Nature's headlamps: A unique light-focusing structure in Parasesarma de Man, 1895 mangrove crabs

IF 4.4 2区 环境科学与生态学 Q1 ECOLOGY
Ecology Pub Date : 2025-05-20 DOI:10.1002/ecy.70107
Peter A. Todd, Ian Zhi Wen Chan, Wendy Yanling Wang, Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang
{"title":"Nature's headlamps: A unique light-focusing structure in Parasesarma de Man, 1895 mangrove crabs","authors":"Peter A. Todd,&nbsp;Ian Zhi Wen Chan,&nbsp;Wendy Yanling Wang,&nbsp;Zuze Boh,&nbsp;Hui Shan Soh,&nbsp;Huiwen Huang","doi":"10.1002/ecy.70107","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Colors are used extensively in animal visual signaling, playing an important role in camouflage, aposematism, mate recognition and choice, and intrasexual competition (Laidre &amp; Johnstone, <span>2013</span>; Pérez-Rodríguez et al., <span>2017</span>). In many cases, animals should benefit from being able to augment the intensity of the visual signals they transmit (Osorio &amp; Vorobyev, <span>2005</span>), especially in low-light environments such as mangrove forest undergrowth and the deep sea. During multiple mangrove fieldtrips, we observed that the pigmented facial bands of the crab <i>Parasesarma eumolpe</i> de Man, 1895, were particularly eye-catching. This common Indo-Pacific crab bears two colored facial bands on the anterior surface of its body, each approximately 6 mm in width and 1.6 mm in height (Figure 1A). Previous field and manipulative studies have shown that the colors of these bands differ between sexes, with more blue reflected by bands of males compared to females (Huang et al., <span>2008</span>). Pigments are known to be derived from carotenoids in the crabs' diet, fading when the crabs are starved and brightening when they are fed, suggesting they provide a cue of physical condition (Todd et al., <span>2011</span>; Wang &amp; Todd, <span>2012</span>). Finally, the bands are used in both intersexual signaling and intrasexual competition, affecting sex recognition and resource holding potential (Todd et al., <span>2011</span>).</p><p>However, this past research did not explain the anatomical basis of the exceptional facial band brightness we saw in the field. Understanding the mechanism of this brightness, potentially micro- or macro-morphological features, could help in determining the function of the crabs' facial bands, for example, whether they are used for communication with conspecifics or otherwise, and thereby shed light on its evolution by pinpointing the trait that has presumably been selected for. Here, we demonstrate that the concave macrostructure of these bands acts in a manner analogous to the reflector of a headlamp, increasing their apparent brightness at the same range of angles that are optimum for signaling between two crabs. Scanning electron microscopy (SEM) indicated no iridescence or structural coloration (Figure 1B; Appendix S1: Figure S1). These facial bands are, to our knowledge, the first example of light-focusing, external, macro signaling structures in animals.</p><p>We first modeled the typical signaling behavior of <i>P. eumolpe</i> (Figure 1C) by recording interactions between crabs (30 male and 26 female) in situ at Mandai Kechil mangrove in Singapore. Crabs were observed from 5 to 10 m away using binoculars. A reference scale (usually a short ruler) was placed in the vicinity of the crabs so distances could be estimated. A pair of crabs was considered to be interacting when they both stopped traveling over the substrate, faced each other, and began to observe and/or respond to each other. We measured (1) the eye stalk length, <i>L</i>, of the crabs: 4.46 ± 0.58 mm (mean ± SD) in males and 3.96 ± 0.34 mm in females; (2) the average crab body angle during interactions, θ<sub>c</sub>: 30°; and (3) typical interaction distances, <i>D</i>: 50–500 mm. We then calculated for the range of interaction distances observed.</p><p>To test the hypothesis that <i>P. eumolpe</i> facial bands focus and reflect light at the same range of optimum angles, we used customized probe holders and an Ocean Insight Jaz reflectance spectrometer to illuminate the crabs' facial bands (10 males and 10 females) from incident light angles, θ<sub>i</sub>, from +0° to +60° (at 5° intervals). For each incident angle, we measured the intensity of the light reflecting off the facial bands toward the receiving probe (also at 5° intervals). In both males and females, the intensity of the light reflected by the bands generally peaks at +0° or +5° (Figure 2). This coincides with the optimum angles for reflected light predicted by our intraspecific interactions model (Figure 1D) and suggests that the structure of the facial bands focuses reflected light to enhance the bands' brightness for signaling between conspecifics.</p><p>To investigate the mechanism that produces this effect, we created a 3D computer model of how light reflects off the facial band's surface (Figure 1E) using the Lightwave 9.6 program. High-resolution texture images were mapped via frontal projection onto a 3D geometric mesh surface generated with a non-contact 3D scanner (Figure 1E). The resulting model was used to simulate 10 different virtual lighting conditions. In each scenario, illuminating light was provided from a single incident angle (from −60° to +60° inclusive at 15° intervals); the paths of the light rays reflecting off the facial band surface were modeled, and the appearance of the crab from the 0° angle was generated. This model showed that the facial bands directed most of the reflected light toward the 0° angle, regardless of incident light angle (Figure 1F). In addition, we determined that the facial band is not iridescent (Appendix S1: Figure S1) and that its color is unlikely to be caused by structural coloration as SEM revealed no distinctive microstructures (Appendix S1: Figure S2). However, it remains possible that these exist within or below the cuticle, and further exploration using techniques such as holographic microscopy, freeze/fracture SEM, or transmission electron microscopy should be able to confirm this. Together, these findings suggest strongly that the ability of the bands to focus the light they reflect at the optimum angle for conspecific signaling is due to the macrostructure of the facial bands—akin to the brightness of car headlamps being increased by the curved reflective surfaces behind the bulbs.</p><p>Given the ability of the facial bands to focus reflected light at the optimal angles for signaling, we investigated whether their brightness and their striking pigment-based hue (blue in males and green in females; Appendix S1: Figure S3A,B) (Wang &amp; Todd, <span>2012</span>) may influence the crabs' ability to communicate using their facial bands. To test the role of brightness alone, we presented <i>P. eumolpe</i> crabs with a choice between two images: one with a bright facial band and another with a dull facial band (with 62% the luminance of the bright band, the average luminance when the band is viewed from outside the +0° to +5° range). Both male and female test specimens were observed to preferentially approach image stimuli with brighter bands (Figure 3A) (males, 20 trials, one-sided <i>p</i> &lt; 0.01; females, 17 trials, one-sided <i>p</i> &lt; 0.01; there was no significant difference between the response of the two sexes, <i>p</i> = 0.26). Negative controls (crabs were placed into chambers without stimuli) confirmed that the crab specimens in our trials were actively making a choice (Figure 3B): when placed into a trial chamber without any stimuli, most specimens remained within the compartment where they were inserted and did not make any choice (<i>N</i> = 96, one-sided <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001). Our results suggest that greater facial band brightness, which the macrostructure has a positive effect on, enhances the crabs' ability to transmit visual signals.</p><p>We further hypothesized that the colors of the facial bands, independent of their brightness, may play a role in how the crabs communicate. To test this, both male and female <i>P. eumolpe</i> crabs were used as test specimens (i.e., “choosers”) in two choice experiments employing V-shaped chambers (Chan et al., <span>2020</span>). In the first experiment (Figure 3C), they chose between two images of androgyne crab bodies (created by interposing images of both sexes to eliminate sex-specific visual cues on the body) bearing either male- or female-specific facial band colors (see Huang et al., <span>2008</span>; Appendix S1: Figure S3). In the second experiment (Figure 3D), they selected between two live crabs of different sexes as a positive control. In both experiments, specimens preferentially approached stimuli of the same sex. Male crabs preferred images with male-colored facial bands and female crabs preferred images with female-colored facial bands (<i>N</i> = 67, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001). Similarly, male crabs preferred male live crabs and female crabs preferred female live crabs (<i>N</i> = 67, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001). These findings indicate a role of facial band color in signaling and raise the possibility that increased facial band brightness, beyond conveying information in and of itself, may also help to make <i>P. eumolpe</i> facial band colors more visible in order to enhance communication between individuals.</p><p>Diet-based carotenoids are used to produce the colors in <i>P. eumolpe</i> facial bands (Wang &amp; Todd, <span>2012</span>), and we show here that this color, which is different in male and female crabs (Huang et al., <span>2008</span>), is involved in conspecific signaling (also, see Todd et al., <span>2011</span>). Our data further demonstrate that differences in facial band brightness alone are sufficient to elicit a behavioral response. Our results, in combination with those from previous works in the field (e.g., Huang et al., <span>2008</span>; Todd et al., <span>2011</span>; Wang &amp; Todd, <span>2012</span>), suggest that the crabs are able to use both color and brightness components of visual signals as separate sources of information (e.g., to reinforce one another or for different purposes altogether), adding to the growing evidence for complex, multi-component signals in animal communication (Hebets et al., <span>2016</span>; Ratcliff &amp; Nydam, <span>2008</span>; Tibbetts et al., <span>2020</span>). However, many questions remain, such as exactly what information is being conveyed by each component (e.g., sex identification versus fitness or health level), what effect smaller changes in brightness may have (Endler, <span>1987</span>), and whether other visual properties may also play a role (Chan et al., <span>2019</span>). It remains unclear exactly what roles color and brightness play in communication within this species and whether their effects may interact with each other (e.g., see Lim et al., <span>2019</span>) or with information conveyed over different signaling modes (e.g., stridulation has been documented in <i>Parasesarma</i> crabs; Boon et al., <span>2009</span>). It is also interesting that the <i>P. eumolpe</i> crabs in our study were attracted to facial band colors representing the same sex, whereas crabs of a closely related species, <i>Parasesarma peninsulare</i>, were attracted to the opposite sex (Chan et al., <span>2020</span>). These results will inform future studies on the role or roles of the brightness and colors of the facial bands in signaling—for example, to attract conspecifics as mates, to repel them to defend territory, or to deter predators—and on how these roles may vary among different species in their natural environment.</p><p>Considering the potential benefits of using light-focusing, reflective macrostructures (such as the facial bands we describe here) to enhance animal visual signals, it is somewhat surprising that such structures are not more common in nature. It may be that, in most animals, it is more effective or efficient to amplify a transmitted signal by expanding the area it covers (e.g., the entire body of the red seven-spot ladybird <i>Coccinella septempunctata</i> forms its aposematic signal; Brakefield et al., <span>1994</span>) rather than by modifying the structure of the existing signal-bearing area. Alternatively, it could be more beneficial for animals to bear structures that improve signal detection rather than transmission. A good example is the <i>tapetum lucidum</i> (the layer of reflective tissue responsible for eyeshine in animals such as cats), which is functionally similar to the facial bands here (i.e., it also focuses the light it reflects), but it is instead used to enhance night vision in a wide range of animals (e.g., teleost fish, crocodilians, elasmobranchs, cetaceans, carnivores, ungulates, and some arachnids) (Ollivier et al., <span>2004</span>). It is conceivable, however, that the same structure could be used by an animal both as a visual signal and a sensory enhancement (e.g., the luminescent organ in the pupils of flashlight fishes; Howland et al., <span>1992</span>). Finally, it may be more efficient for signal transmitters to simply move closer to the intended recipient.</p><p>The nature of the mangrove habitat in which these <i>Parasesarma</i> crabs are found is likely to have played a role in the development of their facial bands. The dull brown colors of the mangrove substrate (which differ distinctly from the crabs' bright blue and green facial bands; Huang et al., <span>2008</span>) and dim lighting conditions in the undergrowth, coupled with the existence of safe hiding places from which to signal (e.g., root structures and leaf litter) and a marked predation risk when leaving these refuges (Wilson, <span>1989</span>), could have created the specific pressures required for the facial bands to evolve. Hence, there may be other examples of similar structures in species inhabiting other low-light environments (where they offer the greatest advantage in signaling) with high but heterogeneous predation pressures, such as the undergrowth of inland tropical rainforests and aquatic benthic zones near the limits of sunlight penetration, which are typically less well studied and where many animals potentially remain undiscovered.</p><p>In summary, we demonstrate how the macrostructure of <i>P. eumolpe</i> facial bands focuses the light they reflect toward the same range of angles that are optimal for transmitting visual signals to conspecifics. We further show that the colors and brightness of the bands are used to communicate between conspecifics, suggesting that the light-focusing effect we describe functions to enhance visual signals in <i>P. eumolpe</i>.</p><p><i>Conceptualization</i>: Peter A. Todd. <i>Methodology</i>: Peter A. Todd, Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, and Wendy Yanling Wang. <i>Investigation</i>: Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Peter A. Todd, and Wendy Yanling Wang. <i>Formal analysis</i>: Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Wendy Yanling Wang, and Ian Zhi Wen Chan. <i>Data curation</i>: Ian Zhi Wen Chan. <i>Writing—original draft</i>: Ian Zhi Wen Chan. <i>Writing—reviewing and editing</i>: Peter A. Todd and Ian Zhi Wen Chan. <i>Supervision</i>: Peter A. Todd. <i>Resources</i>: Peter A. Todd. <i>Funding acquisition</i>: Peter A. Todd. Authors are listed in order of decreasing contribution to this work.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>","PeriodicalId":11484,"journal":{"name":"Ecology","volume":"106 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4000,"publicationDate":"2025-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ecy.70107","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ecology","FirstCategoryId":"93","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecy.70107","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"ECOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Colors are used extensively in animal visual signaling, playing an important role in camouflage, aposematism, mate recognition and choice, and intrasexual competition (Laidre & Johnstone, 2013; Pérez-Rodríguez et al., 2017). In many cases, animals should benefit from being able to augment the intensity of the visual signals they transmit (Osorio & Vorobyev, 2005), especially in low-light environments such as mangrove forest undergrowth and the deep sea. During multiple mangrove fieldtrips, we observed that the pigmented facial bands of the crab Parasesarma eumolpe de Man, 1895, were particularly eye-catching. This common Indo-Pacific crab bears two colored facial bands on the anterior surface of its body, each approximately 6 mm in width and 1.6 mm in height (Figure 1A). Previous field and manipulative studies have shown that the colors of these bands differ between sexes, with more blue reflected by bands of males compared to females (Huang et al., 2008). Pigments are known to be derived from carotenoids in the crabs' diet, fading when the crabs are starved and brightening when they are fed, suggesting they provide a cue of physical condition (Todd et al., 2011; Wang & Todd, 2012). Finally, the bands are used in both intersexual signaling and intrasexual competition, affecting sex recognition and resource holding potential (Todd et al., 2011).

However, this past research did not explain the anatomical basis of the exceptional facial band brightness we saw in the field. Understanding the mechanism of this brightness, potentially micro- or macro-morphological features, could help in determining the function of the crabs' facial bands, for example, whether they are used for communication with conspecifics or otherwise, and thereby shed light on its evolution by pinpointing the trait that has presumably been selected for. Here, we demonstrate that the concave macrostructure of these bands acts in a manner analogous to the reflector of a headlamp, increasing their apparent brightness at the same range of angles that are optimum for signaling between two crabs. Scanning electron microscopy (SEM) indicated no iridescence or structural coloration (Figure 1B; Appendix S1: Figure S1). These facial bands are, to our knowledge, the first example of light-focusing, external, macro signaling structures in animals.

We first modeled the typical signaling behavior of P. eumolpe (Figure 1C) by recording interactions between crabs (30 male and 26 female) in situ at Mandai Kechil mangrove in Singapore. Crabs were observed from 5 to 10 m away using binoculars. A reference scale (usually a short ruler) was placed in the vicinity of the crabs so distances could be estimated. A pair of crabs was considered to be interacting when they both stopped traveling over the substrate, faced each other, and began to observe and/or respond to each other. We measured (1) the eye stalk length, L, of the crabs: 4.46 ± 0.58 mm (mean ± SD) in males and 3.96 ± 0.34 mm in females; (2) the average crab body angle during interactions, θc: 30°; and (3) typical interaction distances, D: 50–500 mm. We then calculated for the range of interaction distances observed.

To test the hypothesis that P. eumolpe facial bands focus and reflect light at the same range of optimum angles, we used customized probe holders and an Ocean Insight Jaz reflectance spectrometer to illuminate the crabs' facial bands (10 males and 10 females) from incident light angles, θi, from +0° to +60° (at 5° intervals). For each incident angle, we measured the intensity of the light reflecting off the facial bands toward the receiving probe (also at 5° intervals). In both males and females, the intensity of the light reflected by the bands generally peaks at +0° or +5° (Figure 2). This coincides with the optimum angles for reflected light predicted by our intraspecific interactions model (Figure 1D) and suggests that the structure of the facial bands focuses reflected light to enhance the bands' brightness for signaling between conspecifics.

To investigate the mechanism that produces this effect, we created a 3D computer model of how light reflects off the facial band's surface (Figure 1E) using the Lightwave 9.6 program. High-resolution texture images were mapped via frontal projection onto a 3D geometric mesh surface generated with a non-contact 3D scanner (Figure 1E). The resulting model was used to simulate 10 different virtual lighting conditions. In each scenario, illuminating light was provided from a single incident angle (from −60° to +60° inclusive at 15° intervals); the paths of the light rays reflecting off the facial band surface were modeled, and the appearance of the crab from the 0° angle was generated. This model showed that the facial bands directed most of the reflected light toward the 0° angle, regardless of incident light angle (Figure 1F). In addition, we determined that the facial band is not iridescent (Appendix S1: Figure S1) and that its color is unlikely to be caused by structural coloration as SEM revealed no distinctive microstructures (Appendix S1: Figure S2). However, it remains possible that these exist within or below the cuticle, and further exploration using techniques such as holographic microscopy, freeze/fracture SEM, or transmission electron microscopy should be able to confirm this. Together, these findings suggest strongly that the ability of the bands to focus the light they reflect at the optimum angle for conspecific signaling is due to the macrostructure of the facial bands—akin to the brightness of car headlamps being increased by the curved reflective surfaces behind the bulbs.

Given the ability of the facial bands to focus reflected light at the optimal angles for signaling, we investigated whether their brightness and their striking pigment-based hue (blue in males and green in females; Appendix S1: Figure S3A,B) (Wang & Todd, 2012) may influence the crabs' ability to communicate using their facial bands. To test the role of brightness alone, we presented P. eumolpe crabs with a choice between two images: one with a bright facial band and another with a dull facial band (with 62% the luminance of the bright band, the average luminance when the band is viewed from outside the +0° to +5° range). Both male and female test specimens were observed to preferentially approach image stimuli with brighter bands (Figure 3A) (males, 20 trials, one-sided p < 0.01; females, 17 trials, one-sided p < 0.01; there was no significant difference between the response of the two sexes, p = 0.26). Negative controls (crabs were placed into chambers without stimuli) confirmed that the crab specimens in our trials were actively making a choice (Figure 3B): when placed into a trial chamber without any stimuli, most specimens remained within the compartment where they were inserted and did not make any choice (N = 96, one-sided p < 0.001). Our results suggest that greater facial band brightness, which the macrostructure has a positive effect on, enhances the crabs' ability to transmit visual signals.

We further hypothesized that the colors of the facial bands, independent of their brightness, may play a role in how the crabs communicate. To test this, both male and female P. eumolpe crabs were used as test specimens (i.e., “choosers”) in two choice experiments employing V-shaped chambers (Chan et al., 2020). In the first experiment (Figure 3C), they chose between two images of androgyne crab bodies (created by interposing images of both sexes to eliminate sex-specific visual cues on the body) bearing either male- or female-specific facial band colors (see Huang et al., 2008; Appendix S1: Figure S3). In the second experiment (Figure 3D), they selected between two live crabs of different sexes as a positive control. In both experiments, specimens preferentially approached stimuli of the same sex. Male crabs preferred images with male-colored facial bands and female crabs preferred images with female-colored facial bands (N = 67, p < 0.001). Similarly, male crabs preferred male live crabs and female crabs preferred female live crabs (N = 67, p < 0.001). These findings indicate a role of facial band color in signaling and raise the possibility that increased facial band brightness, beyond conveying information in and of itself, may also help to make P. eumolpe facial band colors more visible in order to enhance communication between individuals.

Diet-based carotenoids are used to produce the colors in P. eumolpe facial bands (Wang & Todd, 2012), and we show here that this color, which is different in male and female crabs (Huang et al., 2008), is involved in conspecific signaling (also, see Todd et al., 2011). Our data further demonstrate that differences in facial band brightness alone are sufficient to elicit a behavioral response. Our results, in combination with those from previous works in the field (e.g., Huang et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2011; Wang & Todd, 2012), suggest that the crabs are able to use both color and brightness components of visual signals as separate sources of information (e.g., to reinforce one another or for different purposes altogether), adding to the growing evidence for complex, multi-component signals in animal communication (Hebets et al., 2016; Ratcliff & Nydam, 2008; Tibbetts et al., 2020). However, many questions remain, such as exactly what information is being conveyed by each component (e.g., sex identification versus fitness or health level), what effect smaller changes in brightness may have (Endler, 1987), and whether other visual properties may also play a role (Chan et al., 2019). It remains unclear exactly what roles color and brightness play in communication within this species and whether their effects may interact with each other (e.g., see Lim et al., 2019) or with information conveyed over different signaling modes (e.g., stridulation has been documented in Parasesarma crabs; Boon et al., 2009). It is also interesting that the P. eumolpe crabs in our study were attracted to facial band colors representing the same sex, whereas crabs of a closely related species, Parasesarma peninsulare, were attracted to the opposite sex (Chan et al., 2020). These results will inform future studies on the role or roles of the brightness and colors of the facial bands in signaling—for example, to attract conspecifics as mates, to repel them to defend territory, or to deter predators—and on how these roles may vary among different species in their natural environment.

Considering the potential benefits of using light-focusing, reflective macrostructures (such as the facial bands we describe here) to enhance animal visual signals, it is somewhat surprising that such structures are not more common in nature. It may be that, in most animals, it is more effective or efficient to amplify a transmitted signal by expanding the area it covers (e.g., the entire body of the red seven-spot ladybird Coccinella septempunctata forms its aposematic signal; Brakefield et al., 1994) rather than by modifying the structure of the existing signal-bearing area. Alternatively, it could be more beneficial for animals to bear structures that improve signal detection rather than transmission. A good example is the tapetum lucidum (the layer of reflective tissue responsible for eyeshine in animals such as cats), which is functionally similar to the facial bands here (i.e., it also focuses the light it reflects), but it is instead used to enhance night vision in a wide range of animals (e.g., teleost fish, crocodilians, elasmobranchs, cetaceans, carnivores, ungulates, and some arachnids) (Ollivier et al., 2004). It is conceivable, however, that the same structure could be used by an animal both as a visual signal and a sensory enhancement (e.g., the luminescent organ in the pupils of flashlight fishes; Howland et al., 1992). Finally, it may be more efficient for signal transmitters to simply move closer to the intended recipient.

The nature of the mangrove habitat in which these Parasesarma crabs are found is likely to have played a role in the development of their facial bands. The dull brown colors of the mangrove substrate (which differ distinctly from the crabs' bright blue and green facial bands; Huang et al., 2008) and dim lighting conditions in the undergrowth, coupled with the existence of safe hiding places from which to signal (e.g., root structures and leaf litter) and a marked predation risk when leaving these refuges (Wilson, 1989), could have created the specific pressures required for the facial bands to evolve. Hence, there may be other examples of similar structures in species inhabiting other low-light environments (where they offer the greatest advantage in signaling) with high but heterogeneous predation pressures, such as the undergrowth of inland tropical rainforests and aquatic benthic zones near the limits of sunlight penetration, which are typically less well studied and where many animals potentially remain undiscovered.

In summary, we demonstrate how the macrostructure of P. eumolpe facial bands focuses the light they reflect toward the same range of angles that are optimal for transmitting visual signals to conspecifics. We further show that the colors and brightness of the bands are used to communicate between conspecifics, suggesting that the light-focusing effect we describe functions to enhance visual signals in P. eumolpe.

Conceptualization: Peter A. Todd. Methodology: Peter A. Todd, Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, and Wendy Yanling Wang. Investigation: Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Peter A. Todd, and Wendy Yanling Wang. Formal analysis: Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Wendy Yanling Wang, and Ian Zhi Wen Chan. Data curation: Ian Zhi Wen Chan. Writing—original draft: Ian Zhi Wen Chan. Writing—reviewing and editing: Peter A. Todd and Ian Zhi Wen Chan. Supervision: Peter A. Todd. Resources: Peter A. Todd. Funding acquisition: Peter A. Todd. Authors are listed in order of decreasing contribution to this work.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

大自然的大灯:一个独特的光聚焦结构在Parasesarma de Man, 1895年红树林螃蟹
颜色广泛用于动物的视觉信号,在伪装、警告、配偶识别和选择以及性内竞争中起着重要作用(Laidre &amp;约翰斯通,2013;Pérez-Rodríguez等人,2017)。在许多情况下,动物应该受益于能够增强它们传递的视觉信号的强度(奥索里奥&amp;Vorobyev, 2005),特别是在低光环境中,如红树林灌木丛和深海。在多次红树林实地考察中,我们观察到1895年的paresarma eumolpe de Man蟹的色素面部带特别引人注目。这种常见的印度太平洋螃蟹在其身体的前表面有两条彩色的面部带,每条约6毫米宽,1.6毫米高(图1A)。先前的实地和操作研究表明,这些波段的颜色因性别而异,与女性相比,男性波段反射的蓝色更多(Huang et al., 2008)。已知色素来源于螃蟹饮食中的类胡萝卜素,当螃蟹饥饿时会褪色,当它们被喂食时会变亮,这表明它们提供了身体状况的线索(Todd et al., 2011;王,托德,2012)。最后,这些波段用于雌雄之间的信号传递和雌雄之间的竞争,影响性别识别和资源持有潜力(Todd et al., 2011)。然而,过去的研究并没有解释我们在现场看到的特殊面部带亮度的解剖学基础。了解这种亮度的机制,潜在的微观或宏观形态特征,可以帮助确定螃蟹面部带的功能,例如,它们是否用于与同种生物交流或其他,从而通过确定可能被选择的特征来阐明其进化。在这里,我们证明了这些波段的凹形宏观结构的作用方式类似于前照灯的反射器,在相同的角度范围内增加了它们的表观亮度,这是两个螃蟹之间信号的最佳角度。扫描电镜(SEM)显示无虹彩或结构色(图1B;附录S1:图S1)。据我们所知,这些面部带是动物体内光聚焦、外部宏观信号结构的第一个例子。我们首先通过记录新加坡Mandai Kechil红树林螃蟹(30只雄性和26只雌性)之间的相互作用,模拟了P. eumolpe的典型信号行为(图1C)。用双筒望远镜在5 ~ 10米外观察螃蟹。参考比例尺(通常是一把短尺子)放在螃蟹附近,这样就可以估计距离。当一对螃蟹停止在基质上移动,面对对方,并开始观察和/或回应对方时,就被认为是相互作用的。我们测量了:(1)蟹眼柄长度L:雄性4.46±0.58 mm(平均±SD),雌性3.96±0.34 mm;(2)相互作用时蟹体平均角,θc: 30°;(3)典型相互作用距离D: 50 ~ 500mm。然后我们计算了观测到的相互作用距离的范围。为了验证P. eumolpe面部带在相同最佳角度范围内聚焦和反射光的假设,我们使用定制的探针夹具和Ocean Insight Jaz反射光谱仪,从入射光角度θi,从+0°到+60°(间隔5°)照射螃蟹的面部带(10只雄性和10只雌性)。对于每个入射角,我们测量了从面部波段向接收探头反射的光的强度(也是以5°间隔)。在雄性和雌性中,波段反射的光强度通常在+0°或+5°处达到峰值(图2)。这与我们的种内相互作用模型预测的最佳反射光角度相吻合(图1D),并表明面部带的结构聚焦反射光以增强带的亮度,用于在同种生物之间发送信号。为了研究产生这种效果的机制,我们使用Lightwave 9.6程序创建了一个3D计算机模型,显示光线如何从面部带的表面反射(图1E)。通过正面投影将高分辨率纹理图像映射到非接触式3D扫描仪生成的三维几何网格表面上(图1E)。该模型用于模拟10种不同的虚拟照明条件。在每个场景中,照明光从单一入射角提供(从- 60°到+60°,包括15°间隔);对光线从面带表面反射的路径进行建模,生成0°角度螃蟹的外观。该模型显示,无论入射光的角度如何,面部波段都将大部分反射光导向0°角(图1F)。 此外,我们确定面部带不是彩虹色的(附录S1:图S1),其颜色不太可能是由结构着色引起的,因为扫描电镜显示没有明显的微结构(附录S1:图S2)。然而,它们仍然有可能存在于角质层内或之下,并且使用全息显微镜、冷冻/断裂扫描电镜或透射电子显微镜等技术进行进一步的探索应该能够证实这一点。综上所述,这些发现有力地表明,这些波段能够将它们反射的光聚焦在最佳角度上,从而发出相同的信号,这是由于面部波段的宏观结构——类似于汽车前照灯的亮度是由灯泡后面弯曲的反射表面增加的。鉴于面部带能够以最佳角度聚焦反射光以发出信号,我们研究了它们的亮度和引人注目的基于色素的色调(男性为蓝色,女性为绿色;附录S1:图S3A,B) (Wang &amp;Todd, 2012)可能会影响螃蟹使用面部带进行交流的能力。为了单独测试亮度的作用,我们向P. eumolpe螃蟹展示了两幅图像:一幅是明亮的面部带,另一幅是暗淡的面部带(亮度为明亮带的62%,从+0°到+5°范围外观察时的平均亮度)。观察到男性和女性受试者都优先接近较亮波段的图像刺激(图3A)(男性,20次试验,单侧p &lt; 0.01;女性,17个试验,单侧p &lt; 0.01;两性的反应无显著差异(p = 0.26)。阴性对照(螃蟹被放置在没有刺激的房间里)证实了我们试验中的螃蟹标本是主动做出选择的(图3B):当螃蟹被放置在没有任何刺激的房间里时,大多数标本都留在了它们被插入的房间里,没有做出任何选择(N = 96,单侧p &lt; 0.001)。我们的研究结果表明,更大的面部波段亮度,宏观结构有积极的影响,增强了螃蟹传递视觉信号的能力。我们进一步假设,面部带的颜色,独立于它们的亮度,可能在螃蟹的交流方式中发挥作用。为了验证这一点,在使用v形室的两次选择实验中,将雄性和雌性假包皮蟹作为测试样本(即“选择者”)(Chan et al., 2020)。在第一个实验中(图3C),他们在两张雌雄同体的螃蟹身体图像(通过插入两性图像来消除身体上的性别特异性视觉线索)中选择具有雄性或雌性特定面部带颜色的图像(见Huang et al., 2008;附录S1:图S3)。在第二个实验中(图3D),他们从两只不同性别的活蟹中选择一只作为阳性对照。在这两个实验中,实验对象都倾向于接近同性的刺激物。雄蟹偏爱带有雄性色彩的面部带,雌蟹偏爱带有雌性色彩的面部带(N = 67, p &lt; 0.001)。同样,雄蟹偏爱雄活蟹,雌蟹偏爱雌活蟹(N = 67, p &lt; 0.001)。这些发现表明了面部带颜色在信号传递中的作用,并提出了增加面部带亮度的可能性,除了传递信息本身,还可能有助于使假人面部带的颜色更明显,以加强个体之间的交流。以饮食为基础的类胡萝卜素被用来产生假人面部带的颜色(Wang &amp;Todd, 2012),我们在这里表明,这种颜色在雄性和雌性螃蟹中是不同的(Huang et al., 2008),参与了同种信号传导(也见Todd et al., 2011)。我们的数据进一步证明,仅面部波段亮度的差异就足以引发行为反应。我们的研究结果结合了该领域以前的研究成果(例如Huang et al., 2008;Todd et al., 2011;王,Todd, 2012),表明螃蟹能够将视觉信号的颜色和亮度成分作为单独的信息来源(例如,相互加强或完全用于不同的目的),增加了动物交流中复杂的多成分信号的证据(Hebets等人,2016;·拉特克利夫称,Nydam, 2008;Tibbetts et al., 2020)。然而,仍然存在许多问题,例如每个组件到底传达了什么信息(例如,性别识别与健康或健康水平),亮度的较小变化可能产生什么影响(Endler, 1987),以及其他视觉特性是否也可能发挥作用(Chan等人,2019)。目前尚不清楚颜色和亮度在这个物种的交流中究竟起什么作用,以及它们的影响是否会相互作用(例如,见Lim等人)。 (2019)或通过不同信号模式传递的信息(例如,在Parasesarma蟹中记录了鸣声;Boon et al., 2009)。同样有趣的是,在我们的研究中,P. eumolpe螃蟹被代表同性的面部带颜色所吸引,而一个密切相关的物种,Parasesarma半岛的螃蟹则被异性所吸引(Chan et al., 2020)。这些结果将为未来关于面部带的亮度和颜色在信号中的作用的研究提供信息——例如,吸引同种动物作为配偶,击退它们以保卫领土,或阻止捕食者——以及这些作用在不同物种的自然环境中如何变化。考虑到使用光聚焦、反射宏观结构(如我们在这里描述的面部带)来增强动物视觉信号的潜在好处,这种结构在自然界中并不常见,这有点令人惊讶。可能在大多数动物中,通过扩大传播信号覆盖的区域来放大传播信号是更有效的(例如,红色七斑瓢虫七星瓢虫的整个身体形成它的警告信号;Brakefield et al., 1994),而不是通过修改现有信号承载区域的结构。另一种可能是,对动物来说,携带能够改善信号检测而不是传输的结构可能更有益。一个很好的例子是反光绒膜(负责动物如猫的眼睛发光的反射组织层),其功能类似于这里的面部带(即,它也聚焦它反射的光),但它被用于增强各种动物的夜视能力(例如,硬骨鱼、鳄鱼、板鳃目、鲸目、食肉动物、有蹄类动物和一些蛛形纲动物)(Ollivier et al., 2004)。然而,可以想象的是,同样的结构可以被动物既用作视觉信号又用作感觉增强(例如,闪光鱼瞳孔中的发光器官;Howland et al., 1992)。最后,信号发射器简单地靠近预定的接收者可能会更有效。发现这些Parasesarma蟹的红树林栖息地的性质可能在它们面部带的发育中发挥了作用。红树林基底的暗棕色(与蟹的亮蓝色和绿色面部带明显不同);Huang et al., 2008)和灌木丛中暗淡的光照条件,再加上存在安全的藏身之处(例如,根结构和凋落叶),以及离开这些避难所时明显的捕食风险(Wilson, 1989),可能创造了面部带进化所需的特定压力。因此,可能还有其他类似结构的例子,存在于其他低光环境(在那里它们提供了最大的信号优势)中,这些环境具有高但异质性的捕食压力,例如内陆热带雨林的灌木丛和靠近阳光穿透极限的水生底栖区,这些区域通常研究得较少,许多动物可能仍未被发现。总之,我们展示了P. eumolpe面部带的宏观结构如何将它们反射的光聚焦在相同的角度范围内,这是向同种生物传输视觉信号的最佳角度。我们进一步发现,这些波段的颜色和亮度被用来在同种生物之间进行交流,这表明我们所描述的光聚焦效应在类人猿中具有增强视觉信号的功能。概念化:Peter A. Todd。方法:Peter A. Todd, Zuze Boh, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Wendy Yanling Wang。调查:bozuze, Hui Shan Soh, Huiwen Huang, Peter A. Todd, Wendy Yanling Wang。形式分析:bozuze, sohui Shan, Huang Huiwen, Wendy Yanling Wang, Ian Zhi Wen Chan。数据管理:Ian Zhi Wen Chan。原稿:Ian Zhi Wen Chan。撰稿、编辑:Peter A. Todd、Ian Zhi Wen Chan。监督:Peter A. Todd。资料来源:Peter A. Todd。资金获取:Peter A. Todd。作者按对这项工作贡献的递减顺序排列。作者声明无利益冲突。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Ecology
Ecology 环境科学-生态学
CiteScore
8.30
自引率
2.10%
发文量
332
审稿时长
3 months
期刊介绍: Ecology publishes articles that report on the basic elements of ecological research. Emphasis is placed on concise, clear articles documenting important ecological phenomena. The journal publishes a broad array of research that includes a rapidly expanding envelope of subject matter, techniques, approaches, and concepts: paleoecology through present-day phenomena; evolutionary, population, physiological, community, and ecosystem ecology, as well as biogeochemistry; inclusive of descriptive, comparative, experimental, mathematical, statistical, and interdisciplinary approaches.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信