[The adolescent's lexicon: study of lexical variations in normal adolescents depending upon the listener].

La psychiatrie de l'enfant Pub Date : 1995-01-01
V Granboulan, D Chauvin, M Basquin, C Peltier, J F Le Ny
{"title":"[The adolescent's lexicon: study of lexical variations in normal adolescents depending upon the listener].","authors":"V Granboulan,&nbsp;D Chauvin,&nbsp;M Basquin,&nbsp;C Peltier,&nbsp;J F Le Ny","doi":"","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Clinicians and parents are familiar with the fact that adolescents have a special vocabulary, but very few studies have examined this. Linguists describe it as deeply metaphoric, creative and lively, thus showing that young people have a deep knowledge of language and truly experience pleasure using words. This contrasts with teachers' complaints about the little taste adolescents show for oral school activities and how poorly they express themselves. Some of them link this to the use of this polysemic and all purpose vocabulary. The context of locution is probably the explanation for these diverging opinions. Using this hypothesis, we have realised a quantitative study of the lexical variations depending on the person the adolescent is talking to in two groups (20 and 19 subjects), from very different social and educational backgrounds. Each teen-ager had to perform the same linguistic task: the description of a photograph on two occasions, once with an adult examiner and once with a friend. We studied the lexical differences between the two narratives. When adolescents are together they use their particular vocabulary four times more than when with an adult. But this qualitative difference is not a quantitative one, such as the length of the narrative or the number and repetition of whole words, and isn't correlated with the lexical stock. The use of this vocabulary runs across gender and social class categories. It can equally be found in high performance and upper class students as well as in underprivileged youngsters of technical schooling. It is the only variable that does not change between the two high schools. Thus this special vocabulary would not be connected to the subject's lexical competence, nor to gender or social background. It is the psychological function of this language that seems to be prominent.</p>","PeriodicalId":513375,"journal":{"name":"La psychiatrie de l'enfant","volume":"38 2","pages":"655-91"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1995-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"La psychiatrie de l'enfant","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Clinicians and parents are familiar with the fact that adolescents have a special vocabulary, but very few studies have examined this. Linguists describe it as deeply metaphoric, creative and lively, thus showing that young people have a deep knowledge of language and truly experience pleasure using words. This contrasts with teachers' complaints about the little taste adolescents show for oral school activities and how poorly they express themselves. Some of them link this to the use of this polysemic and all purpose vocabulary. The context of locution is probably the explanation for these diverging opinions. Using this hypothesis, we have realised a quantitative study of the lexical variations depending on the person the adolescent is talking to in two groups (20 and 19 subjects), from very different social and educational backgrounds. Each teen-ager had to perform the same linguistic task: the description of a photograph on two occasions, once with an adult examiner and once with a friend. We studied the lexical differences between the two narratives. When adolescents are together they use their particular vocabulary four times more than when with an adult. But this qualitative difference is not a quantitative one, such as the length of the narrative or the number and repetition of whole words, and isn't correlated with the lexical stock. The use of this vocabulary runs across gender and social class categories. It can equally be found in high performance and upper class students as well as in underprivileged youngsters of technical schooling. It is the only variable that does not change between the two high schools. Thus this special vocabulary would not be connected to the subject's lexical competence, nor to gender or social background. It is the psychological function of this language that seems to be prominent.

[青少年的词汇:正常青少年的词汇变化对听者的影响研究]。
临床医生和家长都知道青少年有特殊的词汇,但很少有研究对此进行调查。语言学家将其描述为隐喻性很强,富有创造性,生动活泼,这表明年轻人对语言有很深的了解,真正体验到使用语言的乐趣。这与老师们抱怨青少年对学校口头活动缺乏兴趣以及表达能力差形成了鲜明对比。他们中的一些人将此与多义词和通用词汇的使用联系起来。说话的语境可能是对这些不同意见的解释。利用这一假设,我们已经实现了一项词汇变化的定量研究,这取决于青少年在两组(20和19个受试者)中交谈的人,他们来自非常不同的社会和教育背景。每个青少年都必须完成同样的语言任务:在两种情况下描述一张照片,一次是和成人考官,一次是和朋友。我们研究了两种叙述的词汇差异。当青少年和成年人在一起时,他们使用特定词汇的次数是和成年人在一起时的四倍。但这种质上的差异并不是量上的差异,比如叙述的长度或整个单词的数量和重复,也与词汇储备无关。这些词汇的使用跨越了性别和社会阶层类别。它同样可以在表现优异的上层阶级学生以及贫困的技术学校的年轻人中发现。这是两所高中之间唯一不变的变量。因此,这种特殊词汇不会与主体的词汇能力、性别或社会背景联系在一起。这种语言的心理功能似乎很突出。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信