of this choice, the historiography of ethology is shaped by the Lorenz-Tinbergen axis in Germany and the United Kingdom, with some of the most important ethological and historical research carried out in connection or succession to the scientific impact they had in each country.<sup>22</sup> Therefore, writing the history of ethology includes dealing with the historical contexts and social motives of these localized disciplinary histories. We will recount the central authors, phases, and genres in the historiography of ethology, ending with a reflection on current themes.</p><p>The work of historians and ethologists over the last decades has shown that ethology was never a monolithic, cohesive framework, but instead the result of social dynamics, strategic agendas, and contingent developments, containing diverse approaches and diverging theoretical premises. Its disciplinary boundaries are porous and permeable. We therefore suggest understanding ethology as an unbound discipline from its very inception. This allows us to add new perspectives of its further unbounding, especially by looking at researchers that were not considered to be in the mainstream of ethology and after what is considered its classical stage. Our contributions explore different dynamics of ethology's development. Novel methods allowed an unbounding from the constraints of time and life span: photography, card indexing, and long term data collection moved the object of study from individual animal behaviors to synthesized behavior repertoires over ever-increasing time spans. Similarly, an unbounding from limits placed on research subjects and questions took place, such as the inclusion of human behavior or animal cognition into the scope of ethological research. These changes also imply an unbounding from classic sites of knowledge production in ethology and our contributions explore new sites such as long-term field stations in Kenya, a research compound in the East German part of Berlin, a eugenically minded dog research station in the US as well as Munich kindergartens. These dynamics add complexity to the perception of ethology as a disciplinary monolith and, instead, pose the question of how ethology is kept together as a discipline. Our contributions point to the essentially social nature of ethology as a discipline through shared institutions and publications organs, academic genealogies and lineages, and communal research stations and vibrant university departments.</p><p><b>Kelle Dhein</b> sheds light on Karl von Frisch's hitherto unclarified relationship with ethology regarding experimental methods. The focus of Dhein's article is the grey card experimental method developed by von Frisch in the 1910s. Combining a holistic view of the behavior of his research subjects with a controlled, at times reductionist experimental design, von Frisch succeeded in producing quantifiable research results which provided information about the sensory perception of the bees in his experiments. Among other aspects, the conditioning of the bees, which was necessary for his experiments at the time, represented a clear departure from the methodologies of classical ethology, even leading to von Frisch's exclusion from ethologist circles. It is one of the paradoxes of the history of ethology that precisely this experimental design would become a key component in later phases of the discipline's history and, from the 1970s onwards, play a significant role in the success of research projects associated with the field of neuroethology. In examining how an experiment “has a life on its own,”<sup>72</sup> thus traversing different premises, questions and directions of research, Dhein succeeds in tracing a new line in the history of ethology.</p><p><b>Sophia Gräfe</b>’s contribution also adopts a praxeological approach. Her study of visual media in classical ethology adds archives to the existing catalogue of significant sites in the history of the discipline. Taking as her subject the ethological image collection established by Günter Tembrock at the <i>Forschungsstätte für Tierpsychologie</i> in Berlin, she demonstrates the extent to which these images, along with other visual media such as drawings and film recordings, significantly advanced the ethological search for innate forms of behavior. Images were used to assist <i>Gestalt</i> perception and pattern recognition. They also served as a memory aid, as well as to display the scientists’ detailed knowledge and research experience to their colleagues. In addition, collections of images provided a basis for developing the clearest possible nomenclature of behavior, which, as an ephemeral phenomenon, required new forms of representation. Since the images in ethological collections generally come from various sources, and their motifs span lengthy periods of time, Gräfe's analysis underlines the predominantly collective character of comparative behavioral research. Finally, an examination of the history of ethology from the perspective of its visual media addresses the synthetic character of the knowledge acquired about the behavior of living beings. Tembrock's seminal research on the behavior of red foxes was the result of both his experiments with the animals in the behavioral laboratory and the expert analysis of archive images.</p><p><b>Brad Bolman</b> achieves a multi-layered portrait of canine behavioral studies in the US by using approaches from model organism research. Previous historiographies had so far excluded these studies from the history of ethology and thus overlooked the <i>canine</i> history of this line of behavioral research. The projects reconstructed by Bolman at the intersection of eugenics, genetics, behavioral research, and animal breeding are situated on the periphery of classical ethology and form the starting point for a historiographical intervention: Charles Rupert Stockard, Clarence Cook Little, Raymond C. and Lorna Coppinger and Harry and Martha G. Frank, among others, all studied canine behavior based on the hypothesis of a <i>critical period</i>, in other words certain phases in the early development of living beings in which the influence of specific environmental conditions determines the development of specific behaviors. The gradual incorporation of the critical period concept into everyday usage since 1950 and its more recent establishment as an analytical category in the history of science evokes teleological narratives. Rather than portraying the birth of the discipline as the result of favorable conditions, Bolman's study highlights the winding paths of its history, marked by interruptions and detours.</p><p>By the time Lorenz, Tinbergen and Frisch received the Nobel Prize in 1973, the heyday of classical ethology may already have passed. <b>Jakob Odenwald</b> shows in his contribution how from the late 1960s onwards a new field emerged in comparative behavioral research, when biologist Barbara Hold initiated the innovative “Kindergarten Project,” which served as an excellent example for the developing field of human ethology. Following the principle of Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt's “Cross-Cultural Long-Term Study” (<i>Kulturvergleichende Langzeitstudie</i>), children at several kindergartens in the Munich area became the protagonists of a behavioral study. The idea was that as research subjects the children would provide undistorted access to the natural foundations of social behavior. Odenwald's contribution decisively highlights the political background of the Kindergarten Project, in which the focus of the research was social ranking behavior. The educational approach varied among the groups in the study—a group with an anti-authoritarian approach was also included. Against a backdrop of social debate about class and social justice, Hold's theses on the biological foundations of social ranking provided ample fuel for discussion, as Odenwald illustrates using samples of the surviving correspondence between the scientific, public, and private actors.</p><p>The long-term behavioral biology projects gathered together in <b>Erika Milam</b>’s contribution also colonized new spaces. In their search for cross-generational mechanisms underlying the development of social behavior, population biologists, primatologists, and sociobiologists shifted their research from the laboratory to the field and focused on long-term studies from the 1960s and 1970s onwards. While research locations are already a common theme in the history of ethology, Milam's study focuses on the time factor and correlates specific questions about behavior with the timelines of the research. One effect of adopting this perspective is the emergence of new protagonists in the history of ethology, including local researchers who continued collecting data in the absence of principal investigators, and data analysts who used their skills to analyze vast amounts of behavioral data. Milam's account also draws attention to the biographical side-effects of this new long-term perspective on behavior. In interviews with Kenneth Armitage, Jeanne Altmann, Timothy Hugh Clutton-Brock, and others, she outlines how the professional environment was transformed, turning some researchers into migrating project workers and others into decades-long companions of “their” animal population.</p><p>Finally, <b>Cora Stuhrmann</b> examines the development of two disciplines that followed ethology. By the 1970s, ethology had lost its methodological coherence. The growing emphasis on mathematical methods was undermining the leading status of its key figures, some of whom had by then reached old age. Stuhrmann's examination of the fate of ethology as a discipline explores the dynamics between ethology, behavioral ecology and sociobiology at the inter-disciplinary level and adds the analytical framework of competition to the history of the discipline. She identifies the genre of edited volumes, textbooks, and their respective reviews as the forum of this driving social force, interpreting these publications as strategic components of scientific negotiation processes. The key finding of her study is the negation of the frequently raised question of whether ethology has died. From the loss of relevance of its central theoretical foundations and the rise of a new generation of researchers we can conclude that although ethology has lost relevance as a discipline, it has expanded terminologically to include all studies on animal behavior with a biological foundation.</p>","PeriodicalId":55388,"journal":{"name":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","volume":"45 1-2","pages":"10-29"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6000,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bewi.202200026","citationCount":"2","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte","FirstCategoryId":"98","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bewi.202200026","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"哲学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"HISTORY & PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2

Abstract

Ethology is considered the leading biological discipline within behavioral research in the 20th century. Its history is told as a seemingly straightforward narrative: Ethology has its roots in the 1930s in German-speaking countries, a disciplinary heyday in the 1950s and 1960s, after which it slowly lost relevance. It employs a distinct approach to the comparative study of animal behavior, which is characterized by a physiological method of non-invasive, often observational studies of natural behavioral patterns, which were conceived of as shaped by evolution. Ethology contains stories of charismatic research animals such as the jackdaw Tschock or the goose Martina,1 draws on academic disciplines such as ornithology, ichthyology, and entomology,2 and also incorporates contexts and practices of animal lovers, bird watchers, and hunters, as well as those involved in animal husbandry, wildlife preservation, and livestock farming, or who work in nature reserves or zoological gardens.3 Ethology is further connected to the development of certain visual media, such as chronophotography and the film loop,4 and corresponding forms of perception, such as pattern recognition5 or comparative visual analysis.6 Other methodical highlights include the ethogram,7 dummies,8 and the Kaspar Hauser experiment.9 The history of ethology conventionally focuses on several elements: an illustrious circle of founding figures, indeed founding “fathers,” such as Konrad Lorenz (1903–1989), Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907–1988), Karl von Frisch (1886–1982), Jakob von Uexküll (1864–1944),10 Oskar Heinroth (1871–1945),11 Erwin Stresemann (1889–1972),12 and Otto Koehler (1889–1974),13 the importance of the Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie (later renamed Ethology),14 legendary encounters of individual scholars with their research subjects15 and colleagues during conferences16, and towering intellectual achievements such as famous talks17 or foundational monographs such as Tinbergen's The Study of Instinct, published in 195118. Further markers of ethology's disciplinary history are the recognition of its achievement and disciplinary status with the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine awarded to Lorenz, Tinbergen and von Frisch in 1973 and the ensuing controversy about Lorenz's political background.19

All scientific disciplines, of course, are outlined by a set of protagonists, places, publications, and practices.20 However, ethology is characterized by an unusual preoccupation with its own disciplinary status, demarcating its core and periphery, its borders and boundaries as well as delineating its historical lineage and possible trajectories—all adding to the sense that ethologists’ historical accounts are full of strategic self-historization and purposeful narratives. Consequently, historians of science have engaged with this core question of disciplinary identity, unveiling the layers of strategic narratives employed by the actors and adding complexity to their presentations of disciplinary success stories. Historians of science, ethologists turned historians and ethologists themselves offered narratives of ethology, some motivated by scholarly curiosity, others by a desire for commemoration, historical legitimacy, or strategic considerations.

This special issue contributes to the historiography of ethology beyond the classical phase and reflects ethology's nature as an unbound discipline that is not as uniform and cohesive as the historical efforts of Lorenz and other early ethologists made it out to be.21 It invites an interdisciplinary audience to rediscover the history of ethology from the perspectives of epistemology, animal studies, media studies as well as publication and data history. Contributions in this issue focus on novel methods, hidden sites, and overlooked dynamics and exemplify a wide range of approaches, including archival work, multi-media analysis, oral history interviews, and publication tracing for a multi-focal history of ethology. We engage with central countries in the history and historiography of ethology: German-speaking countries, the United Kingdom, and USA. While we acknowledge the limits of this choice, the historiography of ethology is shaped by the Lorenz-Tinbergen axis in Germany and the United Kingdom, with some of the most important ethological and historical research carried out in connection or succession to the scientific impact they had in each country.22 Therefore, writing the history of ethology includes dealing with the historical contexts and social motives of these localized disciplinary histories. We will recount the central authors, phases, and genres in the historiography of ethology, ending with a reflection on current themes.

The work of historians and ethologists over the last decades has shown that ethology was never a monolithic, cohesive framework, but instead the result of social dynamics, strategic agendas, and contingent developments, containing diverse approaches and diverging theoretical premises. Its disciplinary boundaries are porous and permeable. We therefore suggest understanding ethology as an unbound discipline from its very inception. This allows us to add new perspectives of its further unbounding, especially by looking at researchers that were not considered to be in the mainstream of ethology and after what is considered its classical stage. Our contributions explore different dynamics of ethology's development. Novel methods allowed an unbounding from the constraints of time and life span: photography, card indexing, and long term data collection moved the object of study from individual animal behaviors to synthesized behavior repertoires over ever-increasing time spans. Similarly, an unbounding from limits placed on research subjects and questions took place, such as the inclusion of human behavior or animal cognition into the scope of ethological research. These changes also imply an unbounding from classic sites of knowledge production in ethology and our contributions explore new sites such as long-term field stations in Kenya, a research compound in the East German part of Berlin, a eugenically minded dog research station in the US as well as Munich kindergartens. These dynamics add complexity to the perception of ethology as a disciplinary monolith and, instead, pose the question of how ethology is kept together as a discipline. Our contributions point to the essentially social nature of ethology as a discipline through shared institutions and publications organs, academic genealogies and lineages, and communal research stations and vibrant university departments.

Kelle Dhein sheds light on Karl von Frisch's hitherto unclarified relationship with ethology regarding experimental methods. The focus of Dhein's article is the grey card experimental method developed by von Frisch in the 1910s. Combining a holistic view of the behavior of his research subjects with a controlled, at times reductionist experimental design, von Frisch succeeded in producing quantifiable research results which provided information about the sensory perception of the bees in his experiments. Among other aspects, the conditioning of the bees, which was necessary for his experiments at the time, represented a clear departure from the methodologies of classical ethology, even leading to von Frisch's exclusion from ethologist circles. It is one of the paradoxes of the history of ethology that precisely this experimental design would become a key component in later phases of the discipline's history and, from the 1970s onwards, play a significant role in the success of research projects associated with the field of neuroethology. In examining how an experiment “has a life on its own,”72 thus traversing different premises, questions and directions of research, Dhein succeeds in tracing a new line in the history of ethology.

Sophia Gräfe’s contribution also adopts a praxeological approach. Her study of visual media in classical ethology adds archives to the existing catalogue of significant sites in the history of the discipline. Taking as her subject the ethological image collection established by Günter Tembrock at the Forschungsstätte für Tierpsychologie in Berlin, she demonstrates the extent to which these images, along with other visual media such as drawings and film recordings, significantly advanced the ethological search for innate forms of behavior. Images were used to assist Gestalt perception and pattern recognition. They also served as a memory aid, as well as to display the scientists’ detailed knowledge and research experience to their colleagues. In addition, collections of images provided a basis for developing the clearest possible nomenclature of behavior, which, as an ephemeral phenomenon, required new forms of representation. Since the images in ethological collections generally come from various sources, and their motifs span lengthy periods of time, Gräfe's analysis underlines the predominantly collective character of comparative behavioral research. Finally, an examination of the history of ethology from the perspective of its visual media addresses the synthetic character of the knowledge acquired about the behavior of living beings. Tembrock's seminal research on the behavior of red foxes was the result of both his experiments with the animals in the behavioral laboratory and the expert analysis of archive images.

Brad Bolman achieves a multi-layered portrait of canine behavioral studies in the US by using approaches from model organism research. Previous historiographies had so far excluded these studies from the history of ethology and thus overlooked the canine history of this line of behavioral research. The projects reconstructed by Bolman at the intersection of eugenics, genetics, behavioral research, and animal breeding are situated on the periphery of classical ethology and form the starting point for a historiographical intervention: Charles Rupert Stockard, Clarence Cook Little, Raymond C. and Lorna Coppinger and Harry and Martha G. Frank, among others, all studied canine behavior based on the hypothesis of a critical period, in other words certain phases in the early development of living beings in which the influence of specific environmental conditions determines the development of specific behaviors. The gradual incorporation of the critical period concept into everyday usage since 1950 and its more recent establishment as an analytical category in the history of science evokes teleological narratives. Rather than portraying the birth of the discipline as the result of favorable conditions, Bolman's study highlights the winding paths of its history, marked by interruptions and detours.

By the time Lorenz, Tinbergen and Frisch received the Nobel Prize in 1973, the heyday of classical ethology may already have passed. Jakob Odenwald shows in his contribution how from the late 1960s onwards a new field emerged in comparative behavioral research, when biologist Barbara Hold initiated the innovative “Kindergarten Project,” which served as an excellent example for the developing field of human ethology. Following the principle of Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt's “Cross-Cultural Long-Term Study” (Kulturvergleichende Langzeitstudie), children at several kindergartens in the Munich area became the protagonists of a behavioral study. The idea was that as research subjects the children would provide undistorted access to the natural foundations of social behavior. Odenwald's contribution decisively highlights the political background of the Kindergarten Project, in which the focus of the research was social ranking behavior. The educational approach varied among the groups in the study—a group with an anti-authoritarian approach was also included. Against a backdrop of social debate about class and social justice, Hold's theses on the biological foundations of social ranking provided ample fuel for discussion, as Odenwald illustrates using samples of the surviving correspondence between the scientific, public, and private actors.

The long-term behavioral biology projects gathered together in Erika Milam’s contribution also colonized new spaces. In their search for cross-generational mechanisms underlying the development of social behavior, population biologists, primatologists, and sociobiologists shifted their research from the laboratory to the field and focused on long-term studies from the 1960s and 1970s onwards. While research locations are already a common theme in the history of ethology, Milam's study focuses on the time factor and correlates specific questions about behavior with the timelines of the research. One effect of adopting this perspective is the emergence of new protagonists in the history of ethology, including local researchers who continued collecting data in the absence of principal investigators, and data analysts who used their skills to analyze vast amounts of behavioral data. Milam's account also draws attention to the biographical side-effects of this new long-term perspective on behavior. In interviews with Kenneth Armitage, Jeanne Altmann, Timothy Hugh Clutton-Brock, and others, she outlines how the professional environment was transformed, turning some researchers into migrating project workers and others into decades-long companions of “their” animal population.

Finally, Cora Stuhrmann examines the development of two disciplines that followed ethology. By the 1970s, ethology had lost its methodological coherence. The growing emphasis on mathematical methods was undermining the leading status of its key figures, some of whom had by then reached old age. Stuhrmann's examination of the fate of ethology as a discipline explores the dynamics between ethology, behavioral ecology and sociobiology at the inter-disciplinary level and adds the analytical framework of competition to the history of the discipline. She identifies the genre of edited volumes, textbooks, and their respective reviews as the forum of this driving social force, interpreting these publications as strategic components of scientific negotiation processes. The key finding of her study is the negation of the frequently raised question of whether ethology has died. From the loss of relevance of its central theoretical foundations and the rise of a new generation of researchers we can conclude that although ethology has lost relevance as a discipline, it has expanded terminologically to include all studies on animal behavior with a biological foundation.

动物行为学的历史:一门不受约束的学科的方法、地点和动态
行为学被认为是20世纪行为研究中领先的生物学学科。动物行为学的历史以一种看似直截了当地的叙述方式讲述:动物行为学起源于20世纪30年代的德语国家,在20世纪50年代和60年代是学科的鼎盛时期,之后它慢慢失去了相关性。它采用一种独特的方法对动物行为进行比较研究,其特点是一种非侵入性的生理方法,通常是对自然行为模式的观察性研究,这种研究被认为是由进化形成的。动物行为学包含有魅力的研究动物的故事,如寒鸦Tschock或鹅Martina,1借鉴了鸟类学、鱼类学和昆虫学等学科,2还结合了动物爱好者、鸟类观察者和猎人的背景和实践,以及那些参与畜牧业、野生动物保护和畜牧业的人,或在自然保护区或动物园工作的人动物行为学还与某些视觉媒介的发展有进一步的联系,如时间摄影和电影循环,以及相应的感知形式,如模式识别或比较视觉分析其他有系统的亮点包括直方图,假人,和卡斯帕·豪瑟实验动物行为学的历史通常集中在以下几个方面:一群杰出的奠基人,实际上是奠基人,如康拉德·洛伦兹(1903-1989)、尼古拉斯·廷伯根(1907-1988)、卡尔·冯·弗里施(1886-1982)、雅各布·冯·尤克斯k<e:2>(1864-1944)、奥斯卡·海因罗斯(1871-1945)、欧文·施特雷泽曼(1889-1972)、奥托·克勒(1889-1974)、时代心理学(后更名为动物行为学)的重要性、学者个人在会议期间与其研究对象和同事的传奇遭遇。以及杰出的智力成就,如著名的演讲,或奠基性的专著,如丁伯根于1951年出版的《本能的研究》。行为学学科历史的进一步标志是1973年诺贝尔生理学或医学奖授予洛伦兹、丁伯根和冯·弗里施,以及随后关于洛伦兹政治背景的争议,从而承认了它的成就和学科地位。当然,所有的科学学科都是由一系列的主角、地点、出版物和实践来概括的然而,动物行为学的特点是不寻常地关注自己的学科地位,划定其核心和外围,边界和边界,以及描绘其历史谱系和可能的轨迹——所有这些都增加了动物行为学家的历史叙述充满战略自我历史化和有目的的叙述的感觉。因此,科学史家一直在研究学科认同这一核心问题,揭示了参与者所采用的战略叙事的层次,并增加了他们对学科成功故事的描述的复杂性。从科学史家、动物行为学家转变为历史学家,动物行为学家自己也提供了动物行为学的叙述,有些是出于学术好奇心,有些是出于纪念、历史合法性或战略考虑的愿望。这期特刊对超越古典阶段的动物行为学史学做出了贡献,反映了动物行为学作为一门不受约束的学科的本质,它不像洛伦兹和其他早期动物行为学家所做的历史努力那样统一和有凝聚力它邀请跨学科的观众从认识论、动物研究、媒体研究以及出版和数据历史的角度重新发现动物行为学的历史。本期的贡献集中在新的方法、隐藏的地点和被忽视的动态,并举例说明了广泛的方法,包括档案工作、多媒体分析、口述历史访谈和多焦点动物行为学历史的出版追踪。我们从事与中央国家在历史和行为学的史学:德语国家,英国和美国。虽然我们承认这种选择的局限性,但动物行为学的史学是由德国和英国的洛伦兹-丁伯根轴心塑造的,其中一些最重要的动物行为学和历史研究与他们在每个国家的科学影响有关或接续进行因此,写动物行为学的历史包括处理这些局部学科历史的历史背景和社会动机。我们将叙述动物行为学史学的中心作者、阶段和流派,并以对当前主题的反思结束。 历史学家和动物行为学家在过去几十年的工作表明,动物行为学从来不是一个单一的、有凝聚力的框架,而是社会动态、战略议程和偶然发展的结果,包含了不同的方法和不同的理论前提。它的学科界限是多孔的和可渗透的。因此,我们建议从一开始就把动物行为学理解为一门不受约束的学科。这使我们能够为其进一步的突破增加新的视角,特别是通过观察那些不被认为是动物行为学主流的研究人员,以及在被认为是其经典阶段之后的研究人员。我们的贡献探讨了动物行为学发展的不同动态。新的方法允许从时间和寿命的限制中解脱出来:摄影,卡片索引和长期数据收集将研究对象从个体动物行为转移到合成行为库,时间跨度不断增加。同样,对研究对象和问题的限制也出现了突破,例如将人类行为或动物认知纳入动物行为学研究的范围。这些变化也意味着从行为学知识生产的经典地点脱离出来,我们的贡献探索了新的地点,如肯尼亚的长期野外站,柏林东德部分的研究综合体,美国的优生思想狗研究站以及慕尼黑幼儿园。这些动态增加了动物行为学作为一个学科整体的复杂性,相反,提出了动物行为学如何作为一门学科保持在一起的问题。我们的贡献表明,通过共享的机构和出版机构、学术谱系和血统、公共研究站和充满活力的大学院系,动物行为学作为一门学科的本质是社会性质。凯勒·戴恩阐明了卡尔·冯·弗里施迄今未澄清的关于实验方法的动物行为学的关系。戴恩文章的重点是冯·弗里施在20世纪10年代发展起来的灰卡实验方法。冯·弗里施将他的研究对象的行为的整体观点与受控的、有时是简化的实验设计相结合,成功地产生了可量化的研究结果,这些结果提供了有关实验中蜜蜂感官知觉的信息。在其他方面,蜜蜂的条件反射,这是他当时的实验所必需的,代表了对经典动物行为学方法的明显背离,甚至导致冯·弗里施被排除在动物行为学圈子之外。这是行为学历史上的一个悖论,正是这个实验设计将成为该学科历史后期阶段的关键组成部分,并从20世纪70年代开始,在与神经行为学领域相关的研究项目的成功中发挥了重要作用。在考察一个实验如何“有自己的生命”时,从而穿越了不同的前提、问题和研究方向,戴恩成功地在动物行为学的历史上开辟了一条新的路线。Sophia Gräfe的贡献也采用了行动学的方法。她在古典动物行为学中对视觉媒体的研究为该学科历史上现有的重要地点目录增添了档案。她以柏林Forschungsstätte <s:2> Tierpsychologie的g<s:1> nter Tembrock建立的动物行为学图像集为研究对象,展示了这些图像以及其他视觉媒体(如绘画和电影记录)在多大程度上显著推进了对先天行为形式的动物行为学研究。图像被用来辅助格式塔感知和模式识别。它们还可以作为记忆辅助工具,以及向同事展示科学家详细的知识和研究经验。此外,图像集合为发展最清晰的行为命名提供了基础,行为作为一种短暂的现象,需要新的表现形式。由于动物行为学收藏中的图像通常来自不同的来源,并且它们的主题跨越了很长一段时间,Gräfe的分析强调了比较行为研究的主要集体特征。最后,从视觉媒介的角度考察动物行为学的历史,探讨生物行为知识的综合特征。Tembrock对红狐行为的开创性研究是他在行为实验室对动物进行实验和对档案图像进行专家分析的结果。布拉德·博尔曼通过使用模式生物研究的方法,在美国实现了犬类行为研究的多层肖像。到目前为止,以前的史学编纂将这些研究排除在动物行为学的历史之外,因此忽视了这一行为研究领域的犬类历史。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte
Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 社会科学-科学史与科学哲学
CiteScore
0.80
自引率
16.70%
发文量
43
审稿时长
>12 weeks
期刊介绍: Die Geschichte der Wissenschaften ist in erster Linie eine Geschichte der Ideen und Entdeckungen, oft genug aber auch der Moden, Irrtümer und Missverständnisse. Sie hängt eng mit der Entwicklung kultureller und zivilisatorischer Leistungen zusammen und bleibt von der politischen Geschichte keineswegs unberührt.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信