Challenges and future directions in studying sequencing as a debiasing strategy in forensic psychological assessment: A commentary on Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (2025)

IF 1.9 2区 社会学 Q1 CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY
Verena Oberlader, Bruno Verschuere
{"title":"Challenges and future directions in studying sequencing as a debiasing strategy in forensic psychological assessment: A commentary on Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (2025)","authors":"Verena Oberlader,&nbsp;Bruno Verschuere","doi":"10.1111/lcrp.12314","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>In 1979, Kahneman and Tversky showed that human information processing is subject to bias. They identified a number of factors, such as the order in which information is presented, that systematically lead people to make decisions that are not rational. In 2020, an international group of researchers tested <i>N</i> = 4099 participants from 19 countries and 13 languages and found that 94% of Kahneman and Tversky's (<span>1979</span>) findings were replicated (Ruggeri et al., <span>2020</span>). This makes the evidence for human susceptibility to bias one of the most replicated findings in psychology, and one in urgent need of remedy.</p><p>Intuitively, the most effective way to avoid being biased by certain information is to not have that information. This technique is called masking (or blinding) and inspired the debiasing approach <i>Linear Sequencing Unmasking-Expanded</i> (LSU-E, Dror &amp; Kukucka, <span>2021</span>). The LSU-E approach is based on empirically verified principles and has been applied to several forensic sciences, including the detection of deep-fake images (Casu et al., <span>2024</span>) or the identification of victims (Dahal et al., <span>2022</span>). Against this background, LSU-E is a promising approach for reducing bias in forensic psychological assessments. In an initial study, we tested whether a core factor, that is the sequencing of case information, was effective in reducing bias in criminal risk assessment as an exemplary area of forensic psychological assessment (Oberlader &amp; Verschuere, <span>2024a</span>).</p><p>In their commentary on our article ‘Bias is persistent: Sequencing case information does not protect against contextual bias in criminal risk assessment’ (Oberlader &amp; Verschuere, <span>2024a</span>), Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (<span>2025</span>) critically assessed the validity of our study. The authors warned that our results and interpretations may give an overly pessimistic picture of the effectiveness of the debiasing approach LSU-E (Dror &amp; Kukucka, <span>2021</span>). Here, we first address what we consider to be their main criticisms of our study and then suggest future avenues for investigating the effectiveness of this promising debiasing method.</p><p>In a preregistered experimental study with 308 informed lay participants, we investigated whether the presentation of irrelevant case information biases criminal risk assessment based on an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. We supposed that participants' criminal risk assessment in a fictitious case would be closer to the result of an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool, and thus less biased, if they were given only relevant case information than if they were additionally given irrelevant case information. Case information was defined as relevant if it was necessary for the application of the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool, and as irrelevant if it was not.</p><p>Critically, we also tested whether this bias could be reduced by presenting irrelevant case information after, rather than before, the application of an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. We supposed that sequencing case information could prevent the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool itself from being biased by irrelevant information, thus reducing the influence of this information on the final judgment of the offender's risk of reoffending.</p><p>The participants' criminal risk assessments were indeed biased by the irrelevant case information. Unfortunately, sequencing did not reduce this bias. Participants were equally biased regardless of whether the irrelevant case information was presented after, rather than before, the use of the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (<span>2025</span>) voiced several concerns about our study, which we address in the following three sections.</p><p>‘Information hygiene’—controlling the sequence and nature of the processed information—can help to avoid bias (Oberlader et al., <span>2025</span>), and we highly welcome the LSU-E for providing a framework for dealing with potentially biasing information. To reduce bias, experts should only receive the information they need for a particular step in the assessment. All other information should be kept away from them for as long as possible. Although this idea seems simple at first glance, our initial study shows that it poses challenges for implementation in forensic psychological assessment. These challenges may inspire which elements of LSU-E may be helpful in debiasing forensic psychological assessment, and under what conditions the LSU-E approach may work in forensic psychological assessment.</p><p>First, it is important to consider that the LSU-E approach includes other elements in addition to the sequencing of case information (e.g. documenting changes in assessments as new case information is coming in), and future studies should evaluate these components as well as the integral application of LSU-E for forensic psychological assessment. Moreover, these studies could assess their presumed benefits other than minimizing bias, such as increased transparency (Dror &amp; Kukucka, <span>2021</span>). Second, future research should focus on capturing the psychological processes that take place during the assessment process to understand why the LSU-E approach does (not) work (e.g. how the relevance of case information is assessed; the extent to which experts trust different sources of information). Such processes could, for example, be investigated in think-aloud studies (Someren et al., <span>1994</span>). Third, to increase the ecological validity of study results, it would of course be important to consider a sample of experts rather than informed laypeople. Although there are many similarities between different areas of forensic psychological assessment, it is important to examine the effectiveness of the LSU-E approach in relation to other legal issues, such as credibility assessment, in order to test its generalisability or specific boundary conditions (Oberlader &amp; Verschuere, <span>2024b</span>).</p><p>Since we know that bias is not only inherent in human information processing but also very persistent, scientists should be equally persistent in finding ways to make forensic psychological assessment as objective and fair as possible. Numerous approaches have been discussed in the recent past (e.g. Neal et al., <span>2022</span>; Oberlader et al., <span>2025</span>; Vredeveldt et al., <span>2022</span>). Now we need to thoroughly test them.</p><p><b>Verena Oberlader:</b> Conceptualization; writing – original draft. <b>Bruno Verschuere:</b> Conceptualization; writing – review and editing.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>","PeriodicalId":18022,"journal":{"name":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","volume":"30 2","pages":"188-192"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9000,"publicationDate":"2025-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lcrp.12314","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lcrp.12314","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

In 1979, Kahneman and Tversky showed that human information processing is subject to bias. They identified a number of factors, such as the order in which information is presented, that systematically lead people to make decisions that are not rational. In 2020, an international group of researchers tested N = 4099 participants from 19 countries and 13 languages and found that 94% of Kahneman and Tversky's (1979) findings were replicated (Ruggeri et al., 2020). This makes the evidence for human susceptibility to bias one of the most replicated findings in psychology, and one in urgent need of remedy.

Intuitively, the most effective way to avoid being biased by certain information is to not have that information. This technique is called masking (or blinding) and inspired the debiasing approach Linear Sequencing Unmasking-Expanded (LSU-E, Dror & Kukucka, 2021). The LSU-E approach is based on empirically verified principles and has been applied to several forensic sciences, including the detection of deep-fake images (Casu et al., 2024) or the identification of victims (Dahal et al., 2022). Against this background, LSU-E is a promising approach for reducing bias in forensic psychological assessments. In an initial study, we tested whether a core factor, that is the sequencing of case information, was effective in reducing bias in criminal risk assessment as an exemplary area of forensic psychological assessment (Oberlader & Verschuere, 2024a).

In their commentary on our article ‘Bias is persistent: Sequencing case information does not protect against contextual bias in criminal risk assessment’ (Oberlader & Verschuere, 2024a), Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (2025) critically assessed the validity of our study. The authors warned that our results and interpretations may give an overly pessimistic picture of the effectiveness of the debiasing approach LSU-E (Dror & Kukucka, 2021). Here, we first address what we consider to be their main criticisms of our study and then suggest future avenues for investigating the effectiveness of this promising debiasing method.

In a preregistered experimental study with 308 informed lay participants, we investigated whether the presentation of irrelevant case information biases criminal risk assessment based on an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. We supposed that participants' criminal risk assessment in a fictitious case would be closer to the result of an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool, and thus less biased, if they were given only relevant case information than if they were additionally given irrelevant case information. Case information was defined as relevant if it was necessary for the application of the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool, and as irrelevant if it was not.

Critically, we also tested whether this bias could be reduced by presenting irrelevant case information after, rather than before, the application of an actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. We supposed that sequencing case information could prevent the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool itself from being biased by irrelevant information, thus reducing the influence of this information on the final judgment of the offender's risk of reoffending.

The participants' criminal risk assessments were indeed biased by the irrelevant case information. Unfortunately, sequencing did not reduce this bias. Participants were equally biased regardless of whether the irrelevant case information was presented after, rather than before, the use of the actuarial-empirical risk assessment tool. Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (2025) voiced several concerns about our study, which we address in the following three sections.

‘Information hygiene’—controlling the sequence and nature of the processed information—can help to avoid bias (Oberlader et al., 2025), and we highly welcome the LSU-E for providing a framework for dealing with potentially biasing information. To reduce bias, experts should only receive the information they need for a particular step in the assessment. All other information should be kept away from them for as long as possible. Although this idea seems simple at first glance, our initial study shows that it poses challenges for implementation in forensic psychological assessment. These challenges may inspire which elements of LSU-E may be helpful in debiasing forensic psychological assessment, and under what conditions the LSU-E approach may work in forensic psychological assessment.

First, it is important to consider that the LSU-E approach includes other elements in addition to the sequencing of case information (e.g. documenting changes in assessments as new case information is coming in), and future studies should evaluate these components as well as the integral application of LSU-E for forensic psychological assessment. Moreover, these studies could assess their presumed benefits other than minimizing bias, such as increased transparency (Dror & Kukucka, 2021). Second, future research should focus on capturing the psychological processes that take place during the assessment process to understand why the LSU-E approach does (not) work (e.g. how the relevance of case information is assessed; the extent to which experts trust different sources of information). Such processes could, for example, be investigated in think-aloud studies (Someren et al., 1994). Third, to increase the ecological validity of study results, it would of course be important to consider a sample of experts rather than informed laypeople. Although there are many similarities between different areas of forensic psychological assessment, it is important to examine the effectiveness of the LSU-E approach in relation to other legal issues, such as credibility assessment, in order to test its generalisability or specific boundary conditions (Oberlader & Verschuere, 2024b).

Since we know that bias is not only inherent in human information processing but also very persistent, scientists should be equally persistent in finding ways to make forensic psychological assessment as objective and fair as possible. Numerous approaches have been discussed in the recent past (e.g. Neal et al., 2022; Oberlader et al., 2025; Vredeveldt et al., 2022). Now we need to thoroughly test them.

Verena Oberlader: Conceptualization; writing – original draft. Bruno Verschuere: Conceptualization; writing – review and editing.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abstract Image

Abstract Image

Abstract Image

测序作为法医心理评估中去偏策略研究的挑战与未来方向:评Kukucka and Quigley-McBride (2025)
1979年,卡尼曼(Kahneman)和特沃斯基(Tversky)表明,人类的信息处理会受到偏见的影响。他们发现了一些因素,比如信息呈现的顺序,这些因素会系统性地导致人们做出不理性的决定。2020年,一个国际研究小组对来自19个国家和13种语言的N = 4099名参与者进行了测试,发现94%的Kahneman和Tversky(1979)的研究结果被重复(Ruggeri et al., 2020)。这使得人类易受偏见影响的证据成为心理学中被重复最多的发现之一,也是一个迫切需要补救的发现。直觉上,避免被某些信息偏见的最有效方法就是不知道这些信息。这种技术被称为掩蔽(或盲化),并启发了去偏方法线性测序去掩蔽扩展(LSU-E, Dror &amp;Kukucka, 2021)。LSU-E方法基于经验验证的原则,并已应用于多种法医科学,包括检测深度假图像(Casu等人,2024)或识别受害者(Dahal等人,2022)。在此背景下,LSU-E是减少法医心理评估偏差的一种有希望的方法。在最初的研究中,我们测试了一个核心因素,即案件信息的排序,是否有效地减少了刑事风险评估中的偏见,这是法医心理评估的一个示范领域(Oberlader &amp;Verschuere, 2024)。在他们对我们的文章“偏见是持久的:排序案件信息并不能防止犯罪风险评估中的背景偏见”的评论中(Oberlader &amp;Verschuere, 2024a), Kukucka和Quigley-McBride(2025)批判性地评估了我们研究的有效性。作者警告说,我们的结果和解释可能会对消除偏见方法的有效性给出过于悲观的看法。Kukucka, 2021)。在这里,我们首先解决了我们认为他们对我们研究的主要批评,然后提出了未来研究这种有前途的去偏方法有效性的途径。在一项有308名知情的非专业参与者参与的预登记实验研究中,我们调查了基于精算-经验风险评估工具的不相关案例信息的呈现是否会影响犯罪风险评估。我们假设,在一个虚构的案例中,参与者的犯罪风险评估将更接近于精算经验风险评估工具的结果,因此,如果他们只得到相关的案例信息,而不是额外得到无关的案例信息,那么他们的偏见就会更小。如果案例信息对于应用精算经验风险评估工具是必要的,则定义为相关的,如果不是,则定义为无关的。至关重要的是,我们还测试了是否可以通过在应用精算经验风险评估工具之后而不是之前提供不相关的案例信息来减少这种偏差。我们假设案例信息排序可以防止精算经验风险评估工具本身被不相关信息所偏误,从而降低这些信息对罪犯再犯风险最终判断的影响。参与者的犯罪风险评估确实受到不相关案件信息的影响。不幸的是,测序并没有减少这种偏倚。无论不相关的案例信息是在使用精算经验风险评估工具之后,还是在使用之前,参与者都同样有偏见。kuukucka和Quigley-McBride(2025)对我们的研究提出了几个担忧,我们将在以下三个部分中加以阐述。“信息卫生”——控制处理信息的顺序和性质——有助于避免偏见(Oberlader等人,2025),我们高度欢迎LSU-E提供处理潜在偏见信息的框架。为了减少偏见,专家应该只接收评估中某一特定步骤所需的信息。所有其他的信息都应该尽可能地远离他们。虽然这个想法乍一看似乎很简单,但我们的初步研究表明,它对法医心理评估的实施提出了挑战。这些挑战可能会启发LSU-E的哪些元素可能有助于消除法医心理评估的偏见,以及LSU-E方法在什么条件下可能在法医心理评估中起作用。首先,重要的是要考虑到LSU-E方法除了案件信息的排序之外还包括其他元素(例如,记录新案件信息出现时评估的变化),未来的研究应该评估这些组成部分以及LSU-E在法医心理评估中的整体应用。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
4.00
自引率
4.30%
发文量
31
期刊介绍: Legal and Criminological Psychology publishes original papers in all areas of psychology and law: - victimology - policing and crime detection - crime prevention - management of offenders - mental health and the law - public attitudes to law - role of the expert witness - impact of law on behaviour - interviewing and eyewitness testimony - jury decision making - deception The journal publishes papers which advance professional and scientific knowledge defined broadly as the application of psychology to law and interdisciplinary enquiry in legal and psychological fields.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信