How Can Psychologists Walk the Walk to Promote Racial Justice? Situating Racial Justice Intervention Research Within the Translational Research Framework
{"title":"How Can Psychologists Walk the Walk to Promote Racial Justice? Situating Racial Justice Intervention Research Within the Translational Research Framework","authors":"Nao Hagiwara, Peter Mende-Siedlecki","doi":"10.1111/josi.70019","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The field of psychology boasts a long and robust history of engaging in scientific examinations of racism, as evidenced by the works of Archer and Archer (<span>1970</span>), Van den Berghe (<span>1962</span>), Clark and Clark (<span>1939</span>), Langer (<span>1967</span>), Pettigrew et al. (<span>1958</span>), Pettigrew (<span>1960</span>), and Thomas (<span>1970</span>). Psychologists across the world have endeavored to better understand the origins (e.g., Benner and Graham <span>2013</span>; Federico and Sidanius <span>2002</span>; Louis et al. <span>2013</span>; Sears and Henry <span>2003</span>), underlying mechanisms (e.g., Cuddy et al. <span>2008</span>; Dovidio et al. <span>2004</span>; Duckitt et al. <span>2002</span>; Wilson et al. <span>2000</span>), and consequences (e.g., Brown et al. <span>2000</span>; David et al. <span>2019</span>; Neblett Jr <span>2019</span>; Spencer et al. <span>2016</span>) of racial stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. These inquiries extend across the entire lifespan of our field, as prominent research programs have focused on phenomena associated with racism from its infancy to its contemporary state.</p><p>Despite these extensive efforts, however, racism and racial injustice persist today (Bourabain and Verhaeghe <span>2021</span>; Williams <span>2021</span>). People who have been racially and ethnically minoritized (we will use the phrase “minoritized people” from here on) continue to experience racism firsthand in their daily lives (ABC News <span>2020</span>; Institute for Social Research <span>2023</span>; Konate <span>2023</span>; Pew Research Center <span>2019</span>). Substantiating these personal accounts, documented instances of racial injustice permeate every social domain, including education, criminal justice, community safety and autonomy, employment, and healthcare, across countries and regions such as Australia (Australian Human Rights Commission <span>2022</span>), New Zealand (Stats NZ Tatauranga Aotearoa <span>2024</span>), South Africa (South African Human Rights Commission <span>2017</span>), the United Kingdom (Institute of Race Relations <span>2024</span>), Canada (Cotter <span>2022</span>), the United States (Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health <span>2022</span>), and Europe (European Network Against Racism <span>2012</span>; Statista Research Department, <span>2025</span>). These examples of racism are inherently intertwined with parallel cases of racialized xenophobia—for example, a surge in anti-Asian attitudes during and following the COVID-19 pandemic (Gover et al. <span>2020</span>; He et al. <span>2021</span>). While these examples focus primarily on racism associated with White supremacy culture, we acknowledge that examples of racism and racist ideologies span the entire globe (Agier <span>1995</span>; Bora <span>2019</span>; Busey and Coleman-King <span>2023</span>; Modood and Sealy <span>2022</span>; Sambaraju <span>2021</span>).</p><p>Racial injustice has not only survived in the face of psychological inquiry—it has thrived. Indeed, the recent years have been marked by a historical resurgence of public expressions of White supremacy across the world (Beirich <span>2013</span>; Simi and Futrell <span>2020</span>; Youngblood <span>2020</span>). Critically, these escalations of White supremacy culture are closely linked with the promotion of political radicalization that legitimizes racially and ethnically discriminatory laws and policies (Adamczyk et al. <span>2014</span>; Bilewicz and Soral <span>2020</span>; Rees et al. <span>2019</span>). Examples include voter suppression bills disproportionately targeting racially and ethnically minoritized Americans (Wilder <span>2021</span>); hostile immigration measures unfairly affecting immigrants and asylum seekers from non-European countries in Italy (Gualtieri <span>2024</span>), Denmark (Abend <span>2019</span>), Hungary (Kiss <span>2016</span>), Poland (Waterbury <span>2020</span>), and the United Kingdom (Griffiths and Trebilcock <span>2023</span>); and prohibitions of facial coverings targeting Muslim women in over 30 countries (Open Society Justice Initiative <span>2022</span>). In response to the global upsurge in overt racism, many researchers have offered elegant psychological explanations for the rise of White supremacy culture, xenophobia, hate, and dehumanization, even drawing parallels between these contemporary expressions and their historical antecedents in regimes like Nazi Germany (Esses and Hamilton <span>2021</span>; Markowitz and Slovic <span>2020</span>; Pretus et al. <span>2023</span>; Reyna et al. <span>2022</span>). This implies that while the discipline of psychology is capable of offering adequate scientific insights into racism, this knowledge has not been effectively translated into real improvements in the lives of minoritized people.</p><p>This special issue was conceived in direct response to concerns that are both chronic and acute—the long-simmering frustrations with the slow pace of progress toward racial justice and the urgent need arising from the global resurgence of White supremacy culture in the present moment. As we will discuss more extensively later in this introductory paper, the key impediment hindering the translation of our psychological understanding of racism into the effective promotion of racial justice lies in psychologists’ failure to foreground the reciprocal relationship between individuals and systems. Too often, we as psychologists ignore this critical interplay between individuals’ racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and overarching factors associated with systemic racism when we engage in the process of theorizing, designing studies, and testing interventions.</p><p>The purpose of this special issue is to offer both theoretical guidance and methodological examples that illustrate how psychologists can contribute to the actual promotion of racial justice across social domains. Critically, we highlight the importance of grounding such work in the Translational Research Framework (TRF). To achieve this, we have compiled seven empirical papers and one review paper, all aimed at promoting racial justice across various domains (e.g., criminal justice, the workplace, healthcare) by anchoring their work in the TRF. We refer to research studying interventions promoting racial justice in real-world settings as “racial justice intervention research” hereafter. Our overarching goal is to inspire psychologists, regardless of whether they are engaged in intervention research or simply dedicated to promoting racial justice, to reevaluate their theoretical orientation and refine their study designs, ensuring they accurately capture the complexity of racism. By doing so, we hope to empower their racial justice intervention research to fully maximize its potential for positive impact on people and communities that have been racially and ethnically minoritized. In the upcoming sections, we will first explore why solely focusing on individual attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors is inadequate in successful racial justice intervention research. Next, we provide a brief overview of the TRF and a discussion of its utility in addressing the limitations associated with the exclusive focus on individuals. Finally, we close by describing the collection of articles in this special issue and illustrating how each one aligns with the TRF.</p><p>Racism is a complex and multi-faceted phenomenon, which reflects an interplay between (a) individual-level racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and (b) system-level racist phenomena that mutually reinforce one another (Melson-Silimon et al. <span>2023</span>; Payne et al. <span>2019</span>; Roberts and Rizzo <span>2021</span>; Salter et al. <span>2018</span>; Trawalter et al. <span>2020</span>). Put differently, individuals both exist within and are shaped by racist systems, while these systems are also deliberately designed and sustained by those individuals. For example, negative attitudes toward and stereotypes about minoritized people (i.e., individual-level phenomena) are fueled and maintained by factors like residential segregation, negative media portrayals of minoritized people, voter disenfranchisement, and financial disparities (i.e., system-level phenomena). Psychology, as the scientific study of the mind and behavior (Feist <span>2008</span>; Skinner <span>1965</span>), tends to concentrate on reducing individuals’ racist attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors in its efforts to promote racial justice (Devine et al. <span>2012</span>; Gaertner and Dovidio <span>2014</span>; Galinsky and Moskowitz <span>2000</span>; Rydell and McConnell <span>2006</span>). This individual-level focus is neither inherently good nor bad, as every discipline has its unique scholarly emphasis. That said, it poses significant obstacles to achieving racial justice when psychologists exclusively focus on individuals and disregard the racist systems in which these individuals are embedded—whether these systems pertain to society in general or specific contexts such as criminal justice or healthcare.</p><p>Consider the analogy of sea birds coated with oil due to a spill. After meticulously cleaning off the oil from their bodies, a rescuer inexplicably returns them to the same oil-contaminated ocean. The flaw is glaring: the sea birds will inevitably be recontaminated. Unfortunately, typical psychological approaches to promoting racial justice—with their exclusive focus on individuals—frequently parallel this scenario. To illustrate, envision humans as sea birds, racist ideologies as oil, and the system as the ocean. While the psychological interventions under study in this work may ultimately reduce individuals’ racist attitudes and beliefs, they neglect the broader systemic context. Just as the sea birds will once again be covered with oil, these individuals are likely to reacquire racist ideologies in the racist systems. Nevertheless, most psychological approaches to racial justice allow individuals to return to an environment filled with racist ideologies and policies after “cleansing” their racist attitudes and beliefs, with the optimistic assumption that their interventions will yield lasting effects.</p><p>This is not to suggest that a psychological approach to racial justice intervention research is of little value. Indeed, interventions that ignore individual racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors also fall short of fully achieving racial justice due to the bidirectional relationship between individual attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and systemic racism. Systems, after all, are composed of people, and considering their psychology—both individual and collective—is critical for bringing about systemic change. Analogously, releasing a large number of oil-contaminated sea birds back into the ocean after a spill, even post-cleansing, could result in the reintegration of oil into the water, disrupting the local ecosystem once again. Thus, psychologists should actively participate in research that aims to develop multi-level interventions that are designed to address both individual-level racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and system-level racist phenomena. Additionally, racist systems are proven to be extremely resistant to change because they are carefully developed and maintained by those in positions of power who benefit from them (Kraus and Callaghan <span>2014</span>; Maxwell and Shields <span>2014</span>; Wilson <span>2009</span>). Consequently, combatting systemic racism—if it is even possible—is a formidable challenge that demands substantial investments of time and collective effort (Johnston <span>1996</span>; Wood <span>2000</span>). This underscores the significant potential for psychologists to take a central role in spearheading efforts to alleviate the current suffering of numerous individuals experiencing racial injustice globally, while scholars and stakeholders continue their work in changing racist laws, policies, and practices.</p><p>Whether psychologists are participating in or leading racial justice intervention research, they must carefully consider the context of systemic racism within which their individual-level interventions are designed, tested, and implemented. We propose that the TRF, which is directly informed by the well-established Clinical and Translational Science (CTS) framework, is a valuable tool for bringing the reciprocal relationship between individual-level factors and systemic racism to the forefront at every stage of intervention design, testing, implementation, and dissemination.</p><p>In recent years, a focus on the intricate relationship between individual- and system-level factors has intensified within the psychological study of racism (Melson-Silimon et al. <span>2023</span>; Modood and Sealy <span>2022</span>; Salter et al. <span>2018</span>; Trawalter et al. <span>2020</span>). At the same time, it must be acknowledged that racial justice intervention research did not start yesterday, and moreover, that psychology is not the only discipline with a longstanding commitment to studying racism. Despite these considerations, many well-intentioned psychologists often find themselves stalled at the T1 stage—or perhaps worse, content with remaining at T1. One factor contributing to this stagnancy is that psychologists typically receive insufficient training in conducting interdisciplinary research (Sewell <span>1989</span>; Swisher <span>2022</span>). In the context of racial justice intervention research within healthcare, an interdisciplinary research team might consist of a diverse array of professionals and backgrounds, including psychologists, epidemiologists, health economists, health services researchers, political scientists, clinicians, health educators/patient advocates, patients, and lobbyists. Each member brings distinct priorities and perspectives to the table. Successfully transforming the challenges arising from diverse perspectives within interdisciplinary collaborations into sources of innovation requires both training and hands-on practice.</p><p>Another contributing factor is the prevalent bias in the field of psychology that favors experimental designs as the “gold standard” (Abrahamse <span>2016</span>; Greenhoot <span>2003</span>; Hunziker and Blankenagel <span>2021</span>; Kaptchuk <span>2001</span>; McCall and Green <span>2004</span>; McDermott <span>2011</span>; Reis and Judd <span>2000</span>; Smith <span>2012</span>). With this bias, many psychologists tend to perceive laboratory experimental studies as more scientifically rigorous than field observational studies (Diener et al. <span>2022</span>). This holds true for many studies in T1 and even T2, where the primary objective is to establish a causal relationship between a given intervention and a desired outcome in highly controlled settings. However, the criteria for rigorous scientific approaches and methods change as research advances through the stages in the TRF. The ability to accurately define scientific rigor and identify the most suitable methodological approaches within each stage is also cultivated through training and practice.</p><p>This special issue addresses both issues by presenting a diverse collection of empirical and review articles that strongly align with the TRF in their approach to racial justice intervention research. It aims to offer both theoretical guidance and methodological examples and to inspire further theorizing, research, interventions, and policy reform in the realm of racial justice intervention research.</p><p>This special issue will consist of four distinct sections, each dedicated to illuminating pervasive racial injustices in a particular social domain: Society in General, Criminal Justice, Workplace, and Healthcare. In each section, we curated papers that represent different stages of the TRF (Figure 1). A distinctive feature of this special issue lies in the authors’ explicit exploration of how their ongoing research contributes to subsequent stages of the TRF and ultimately to potential policy changes. Each paper also highlights the authors’ personal experiences in racial justice intervention research, offering valuable insights. Authors reflect on the imperative need for interdisciplinary collaboration, sharing the challenges encountered and innovative solutions devised in the pursuit of their work. This personal dimension adds depth to the scholarly discourse and underscores the multifaceted nature of addressing racial injustices. Finally, this special issue concludes with a commentary focusing on the promise and potential of racial justice intervention research, as well as the challenges facing the psychologists who engage in this work. By providing a comprehensive overview and synthesizing the unique contributions detailed in each section, this commentary aims to serve as a thought-provoking conclusion, fostering continued dialogue and action in the field of racial justice.</p><p>In sum, this special issue presents a diverse assortment of scholarly work illustrating how racial justice intervention research can be grounded in the TRF. As we argue in this introduction, this is precisely the kind of work that is best positioned to produce real change that improves the lives of minoritized people. In composing this special issue, we were careful to solicit and select examples of research that varied in terms of their domain of focus (e.g., education, criminal justice, healthcare, etc.), the methodology they employed (i.e., quantitative, qualitative, or mixed methods), and the stage of the TRF that they represent. Moreover, in each article, we have asked authors to reflect on their personal experiences of conducting this work, the challenges they faced, and their approaches to overcoming these challenges. We hope these reflections will be of particular use to scholars who are just beginning to familiarize themselves with racial justice intervention research.</p><p>Of course, this collection is not without its limitations. While we actively sought out research that sheds light on various forms of racism, such as anti-Indigenous, anti-Asian, and anti-Muslim racism, from different nations and cultures, the majority of submissions we received focus primarily on anti-Black racism in the US context. As such, with some exceptions, the collected articles can be characterized by this same limited scope. Similarly, we received no submissions demonstrating work at the T4 stage, and relatively few at even the T3 stage. This gap in representation underscores both the challenges and opportunities for psychologists. On the one hand, the gap reflects the inherent challenge of seeing a program of research through to the point of translation to communities due to multiple limitations within the current landscape of psychology as a field, as discussed earlier. On the other hand, psychologists possess significant potential to contribute to the promotion of racial justice by specifically targeting this translational gap. In light of these limitations, we hope that the framework outlined in this special issue and the lessons offered by its contributing authors will motivate researchers to fill the gap. In particular, we are especially eager for psychologists currently working at the T0 and T1 stages or on other expressions of racism (e.g., Asian American and Pacific Islander hate, Islamophobia, etc.) to take the leap and pursue their work in the TRF.</p>","PeriodicalId":17008,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Social Issues","volume":"81 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.7000,"publicationDate":"2025-09-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/josi.70019","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Social Issues","FirstCategoryId":"90","ListUrlMain":"https://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/josi.70019","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
The field of psychology boasts a long and robust history of engaging in scientific examinations of racism, as evidenced by the works of Archer and Archer (1970), Van den Berghe (1962), Clark and Clark (1939), Langer (1967), Pettigrew et al. (1958), Pettigrew (1960), and Thomas (1970). Psychologists across the world have endeavored to better understand the origins (e.g., Benner and Graham 2013; Federico and Sidanius 2002; Louis et al. 2013; Sears and Henry 2003), underlying mechanisms (e.g., Cuddy et al. 2008; Dovidio et al. 2004; Duckitt et al. 2002; Wilson et al. 2000), and consequences (e.g., Brown et al. 2000; David et al. 2019; Neblett Jr 2019; Spencer et al. 2016) of racial stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. These inquiries extend across the entire lifespan of our field, as prominent research programs have focused on phenomena associated with racism from its infancy to its contemporary state.
Despite these extensive efforts, however, racism and racial injustice persist today (Bourabain and Verhaeghe 2021; Williams 2021). People who have been racially and ethnically minoritized (we will use the phrase “minoritized people” from here on) continue to experience racism firsthand in their daily lives (ABC News 2020; Institute for Social Research 2023; Konate 2023; Pew Research Center 2019). Substantiating these personal accounts, documented instances of racial injustice permeate every social domain, including education, criminal justice, community safety and autonomy, employment, and healthcare, across countries and regions such as Australia (Australian Human Rights Commission 2022), New Zealand (Stats NZ Tatauranga Aotearoa 2024), South Africa (South African Human Rights Commission 2017), the United Kingdom (Institute of Race Relations 2024), Canada (Cotter 2022), the United States (Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health 2022), and Europe (European Network Against Racism 2012; Statista Research Department, 2025). These examples of racism are inherently intertwined with parallel cases of racialized xenophobia—for example, a surge in anti-Asian attitudes during and following the COVID-19 pandemic (Gover et al. 2020; He et al. 2021). While these examples focus primarily on racism associated with White supremacy culture, we acknowledge that examples of racism and racist ideologies span the entire globe (Agier 1995; Bora 2019; Busey and Coleman-King 2023; Modood and Sealy 2022; Sambaraju 2021).
Racial injustice has not only survived in the face of psychological inquiry—it has thrived. Indeed, the recent years have been marked by a historical resurgence of public expressions of White supremacy across the world (Beirich 2013; Simi and Futrell 2020; Youngblood 2020). Critically, these escalations of White supremacy culture are closely linked with the promotion of political radicalization that legitimizes racially and ethnically discriminatory laws and policies (Adamczyk et al. 2014; Bilewicz and Soral 2020; Rees et al. 2019). Examples include voter suppression bills disproportionately targeting racially and ethnically minoritized Americans (Wilder 2021); hostile immigration measures unfairly affecting immigrants and asylum seekers from non-European countries in Italy (Gualtieri 2024), Denmark (Abend 2019), Hungary (Kiss 2016), Poland (Waterbury 2020), and the United Kingdom (Griffiths and Trebilcock 2023); and prohibitions of facial coverings targeting Muslim women in over 30 countries (Open Society Justice Initiative 2022). In response to the global upsurge in overt racism, many researchers have offered elegant psychological explanations for the rise of White supremacy culture, xenophobia, hate, and dehumanization, even drawing parallels between these contemporary expressions and their historical antecedents in regimes like Nazi Germany (Esses and Hamilton 2021; Markowitz and Slovic 2020; Pretus et al. 2023; Reyna et al. 2022). This implies that while the discipline of psychology is capable of offering adequate scientific insights into racism, this knowledge has not been effectively translated into real improvements in the lives of minoritized people.
This special issue was conceived in direct response to concerns that are both chronic and acute—the long-simmering frustrations with the slow pace of progress toward racial justice and the urgent need arising from the global resurgence of White supremacy culture in the present moment. As we will discuss more extensively later in this introductory paper, the key impediment hindering the translation of our psychological understanding of racism into the effective promotion of racial justice lies in psychologists’ failure to foreground the reciprocal relationship between individuals and systems. Too often, we as psychologists ignore this critical interplay between individuals’ racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and overarching factors associated with systemic racism when we engage in the process of theorizing, designing studies, and testing interventions.
The purpose of this special issue is to offer both theoretical guidance and methodological examples that illustrate how psychologists can contribute to the actual promotion of racial justice across social domains. Critically, we highlight the importance of grounding such work in the Translational Research Framework (TRF). To achieve this, we have compiled seven empirical papers and one review paper, all aimed at promoting racial justice across various domains (e.g., criminal justice, the workplace, healthcare) by anchoring their work in the TRF. We refer to research studying interventions promoting racial justice in real-world settings as “racial justice intervention research” hereafter. Our overarching goal is to inspire psychologists, regardless of whether they are engaged in intervention research or simply dedicated to promoting racial justice, to reevaluate their theoretical orientation and refine their study designs, ensuring they accurately capture the complexity of racism. By doing so, we hope to empower their racial justice intervention research to fully maximize its potential for positive impact on people and communities that have been racially and ethnically minoritized. In the upcoming sections, we will first explore why solely focusing on individual attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors is inadequate in successful racial justice intervention research. Next, we provide a brief overview of the TRF and a discussion of its utility in addressing the limitations associated with the exclusive focus on individuals. Finally, we close by describing the collection of articles in this special issue and illustrating how each one aligns with the TRF.
Racism is a complex and multi-faceted phenomenon, which reflects an interplay between (a) individual-level racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and (b) system-level racist phenomena that mutually reinforce one another (Melson-Silimon et al. 2023; Payne et al. 2019; Roberts and Rizzo 2021; Salter et al. 2018; Trawalter et al. 2020). Put differently, individuals both exist within and are shaped by racist systems, while these systems are also deliberately designed and sustained by those individuals. For example, negative attitudes toward and stereotypes about minoritized people (i.e., individual-level phenomena) are fueled and maintained by factors like residential segregation, negative media portrayals of minoritized people, voter disenfranchisement, and financial disparities (i.e., system-level phenomena). Psychology, as the scientific study of the mind and behavior (Feist 2008; Skinner 1965), tends to concentrate on reducing individuals’ racist attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors in its efforts to promote racial justice (Devine et al. 2012; Gaertner and Dovidio 2014; Galinsky and Moskowitz 2000; Rydell and McConnell 2006). This individual-level focus is neither inherently good nor bad, as every discipline has its unique scholarly emphasis. That said, it poses significant obstacles to achieving racial justice when psychologists exclusively focus on individuals and disregard the racist systems in which these individuals are embedded—whether these systems pertain to society in general or specific contexts such as criminal justice or healthcare.
Consider the analogy of sea birds coated with oil due to a spill. After meticulously cleaning off the oil from their bodies, a rescuer inexplicably returns them to the same oil-contaminated ocean. The flaw is glaring: the sea birds will inevitably be recontaminated. Unfortunately, typical psychological approaches to promoting racial justice—with their exclusive focus on individuals—frequently parallel this scenario. To illustrate, envision humans as sea birds, racist ideologies as oil, and the system as the ocean. While the psychological interventions under study in this work may ultimately reduce individuals’ racist attitudes and beliefs, they neglect the broader systemic context. Just as the sea birds will once again be covered with oil, these individuals are likely to reacquire racist ideologies in the racist systems. Nevertheless, most psychological approaches to racial justice allow individuals to return to an environment filled with racist ideologies and policies after “cleansing” their racist attitudes and beliefs, with the optimistic assumption that their interventions will yield lasting effects.
This is not to suggest that a psychological approach to racial justice intervention research is of little value. Indeed, interventions that ignore individual racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors also fall short of fully achieving racial justice due to the bidirectional relationship between individual attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and systemic racism. Systems, after all, are composed of people, and considering their psychology—both individual and collective—is critical for bringing about systemic change. Analogously, releasing a large number of oil-contaminated sea birds back into the ocean after a spill, even post-cleansing, could result in the reintegration of oil into the water, disrupting the local ecosystem once again. Thus, psychologists should actively participate in research that aims to develop multi-level interventions that are designed to address both individual-level racist attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors and system-level racist phenomena. Additionally, racist systems are proven to be extremely resistant to change because they are carefully developed and maintained by those in positions of power who benefit from them (Kraus and Callaghan 2014; Maxwell and Shields 2014; Wilson 2009). Consequently, combatting systemic racism—if it is even possible—is a formidable challenge that demands substantial investments of time and collective effort (Johnston 1996; Wood 2000). This underscores the significant potential for psychologists to take a central role in spearheading efforts to alleviate the current suffering of numerous individuals experiencing racial injustice globally, while scholars and stakeholders continue their work in changing racist laws, policies, and practices.
Whether psychologists are participating in or leading racial justice intervention research, they must carefully consider the context of systemic racism within which their individual-level interventions are designed, tested, and implemented. We propose that the TRF, which is directly informed by the well-established Clinical and Translational Science (CTS) framework, is a valuable tool for bringing the reciprocal relationship between individual-level factors and systemic racism to the forefront at every stage of intervention design, testing, implementation, and dissemination.
In recent years, a focus on the intricate relationship between individual- and system-level factors has intensified within the psychological study of racism (Melson-Silimon et al. 2023; Modood and Sealy 2022; Salter et al. 2018; Trawalter et al. 2020). At the same time, it must be acknowledged that racial justice intervention research did not start yesterday, and moreover, that psychology is not the only discipline with a longstanding commitment to studying racism. Despite these considerations, many well-intentioned psychologists often find themselves stalled at the T1 stage—or perhaps worse, content with remaining at T1. One factor contributing to this stagnancy is that psychologists typically receive insufficient training in conducting interdisciplinary research (Sewell 1989; Swisher 2022). In the context of racial justice intervention research within healthcare, an interdisciplinary research team might consist of a diverse array of professionals and backgrounds, including psychologists, epidemiologists, health economists, health services researchers, political scientists, clinicians, health educators/patient advocates, patients, and lobbyists. Each member brings distinct priorities and perspectives to the table. Successfully transforming the challenges arising from diverse perspectives within interdisciplinary collaborations into sources of innovation requires both training and hands-on practice.
Another contributing factor is the prevalent bias in the field of psychology that favors experimental designs as the “gold standard” (Abrahamse 2016; Greenhoot 2003; Hunziker and Blankenagel 2021; Kaptchuk 2001; McCall and Green 2004; McDermott 2011; Reis and Judd 2000; Smith 2012). With this bias, many psychologists tend to perceive laboratory experimental studies as more scientifically rigorous than field observational studies (Diener et al. 2022). This holds true for many studies in T1 and even T2, where the primary objective is to establish a causal relationship between a given intervention and a desired outcome in highly controlled settings. However, the criteria for rigorous scientific approaches and methods change as research advances through the stages in the TRF. The ability to accurately define scientific rigor and identify the most suitable methodological approaches within each stage is also cultivated through training and practice.
This special issue addresses both issues by presenting a diverse collection of empirical and review articles that strongly align with the TRF in their approach to racial justice intervention research. It aims to offer both theoretical guidance and methodological examples and to inspire further theorizing, research, interventions, and policy reform in the realm of racial justice intervention research.
This special issue will consist of four distinct sections, each dedicated to illuminating pervasive racial injustices in a particular social domain: Society in General, Criminal Justice, Workplace, and Healthcare. In each section, we curated papers that represent different stages of the TRF (Figure 1). A distinctive feature of this special issue lies in the authors’ explicit exploration of how their ongoing research contributes to subsequent stages of the TRF and ultimately to potential policy changes. Each paper also highlights the authors’ personal experiences in racial justice intervention research, offering valuable insights. Authors reflect on the imperative need for interdisciplinary collaboration, sharing the challenges encountered and innovative solutions devised in the pursuit of their work. This personal dimension adds depth to the scholarly discourse and underscores the multifaceted nature of addressing racial injustices. Finally, this special issue concludes with a commentary focusing on the promise and potential of racial justice intervention research, as well as the challenges facing the psychologists who engage in this work. By providing a comprehensive overview and synthesizing the unique contributions detailed in each section, this commentary aims to serve as a thought-provoking conclusion, fostering continued dialogue and action in the field of racial justice.
In sum, this special issue presents a diverse assortment of scholarly work illustrating how racial justice intervention research can be grounded in the TRF. As we argue in this introduction, this is precisely the kind of work that is best positioned to produce real change that improves the lives of minoritized people. In composing this special issue, we were careful to solicit and select examples of research that varied in terms of their domain of focus (e.g., education, criminal justice, healthcare, etc.), the methodology they employed (i.e., quantitative, qualitative, or mixed methods), and the stage of the TRF that they represent. Moreover, in each article, we have asked authors to reflect on their personal experiences of conducting this work, the challenges they faced, and their approaches to overcoming these challenges. We hope these reflections will be of particular use to scholars who are just beginning to familiarize themselves with racial justice intervention research.
Of course, this collection is not without its limitations. While we actively sought out research that sheds light on various forms of racism, such as anti-Indigenous, anti-Asian, and anti-Muslim racism, from different nations and cultures, the majority of submissions we received focus primarily on anti-Black racism in the US context. As such, with some exceptions, the collected articles can be characterized by this same limited scope. Similarly, we received no submissions demonstrating work at the T4 stage, and relatively few at even the T3 stage. This gap in representation underscores both the challenges and opportunities for psychologists. On the one hand, the gap reflects the inherent challenge of seeing a program of research through to the point of translation to communities due to multiple limitations within the current landscape of psychology as a field, as discussed earlier. On the other hand, psychologists possess significant potential to contribute to the promotion of racial justice by specifically targeting this translational gap. In light of these limitations, we hope that the framework outlined in this special issue and the lessons offered by its contributing authors will motivate researchers to fill the gap. In particular, we are especially eager for psychologists currently working at the T0 and T1 stages or on other expressions of racism (e.g., Asian American and Pacific Islander hate, Islamophobia, etc.) to take the leap and pursue their work in the TRF.
心理学领域对种族主义的科学研究有着悠久而坚实的历史,阿彻和阿彻(1970年)、范登伯格(1962年)、克拉克和克拉克(1939年)、兰格(1967年)、佩蒂格鲁等人(1958年)、佩蒂格鲁(1960年)和托马斯(1970年)的作品证明了这一点。世界各地的心理学家都在努力更好地理解种族刻板印象、偏见和歧视的起源(例如,Benner和Graham 2013; Federico和Sidanius 2002; Louis等人2013;Sears和Henry 2003)、潜在机制(例如,Cuddy等人2008;Dovidio等人2004;Duckitt等人2002;Wilson等人2000)和后果(例如,Brown等人2000;David等人2019;Neblett Jr 2019; Spencer等人2016)。这些调查贯穿了我们这个领域的整个生命周期,因为著名的研究项目一直专注于从婴儿期到当代状态与种族主义相关的现象。然而,尽管这些广泛的努力,种族主义和种族不公正今天仍然存在(Bourabain和Verhaeghe 2021; Williams 2021)。在种族和民族上被少数化的人(我们从这里开始使用“少数人”这个词)继续在日常生活中亲身体验种族主义(ABC News 2020; Institute for Social Research 2023; Konate 2023; Pew Research Center 2019)。证实这些个人陈述的是,记录在案的种族不公正现象渗透到各个社会领域,包括教育、刑事司法、社区安全和自治、就业和医疗保健,这些国家和地区包括澳大利亚(澳大利亚人权委员会2022年)、新西兰(Stats NZ Tatauranga Aotearoa 2024年)、南非(南非人权委员会2017年)、英国(种族关系研究所2024年)、加拿大(科特2022年)、澳大利亚(澳大利亚人权委员会2022年)、新西兰(新西兰国家人权委员会2024年)、新西兰(新西兰国家人权委员会2024年)、新西兰(新西兰国家人权委员会2024年)、加拿大(科特2022年)。美国(哈佛大学陈曾熙公共卫生学院,2022年)和欧洲(欧洲反对种族主义网络,2012年;统计研究部,2025年)。这些种族主义的例子本质上与种族化的仇外心理的平行案例交织在一起——例如,在2019冠状病毒病大流行期间和之后,反亚洲态度激增(Gover et al. 2020; He et al. 2021)。虽然这些例子主要关注与白人至上文化相关的种族主义,但我们承认,种族主义和种族主义意识形态的例子遍及全球(Agier 1995; Bora 2019; Busey和Coleman-King 2023; Modood和Sealy 2022; Sambaraju 2021)。面对心理调查,种族不公正不仅存活了下来,而且愈演愈烈。事实上,近年来,世界各地对白人至上主义的公开表达出现了历史性的复苏(Beirich 2013; Simi and Futrell 2020; Youngblood 2020)。至关重要的是,白人至上文化的这些升级与政治激进化的促进密切相关,这种激进化使种族和民族歧视的法律和政策合法化(Adamczyk et al. 2014; Bilewicz and Soral 2020; Rees et al. 2019)。例子包括不成比例地针对种族和少数民族美国人的选民压制法案(Wilder 2021);在意大利(Gualtieri 2024)、丹麦(Abend 2019)、匈牙利(Kiss 2016)、波兰(Waterbury 2020)和英国(Griffiths and Trebilcock 2023),敌对移民措施不公平地影响了来自非欧洲国家的移民和寻求庇护者;以及在30多个国家禁止针对穆斯林妇女的面部覆盖物(开放社会正义倡议2022)。为了应对全球公开种族主义的高涨,许多研究人员为白人至上文化、仇外心理、仇恨和非人性化的兴起提供了优雅的心理学解释,甚至将这些当代表达与纳粹德国等政权的历史先例进行了比较(Esses和Hamilton 2021; Markowitz和Slovic 2020; Pretus等人2023;Reyna等人2022)。这意味着,虽然心理学学科有能力对种族主义提供充分的科学见解,但这些知识并没有有效地转化为对少数民族生活的真正改善。这期特刊的诞生是为了直接回应长期而尖锐的担忧——对种族正义进展缓慢的长期不满,以及当前全球白人至上文化复苏所带来的迫切需求。正如我们稍后将在这篇介绍性论文中更广泛地讨论的那样,阻碍我们将对种族主义的心理学理解转化为有效促进种族正义的关键障碍在于心理学家未能突出个人和系统之间的互惠关系。作为心理学家,我们在进行理论、设计研究和测试干预时,往往忽略了个人种族主义态度、信仰和行为与系统性种族主义相关的总体因素之间的关键相互作用。 本期特刊的目的是提供理论指导和方法上的例子,说明心理学家如何为跨社会领域的种族正义的实际促进做出贡献。至关重要的是,我们强调了在转化研究框架(TRF)中开展此类工作的重要性。为了实现这一目标,我们编写了七篇实证论文和一篇综述论文,所有这些论文都旨在通过将他们的工作锚定在基金会中来促进各个领域(例如,刑事司法、工作场所、医疗保健)的种族正义。我们将研究在现实环境中促进种族正义的干预措施的研究称为“种族正义干预研究”。我们的首要目标是激励心理学家,无论他们是否从事干预研究或仅仅致力于促进种族正义,重新评估他们的理论取向和完善他们的研究设计,确保他们准确地捕捉种族主义的复杂性。通过这样做,我们希望使他们的种族正义干预研究能够充分发挥其对种族和民族少数群体和社区产生积极影响的潜力。在接下来的章节中,我们将首先探讨为什么仅仅关注个人的态度、信仰和行为在成功的种族正义干预研究中是不够的。接下来,我们将简要概述扶轮基金会,并讨论其在解决与只关注个人有关的限制方面的作用。最后,我们将以描述本期特刊的文章集并说明每篇文章如何与扶轮基金会配合来结束。种族主义是一个复杂的、多方面的现象,它反映了(a)个人层面的种族主义态度、信仰和行为与(b)相互加强的系统层面的种族主义现象之间的相互作用(Melson-Silimon et al. 2023; Payne et al. 2019; Roberts and Rizzo 2021; Salter et al. 2018; Trawalter et al. 2020)。换句话说,个人既存在于种族主义制度之中,又受到种族主义制度的影响,而这些制度也是由这些个人故意设计和维持的。例如,对少数群体的负面态度和刻板印象(即个人层面的现象)是由居住隔离、媒体对少数群体的负面描绘、选民权利被剥夺和财务差距(即系统层面的现象)等因素助长和维持的。心理学作为一门研究心理和行为的科学(Feist 2008; Skinner 1965),在促进种族正义的努力中,倾向于集中精力减少个人的种族主义态度、信仰或行为(Devine et al. 2012; Gaertner and Dovidio 2014; Galinsky and Moskowitz 2000; Rydell and McConnell 2006)。这种个人层面的关注本身既不好也不好,因为每个学科都有其独特的学术重点。也就是说,当心理学家只关注个人,而忽视了这些人所处的种族主义体系时,无论这些体系是与一般社会有关,还是与刑事司法或医疗保健等特定环境有关,这对实现种族正义构成了重大障碍。考虑一下海鸟由于泄漏而被涂上石油的类比。在一丝不苟地清理掉他们身上的油污后,一名救援人员莫名其妙地将他们送回了同样被石油污染的海洋。缺陷是显而易见的:海鸟将不可避免地再次受到污染。不幸的是,典型的促进种族正义的心理学方法——他们只关注个人——经常与这种情况相似。为了说明这一点,我们可以把人类想象成海鸟,把种族主义意识形态想象成石油,把整个系统想象成海洋。虽然本研究中研究的心理干预可能最终会减少个人的种族主义态度和信仰,但它们忽视了更广泛的系统背景。就像海鸟会再次被石油覆盖一样,这些人很可能在种族主义制度中重新获得种族主义意识形态。然而,大多数种族正义的心理学方法允许个人在“清洗”其种族主义态度和信仰后回到充满种族主义意识形态和政策的环境中,并乐观地假设他们的干预将产生持久的影响。这并不是说用心理学方法来研究种族正义干预没有什么价值。事实上,由于个人态度、信仰和行为与系统性种族主义之间的双向关系,忽视个人种族主义态度、信仰和行为的干预也不能完全实现种族正义。毕竟,系统是由人组成的,考虑他们的心理——无论是个人的还是集体的——对于实现系统变革至关重要。 这个特刊的一个显著特点在于作者明确地探讨他们正在进行的研究如何有助于基金会的后续阶段,并最终有助于潜在的政策变化。每篇论文还突出了作者在种族正义干预研究中的个人经历,提供了宝贵的见解。作者反思跨学科合作的迫切需要,分享在追求他们的工作中遇到的挑战和创新的解决方案。这种个人层面增加了学术论述的深度,并强调了解决种族不公正问题的多面性。最后,本期特刊以一篇关于种族正义干预研究的前景和潜力以及从事这项工作的心理学家面临的挑战的评论作为结束语。通过提供全面的概述和综合每一节详细介绍的独特贡献,本评论旨在作为一个发人深省的结论,促进在种族正义领域的持续对话和行动。总而言之,这期特刊呈现各式各样的学术作品,说明种族正义干预研究如何能以扶轮基金会为基础。正如我们在引言中所说的,这正是一种最适合产生真正改变、改善少数群体生活的工作。在撰写这期特刊时,我们仔细地征求和选择了研究的例子,这些研究的重点领域(例如,教育,刑事司法,医疗保健等),他们采用的方法(即,定量,定性或混合方法),以及他们所代表的基金会的阶段。此外,在每篇文章中,我们都要求作者反思他们开展这项工作的个人经历,他们面临的挑战,以及他们克服这些挑战的方法。我们希望这些反思对那些刚刚开始熟悉种族正义干预研究的学者有特别的用处。当然,这个系列并非没有其局限性。虽然我们积极寻求研究,揭示各种形式的种族主义,如反土著,反亚洲,反穆斯林种族主义,来自不同的国家和文化,我们收到的大多数提交主要集中在美国背景下的反黑人种族主义。因此,除了一些例外,所收集的文章可以具有同样有限的范围。同样,我们在T4阶段没有收到任何展示作品的提交,甚至在T3阶段也相对较少。这种表现上的差距凸显了心理学家面临的挑战和机遇。一方面,如前所述,由于当前心理学作为一个领域的诸多限制,这种差距反映了将一个研究项目翻译到社区的内在挑战。另一方面,心理学家通过专门针对这一翻译差距,在促进种族正义方面具有巨大的潜力。鉴于这些局限性,我们希望本期特刊概述的框架及其贡献作者提供的经验教训将激励研究人员填补这一空白。特别是,我们特别渴望目前在T0和T1阶段工作的心理学家或其他种族主义表达(例如,亚裔美国人和太平洋岛民的仇恨,伊斯兰恐惧症等)迈出这一步,在基金会继续他们的工作。
期刊介绍:
Published for The Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues (SPSSI), the Journal of Social Issues (JSI) brings behavioral and social science theory, empirical evidence, and practice to bear on human and social problems. Each issue of the journal focuses on a single topic - recent issues, for example, have addressed poverty, housing and health; privacy as a social and psychological concern; youth and violence; and the impact of social class on education.