{"title":"Sustained Attention: Alternative to Joint Attention or Ambiguous Concept?","authors":"Kimberley M. Hudspeth, C. Lewis","doi":"10.1159/000515681","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"For forty years it has been assumed that joint attention is a driving force in the development of early communication (Bates, 1979), and much evidence has been provided to support this idea. Shared attention at the end of the infant’s first year has been found to relate to the infant’s first words (Baldwin, 1995; Bates, 1979; Bruner, 1974; Markus et al., 2000; Tomasello, 1988, 1995; Tomasello & Todd, 1983), early learning (Striano et al., 2006), emotion regulation (Morales et al., 2005), social development (Mundy & Sigman, 2006; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2007) and early symbolic thinking (Mundy & Jarrold, 2010). A few recent investigations have suggested an alternative position, that it is infants’ abilities to sustain attention rather than share a focus with their caregiver, that may reveal the underlying mechanism for these achievements, particularly their early vocabulary growth (Yu et al., 2019). Although this literature is far smaller, it has received a lot of interest. Not only has it been used to explain individual differences in vocabulary acquisition (Brooks et al., 2018), it has also been shown to relate to cognitive performance, notably problem solving in the later toddler period (Choudhury & Gorman, 2000). This recent research posits a challenge to traditional theories, suggesting that joint attention may merely be a proxy for the ability to sustain a focus on objects (toys and people), as the true driving force behind later developmental abilities. Given the centrality of joint attention as a construct (Carpenter et al., 1998), the theoretical implications could be major. Sustained attention is characterised by a focus or fixation on a particular stimulus. The recent literature is not wholly clear about what this reveals, but it is implied that attention is a demonstration of the ability to be more connected to objects and, therefore, an ability to use this information to develop more complex associations – what Richards and Casey (1992) term “information processing.” Yet to have to compete with, or replace, joint attention, the construct of sustained attention needs much more critical analysis. The issues with this concept can be divided into 2 main areas, measurement and definitional problems, which we explore in turn before exploring some deeper theoretical concerns.","PeriodicalId":47837,"journal":{"name":"Human Development","volume":"65 1","pages":"67 - 71"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4000,"publicationDate":"2021-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000515681","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Human Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000515681","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Abstract
For forty years it has been assumed that joint attention is a driving force in the development of early communication (Bates, 1979), and much evidence has been provided to support this idea. Shared attention at the end of the infant’s first year has been found to relate to the infant’s first words (Baldwin, 1995; Bates, 1979; Bruner, 1974; Markus et al., 2000; Tomasello, 1988, 1995; Tomasello & Todd, 1983), early learning (Striano et al., 2006), emotion regulation (Morales et al., 2005), social development (Mundy & Sigman, 2006; Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2007) and early symbolic thinking (Mundy & Jarrold, 2010). A few recent investigations have suggested an alternative position, that it is infants’ abilities to sustain attention rather than share a focus with their caregiver, that may reveal the underlying mechanism for these achievements, particularly their early vocabulary growth (Yu et al., 2019). Although this literature is far smaller, it has received a lot of interest. Not only has it been used to explain individual differences in vocabulary acquisition (Brooks et al., 2018), it has also been shown to relate to cognitive performance, notably problem solving in the later toddler period (Choudhury & Gorman, 2000). This recent research posits a challenge to traditional theories, suggesting that joint attention may merely be a proxy for the ability to sustain a focus on objects (toys and people), as the true driving force behind later developmental abilities. Given the centrality of joint attention as a construct (Carpenter et al., 1998), the theoretical implications could be major. Sustained attention is characterised by a focus or fixation on a particular stimulus. The recent literature is not wholly clear about what this reveals, but it is implied that attention is a demonstration of the ability to be more connected to objects and, therefore, an ability to use this information to develop more complex associations – what Richards and Casey (1992) term “information processing.” Yet to have to compete with, or replace, joint attention, the construct of sustained attention needs much more critical analysis. The issues with this concept can be divided into 2 main areas, measurement and definitional problems, which we explore in turn before exploring some deeper theoretical concerns.
四十年来,人们一直认为共同注意是早期沟通发展的驱动力(Bates, 1979),并且提供了许多证据来支持这一观点。在婴儿一岁结束时的共同注意已被发现与婴儿的第一个单词有关(Baldwin, 1995;贝茨,1979;布鲁纳,1974;Markus et al., 2000;Tomasello, 1988, 1995;Tomasello & Todd, 1983),早期学习(Striano et al., 2006),情绪调节(Morales et al., 2005),社会发展(Mundy & Sigman, 2006;Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2007)和早期符号思维(Mundy & Jarrold, 2010)。最近的一些调查提出了另一种观点,即婴儿保持注意力而不是与照顾者分享注意力的能力,可能揭示了这些成就的潜在机制,特别是他们早期的词汇增长(Yu et al., 2019)。虽然这方面的文献要少得多,但却引起了很多人的兴趣。它不仅被用来解释词汇习得的个体差异(Brooks et al., 2018),也被证明与认知表现有关,特别是在幼儿后期解决问题的能力(Choudhury & Gorman, 2000)。最近的这项研究对传统理论提出了挑战,表明共同注意力可能仅仅是对物体(玩具和人)持续关注的能力的一种代表,是后期发展能力的真正驱动力。鉴于联合注意作为一种结构的中心性(Carpenter et al., 1998),其理论意义可能是重大的。持续注意力的特征是对特定刺激的集中或固定。最近的文献并不完全清楚这揭示了什么,但它暗示了注意力是一种能力的展示,它与物体有更多的联系,因此,有能力利用这些信息来发展更复杂的联系——Richards和Casey(1992)称之为“信息处理”。然而,要与共同注意力竞争或取代共同注意力,持续注意力的构建需要更批判性的分析。这个概念的问题可以分为两个主要领域,测量和定义问题,我们在探讨一些更深层次的理论问题之前依次探讨。
期刊介绍:
Distinguished by its international recognition since 1958, "Human Development" publishes in-depth conceptual articles, commentaries, and essay book reviews that advance our understanding of developmental phenomena. Contributions serve to raise theoretical issues, flesh out interesting and potentially powerful ideas, and differentiate key constructs. Contributions are welcomed from varied disciplines, including anthropology, biology, education, history, philosophy, psychology, and sociology.